Podchaser Logo
Home
Ep45 "Why did a man shoot himself after hearing the lottery numbers?" (Time Traveling: Part 3)

Ep45 "Why did a man shoot himself after hearing the lottery numbers?" (Time Traveling: Part 3)

Released Monday, 5th February 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Ep45 "Why did a man shoot himself after hearing the lottery numbers?" (Time Traveling: Part 3)

Ep45 "Why did a man shoot himself after hearing the lottery numbers?" (Time Traveling: Part 3)

Ep45 "Why did a man shoot himself after hearing the lottery numbers?" (Time Traveling: Part 3)

Ep45 "Why did a man shoot himself after hearing the lottery numbers?" (Time Traveling: Part 3)

Monday, 5th February 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:05

Why would a man without a lottery ticket

0:07

shoot himself after hearing

0:10

the winning numbers? Who

0:12

is the most disappointed person

0:14

at the Olympics? And what does this have

0:16

to do with Pan American airlines

0:19

or botox? And why should

0:21

you always put yourself in the shoes

0:23

of future people? Welcome

0:28

to enter cosmos with me David Eagleman.

0:30

I'm a neuroscientist and an author at

0:32

Stanford and in these episodes we

0:35

sail deeply into our three

0:37

pound universe to understand

0:39

why and how our lives look

0:42

the way they do. Today's

0:52

episode is about mental

0:54

time travel. Now we've done

0:56

two episodes in the past two weeks on time

0:59

traveling. I started with the concept

1:01

of memory, which is our way

1:04

of unhooking from the here and

1:06

now and putting ourselves into

1:08

past time points. And

1:11

in order to do this, you need this whole

1:13

network of brain areas that are involved

1:15

in situating you not in

1:17

the world that's right in front of you, but a

1:20

remembered world. This network

1:23

allows you to resimulate

1:25

what the spatial layout was, who

1:28

was there, what your emotions were,

1:30

sounds and smells. All of this

1:33

is run like a simulation,

1:35

and nobody else can see it. It

1:37

takes place entirely in

1:40

the privacy of your own

1:42

skull. Then last week

1:44

I talked about the other direction of

1:47

time travel. Imagining possible

1:49

futures. Simulation

1:52

of what could happen next is

1:54

one of the most important jobs

1:57

of brains. We plan out

1:59

what are going to say, what we're

2:01

going to do, how we're going to act

2:04

in a situation, what might happen

2:06

to us, and on and on. And as

2:08

a reminder about a couple important points

2:10

I made throughout those episodes. Point

2:13

one was that we spend most of our

2:15

time as humans disconnected

2:18

from the here and now and playing

2:20

these little movies in our heads were

2:23

reminiscing on the past or we're simulating

2:25

the future. And point two

2:27

is it turns out to actually be the

2:30

same core network of brain

2:32

areas that's involved both in

2:35

memory and in simulation of the future.

2:38

If a person gets damaged to a particular

2:40

part of their brain, like the hippocampus,

2:43

they can get amnesia, meaning that they

2:45

can't remember anything about their past, and

2:47

they also end up unable

2:50

to simulate possible futures. If

2:52

you ask them to imagine

2:54

standing in the shopping mall in an hour

2:57

from now, they just draw a

2:59

blank. They can't put a simulation

3:01

together. They're not seeing a little

3:04

movie in their heads. So memory

3:06

and future imagination use the same

3:09

brain mechanisms. They are both versions

3:12

of simulation. So

3:15

I used to think that who you are is

3:17

the sum total of your memories.

3:19

But it's even more interesting than that, because

3:22

I would argue now that who you are

3:24

includes the sum total

3:27

of your future simulations.

3:29

If you're a person who

3:31

envisions big goals for yourself, that

3:33

makes you a bit different from someone who has

3:36

pedestrian goals. Now,

3:38

from the outside, you don't

3:40

know what a person is simulating. On

3:43

the inside, you see someone

3:45

sitting at the restaurant booth next to you, sipping

3:48

on their coffee, and you don't

3:50

know if they're thinking deeply

3:52

about their path to a Nobel prize

3:55

or instead they're just thinking about wanting

3:57

a bag of chips at the local gas station.

4:00

In the same way that we don't act out

4:02

our dreams when we're asleep, we

4:04

can dream of the past

4:07

and future without execution

4:10

by the awake body. So

4:12

now we're all set up for two days

4:14

episode, and this one involves

4:17

a hypothesis that I've been working on for

4:19

years. And to get going, I'm

4:21

going to start with a small event that happened

4:24

in Liverpool, England in nineteen

4:26

ninety five when a man was

4:28

listening to the radio. So this

4:30

man had a wife and two children, and

4:32

he was listening to the lottery numbers

4:34

being read out. Now, he

4:37

did not have a lottery

4:39

ticket, but he listened

4:41

to the first number and he

4:43

was transfixed. Now the second number

4:46

was announced, and then the third, and the fourth,

4:48

and he was frozen. And

4:50

after the sixth number got

4:52

read out, he went and

4:55

got down his gun and he shot

4:57

himself. Six numbers that

5:00

translated to his suicide,

5:02

even though he didn't have a lottery

5:05

ticket. Where the numbers

5:07

some sort of code. What exactly had

5:09

happened will return to Tim's

5:11

story in a moment. First,

5:14

I want to tell you about facial

5:16

muscles. So just over one hundred

5:18

and sixty years ago, a French neurologist

5:21

named Guillome Duchen studied

5:24

lots of patients, and he's immortalized

5:26

in the names of diseases

5:29

like Duschen's muscular dystrophe and

5:31

several others. But somewhat less

5:33

known about him is that he was obsessed with

5:35

using electrodes to send zaps

5:38

of electricity into facial muscles

5:40

to characterize how

5:43

facial expressions got made. And

5:45

what he realized is that when a person

5:49

smiles, it's caused by the contraction

5:51

of just two very particular

5:54

muscles of the face. Around

5:56

the mouth. There's a muscle called

5:58

the zygomaticus major, which raises

6:01

the corners of the mouth and draws

6:03

it back, and the muscles around

6:05

the eye, called the orbicularis

6:08

oculide, raise the cheeks

6:10

and send out crows feet around

6:12

the eyes. And so when your mouth

6:15

and eyes smile at the same

6:17

time, this is called smising

6:19

or smiling with the eyes, and

6:21

this is described as the Duchen

6:24

smile. But why does this

6:26

have a special name Because

6:28

people eventually realized that this

6:31

only happens when someone is genuinely

6:33

happy. But there's another way that

6:35

people sometimes smile when they're

6:37

not actually happy, and this is known

6:40

as a non Duchen smile,

6:42

and it involves only the zygomaticus

6:45

major muscle around the mouth and nothing

6:48

goes on around the eyes. Now,

6:50

as it turns out, several groups have researched

6:53

smiles, which sounds like a really fun job,

6:55

and the conclusion is that the eye

6:58

muscles are only involved when someone

7:00

is actually happy. In

7:02

other words, the Duschen smile only

7:04

occurs when there is genuine

7:07

positive emotion. People

7:09

have long noticed the non Dushen

7:11

smile, and for a long time this was popularly

7:14

called the pan Am smile

7:17

after the airliner where the flight stewardesses

7:19

apparently gave this same non

7:22

smising smile to everyone all

7:24

the time because they weren't genuinely

7:26

happy. Also, bowtos

7:29

can paralyze the small muscles around

7:31

the eye, which means that people

7:33

with botox sometimes just can't

7:35

pull off a smiling with

7:38

the eyes Duschen smile, and

7:40

so a non dus Shen smile is

7:42

sometimes called a botox smile

7:44

as well. Okay,

7:47

I tell you all that because of an observation

7:50

that has been replicated multiple times, and

7:52

that involves the question who

7:55

is happiest when they receive an

7:57

Olympic medal. Now,

7:59

it seems like the answer is obvious. The gold

8:01

medalist is the most happy, the silver medallist

8:04

is the next happiest, and the bronze medallist

8:06

is the least happy. But

8:09

that's not what researchers

8:11

find. Instead, they find

8:13

that the gold and bronze

8:15

medalists are the most happy, and

8:17

this silver medalist is

8:20

the least happy. For example,

8:22

one study examined photographs from

8:25

eighty four medallists from the two thousand

8:27

and four Athens Olympics, and they

8:29

found that at the metal ceremony,

8:32

the gold and bronze medallists

8:34

tended to have Duchen smiles, while

8:37

the silver medallists tended to have

8:39

non Duchen smiles. In other words, the

8:42

silver medallists weren't really

8:44

smiling joyfully. And this was

8:46

actually an extension of a study that had been

8:48

done a decade earlier, when

8:50

three researchers gathered footage

8:52

from the metal ceremonies of the

8:55

nineteen ninety two Summer Olympics in Barcelona,

8:57

and they got a bunch of undergraduates to

9:00

rate the happiness of each medalist

9:02

from one to ten, where one was agony,

9:04

in ten was ecstasy. At the

9:06

ceremonies where they are given the medals,

9:09

the silver medallists scored an

9:11

average of four point three on this

9:13

happiness scale, while the bronze

9:16

medalists scored a five

9:18

point seven. The bronze medalists

9:20

were happier, and it was obvious to

9:22

everyone with the naked eye. Now

9:25

this is weird, right, How could the second

9:27

place winners be less happy

9:29

than the third place winners? So

9:32

let's unpack what's happening here. Why

9:35

would a man commit suicide after

9:37

hearing the lottery numbers? And why

9:39

would a silver medalist be more disappointed

9:42

than a bronze medallist. The

9:45

key is what is known as

9:47

counterfactual thinking. The

9:50

brain doesn't just run simulations

9:52

forwards and backwards, but it can

9:54

step back to past time

9:57

points and crank things forward

10:00

to see what could have

10:02

been now. In

10:04

other words, this is not a simulation

10:07

of the future, but instead

10:09

a simulation of the present

10:12

from a previous time point.

10:15

This is called counterfactual

10:17

thinking because the now

10:20

that gets simulated is not factual.

10:23

It's the brain's own construction, its own

10:25

internal creation. But we

10:27

can imagine present

10:29

moments that might have been.

10:33

Now. Why did the man in Liverpool

10:36

shoot himself after hearing

10:38

the lottery numbers? It

10:41

was because he had been

10:43

playing those exact numbers

10:46

every week for five weeks, that

10:48

sequence of six numbers, but

10:51

this week he hadn't bought

10:53

the ticket. As he listened

10:56

to the numbers being read out, he thought

10:58

those were my numbers. He

11:01

envisioned the ways that his

11:03

life could have changed. If he had just

11:05

bought that ticket, His present

11:08

would be different. He could immediately

11:10

plug into a pastime point and

11:12

crank the machinery forward to a

11:15

potential now, a now

11:18

that didn't happen, and the comparison

11:20

of that now to the present

11:23

was overwhelming to him. He thought about

11:25

his potential now, the one he had missed.

11:27

He pictured having an ability

11:30

to pay those bills, to finish off his mortgage,

11:33

to beat the financial struggles

11:35

that he was facing, and he was haunted

11:38

by this now that wasn't

11:40

real, that was counterfactual, but

11:43

that could have been. It

11:46

felt so distant from where he was

11:49

in reality, everything would

11:51

be different if he had just bought

11:54

that ticket. He was filled

11:56

with regret, a

11:58

regret so strong wrong that

12:01

he chose to end his life.

12:03

Now, I suggest we can understand regret

12:06

from a computational point of view,

12:09

in other words, an algorithm that the brain

12:11

runs. The key is that for

12:13

most situations, the emotion of

12:16

regret acts as

12:18

a learning signal. That means your

12:20

brain readjusts itself

12:23

based on that signal. So

12:25

if you get mad at someone and say

12:27

something mean and then that person stops

12:30

talking with you. You

12:32

might feel regret about

12:34

what you've done to that relationship, because

12:36

in the present moment you have lost

12:39

a friendship, and your brain runs

12:41

the simulation from that past time

12:43

point moving forward, in which

12:45

it constructs a scenario where

12:48

you didn't say or do that offensive thing,

12:50

and you're still friends. And

12:53

you compare your actual present,

12:55

which is uncomfortable and bad, to your

12:57

simulated present in which every

13:00

thing is warm and close, and

13:02

the difference generates a signal

13:05

of regret, and your brain uses

13:08

that signal to learn on to hopefully

13:10

improve its performance next

13:12

time. Here's another example.

13:15

Kids are always saying no to

13:17

things that their parents want them to do, and

13:19

parents, when they are thoughtful about

13:22

this, will let this kind

13:24

of learning signal do its own work.

13:26

So the parent says, hey, it's freezing

13:29

outside, put on your jacket, and the kid

13:31

says, I don't want to wear a jacket. I'm

13:33

not gonna do it. So after some push

13:36

and shove, the right thing for the parent

13:38

to do is say, fine, don't

13:40

do it. There will be natural consequences.

13:43

So the kid goes out and after

13:45

a while finds that he's really cold

13:47

and uncomfortable. So he now

13:49

has this learning signal that tells

13:52

him, had I taken the

13:54

jacket, my now

13:56

would be different, I would be warm. And

13:59

the regret, the regret that they feel, navigates

14:02

their future behavior. I

14:05

saw a quotation from the author Kurt

14:07

Vonnegut, who wrote, of all

14:10

the words of mice and men, the

14:12

saddest r it might

14:14

have been, But I slightly

14:16

disagree. I would rephrase it, Of

14:19

all the thoughts of mice and men, the

14:21

most important r it

14:24

might have been. Regret

14:26

is a way for us to build

14:29

different nows moving forward.

14:32

Now, note that in some circumstances,

14:35

you might take a past time point and

14:37

crank that forward, and that imagined

14:40

now, your counterfactual now is

14:43

actually worse than you're real

14:45

now, and in that case you experience

14:47

a different emotion relief.

14:51

This is what happens if you're considering

14:53

investing in some stock and then you don't

14:56

get around to it, and then you find out the

14:58

company went out of business and the lost

15:00

all their money on it. Your present

15:03

now is better than it would have been,

15:05

so you feel relief at

15:07

the path that you took versus what

15:10

you might have done, or

15:12

to return to the weather. Imagine

15:14

that you go for a day hike and

15:16

then it rains unexpectedly, and

15:18

you know you don't have an umbrella in your car,

15:21

but you search around in the trunk of your car

15:23

anyway, and then you find one and

15:25

you feel relief. And again,

15:27

this can be a learning signal because

15:30

it tells your brain, hey, this was a

15:32

fortunate accident, but you can make

15:34

this happen more purposefully from now

15:36

on. So let's

15:38

return to the Olympic medalists. Why

15:42

is there the happiness difference between

15:44

the silver and the bronze medalist Where the

15:46

silver medalist is less

15:48

happy. It's because the

15:51

silver medalist almost got gold

15:53

but missed. He or she is

15:55

running the simulation over and over of how

15:58

things could have gone, and

16:00

in those imagined scenarios,

16:03

they're the ones standing on the top platform

16:05

and getting wreathed with gold. The

16:08

bronze medalist, in contrast, is

16:10

just happy to be there. They know they're not good

16:12

enough for gold or silver, and they can

16:15

imagine plenty of scenarios where

16:17

they're not on the platform at all.

16:19

So the silver medallists now

16:23

is worse than his other imaginary

16:25

now's, while the bronze holders

16:27

now is better than most of his imagined

16:30

scenarios. Now, interestingly,

16:33

this general observation about

16:35

second place winners is not new. In

16:37

eighteen ninety two, the great

16:39

American psychologist William James

16:42

touched on this. He wrote, quote,

16:44

so we have the paradox of a man

16:47

shamed to death because he is only

16:50

the second pugilist or the second

16:52

oarsman in the world that he

16:54

is able to beat the whole population of the

16:56

globe minus one is nothing

16:59

end quote. So the second

17:01

place winner is so close to

17:03

a victory that could have been, and that

17:05

is a more painful place to be. So

17:24

this is mental time travel. But

17:26

it's not from now to the

17:29

past, which is memory, as I talked about two

17:31

episodes ago. And it's not from now

17:34

to the future, which is prediction, which

17:36

I talked about in the last episode. But

17:38

instead this is starting from past

17:40

time points and simulating

17:43

forward to what could have been

17:45

right now now.

17:48

I want to make a point that I'll return to at the end,

17:50

which is that as you simulate

17:52

yourself for all these different time

17:54

points, this allows you to

17:56

see different possibilities

17:58

about who you could have been. And

18:01

so in this sense, you're used to

18:03

thinking about different us, different

18:06

versions of yourself that might have existed.

18:09

So before we dive deeper into the science,

18:11

we're going to take a moment to dive into

18:13

the literature. I'm going to read

18:15

a story from my book of short stories.

18:17

Some this story is called subjunctive.

18:21

In the afterlife, you are judged not

18:24

against other people, but against yourself.

18:27

Specifically, you are judged against what

18:29

you could have been. So the

18:32

afterworld is much like the present world, but

18:34

it now includes all the us

18:37

that could have been. In an

18:39

elevator, you might find more successful

18:42

versions of yourself, perhaps the

18:44

you that chose to leave your hometown

18:46

three years earlier, or the

18:48

you that happened to board an airplane

18:51

next to a company president who then hired

18:53

you. As you meet these ewes,

18:55

you experience a pride of the sort

18:58

you feel for a successful cousin. Although

19:00

the accomplishments don't directly belong

19:03

to you, it somehow feels close. But

19:06

soon you fall victim to intimidation.

19:10

These us are not really you. They

19:12

are better than you. They

19:14

made smarter choices, worked

19:17

harder, invested the extra

19:19

effort into pushing on closed doors.

19:22

These doors eventually broke open for

19:24

them and allowed their lives to splash

19:27

out in colorful new directions. Such

19:30

success can't be explained away

19:32

by a better genetic hand. Instead,

19:35

they played your cards better

19:38

in their parallel lives. They made

19:41

better decisions, avoided moral

19:43

lapses, did not give up

19:45

on love so easily. They

19:47

worked harder than you did to correct their

19:50

mistakes, and apologized

19:52

more often. Eventually

19:54

you cannot stand hanging around these

19:57

better use. You discover you've

19:59

never felt more competitive with anyone

20:02

in your life. You try

20:04

to mingle with the lesser use, but it

20:06

doesn't assuage the sting. In

20:08

truth, you have little sympathy for

20:10

these less significant use and

20:12

more than a little haughtiness about their

20:15

indolence. If you had quit

20:17

watching TV and gotten off the couch, you wouldn't

20:19

be in this situation. You tell them when

20:21

you bother to interact with them at all, but

20:24

the better use are always in your face.

20:26

In the afterlife. In the bookstore,

20:28

you'll see one of them arm in arm

20:30

with the affectionate woman who you let slip

20:33

away. Another you is

20:35

browsing the shelves, running his fingers

20:37

over the book he actually finished writing,

20:40

And look at this one jogging past outside.

20:43

He's got a much better body than yours, thanks

20:45

to a consistency at the gym that

20:47

you never kept up. Eventually

20:50

you sink into a defensive posture,

20:53

seeking reasons why you wouldn't

20:55

want to be so well behaved and virtuous.

20:57

In any case, you grudgingly

21:00

be friends some of the lesser use and go drinking

21:02

with them. Even at the bar, you

21:04

see the better use buying rounds

21:07

for their friends, celebrating their

21:09

latest good choice, And

21:11

thus your punishment is cleverly

21:14

and automatically regulated in the

21:16

afterlife. The more you

21:19

fall short of your potential, the

21:21

more of these annoying selves

21:24

you are forced to deal with. So

21:27

that's the sense in which our constant time

21:29

travel generates lots of different

21:32

versions of who we could be, and

21:34

presumably there's no afterlife where

21:36

we meet them, but nonetheless

21:39

different use exist right

21:42

now, trapped in the boundaries

21:44

of your skull. Now,

21:46

I'll mention one interesting note about regret,

21:49

one that's been noticed by psychologists and economists,

21:52

and I'm going to tell you this just in case you decide

21:54

to open a restaurant. Don't

21:57

have too many choices on

21:59

the menu, because when there are too

22:01

many choices, customers feel

22:04

higher levels of regret

22:06

after the meal is over. How

22:09

do we understand this, Well, you can only

22:11

choose one thing for your main meal. If

22:15

there were lots of choices on the menu,

22:17

then your brain keeps running

22:19

simulations of Oh,

22:22

I would have gotten this, and this is what the experience

22:24

could have been like, or I could have gotten that,

22:27

or I could have gotten that. More

22:29

choices lead to more

22:32

regret afterward. So compare

22:35

going to the cheesecake factory,

22:37

which has a twelve page menu

22:40

spilling over with choices, versus

22:42

you go to in an Outburger whose

22:45

menu reads Hamburger cheeseburger

22:47

fries. So when you're

22:49

done with an in an Outburger meal,

22:52

you don't really have much of anything to compare

22:55

for what could have been. But

22:58

after a cheesecake factory meal,

23:01

your brain unconsciously churns

23:04

on all the choices that it didn't

23:06

take, and it might conclude,

23:08

correctly or incorrectly, that one

23:10

of those other choices would have been

23:13

better, and then you

23:15

feel slightly less happy

23:17

about the choice you made.

23:20

So here's where we are so far. Regret

23:23

is any emotion that a companies

23:25

negative outcomes to decisions

23:27

for which we've been responsible. But

23:30

it turns out, since we are creatures

23:32

who are so good at moving around in

23:34

time, mentally, we come

23:37

to operate and make decisions

23:39

based on anticipated

23:41

regret. That is, we

23:43

come to anticipate the emotional

23:45

consequences of decisions we're making

23:48

now. So imagine you're facing

23:50

two choices. Let's say which new

23:52

job to take, and one of them

23:54

is a really risky startup with big

23:57

dreams about where they'll go, and

23:59

the other is a well established

24:01

company that's a little boring but very

24:03

stable. So a lot of people will

24:05

gravitate toward the stable choice,

24:08

and you might think, fine, I get it, they're avoiding

24:11

risk. But there's a slightly richer way

24:13

to view this, which is that they're avoiding

24:16

anticipated regret.

24:19

If one of the choices is risky and the other

24:21

certain, and the startup goes

24:23

out of business, you'll feel

24:26

bad that you took such a big risk.

24:28

But if the stable company were to go out of business,

24:31

you wouldn't feel much regret because

24:33

that was an unforeseen

24:35

possibility. You had made the right

24:38

choice by placing your chips on something

24:40

that was unlikely to fail. So

24:42

the simulation of the future generally

24:45

drives people to choose the

24:47

safer choice to avoid the possibility

24:50

of feeling the really bad feelings

24:53

later. This is also suggested

24:55

to be why you will buy a brand

24:58

that you know, even if it it's more expensive,

25:01

over a brand that you don't know that's

25:03

less expensive, even though it's

25:05

a better deal to take the unknown brand,

25:08

For many people, it feels worth it to

25:10

spend the extra money because

25:13

they anticipate they will have more regret

25:15

if the unknown brand ends up being

25:17

a bad choice. And

25:19

this comes up in a thousand ways in our lives.

25:22

Anticipated regret is what gets

25:25

you to buy something that's on sale now

25:27

instead of waiting for maybe a better

25:30

sale later, because you're afraid

25:32

that the sale won't last. And then you'll

25:34

think, oh, man, I could have had

25:36

that for ten percent off, but I waited

25:39

and I missed my chance. It's

25:41

not that you're experiencing regret now, it's

25:43

that you're anticipating that you will

25:46

feel regret in the future, and so that

25:48

steers your behavior now.

25:51

And this is of course what high pressure

25:54

car salesmen try to do when they say

25:56

I'll give you this special discount, but it

25:58

only applies right now. The second

26:01

you walk off this car lot, the

26:03

offer goes away forever, so

26:06

you simulate how you would feel

26:08

if you lost this offer that's

26:10

being dangled in front of you right now.

26:13

The anticipation of that regret

26:16

spurs you into actions so you won't

26:18

have to feel bad later. So

26:21

your brain simulates futures and

26:23

it feels them, and often that

26:26

includes questions like how bad

26:28

will I feel if I lose this

26:30

gamble. You don't just minimize

26:33

risk, you minimize the regret that

26:35

you expect to feel. But

26:38

anticipated regret isn't just about

26:40

avoiding risk. Sometimes you can leverage

26:43

the issue of anticipation to

26:46

improve your decision making. So

26:48

when I was younger, if I was having

26:51

a hard time making a decision between

26:53

two choices, my mother

26:55

would tell me to toss a coin.

26:58

Heads meant that I would take the first choice, and

27:00

tails meant I would take the second choice. But

27:03

the coin toss wasn't the

27:05

thing. The key, she told me was to

27:07

toss the coin and see the result, and

27:10

then see how that result felt

27:12

in my gut. When you see

27:14

which choice gets indicated by the

27:16

coin, you might feel a

27:19

tiny bit of relief, well

27:21

I'm glad Atlanta that way, or you might

27:23

feel instead a

27:25

tinge of regret, and

27:28

the second that that regret bubbles

27:30

up, then you know that the other

27:32

choice is the better one for

27:35

you. And here's another

27:37

example. Let's return to the issue

27:39

of getting a kid to wear a jacket

27:41

in the snow. When the parent says,

27:44

look, it's natural consequences. That's

27:46

one way to teach the kid, But really what they're

27:48

hoping for is that the anticipated

27:51

regret will be enough the kid will

27:54

think about how things might

27:56

feel in the future when they are shivering miserably,

27:59

and that anticipated regret will

28:01

be sufficient to force their hand

28:04

to make the right choice now.

28:07

And before we move on, I'll just mention

28:09

an interesting tangent here, which is

28:11

that people with psychopathy. Psychopaths

28:15

generally have much lower

28:17

anxiety than the general population.

28:20

Why it's because one of the characteristics

28:23

of psychopathy is an inability

28:25

to simulate possible futures. So,

28:28

just as an example, if I hook up

28:31

electrodes to your tongue and then

28:33

I say, okay, I'm hooking the other end to this car

28:35

battery and you're going to get this terrible

28:37

shock on your tongue, you will

28:40

probably feel a lot of trepidation

28:42

and anxiety and maybe break a sweat.

28:45

But that's not what happens to somebody who

28:47

is a psychopath. They just don't care. They

28:50

don't get sweaty and anxious. And

28:52

it's not because they're tougher than you. It's simply

28:54

because their brains don't

28:56

simulate the future very well. Now,

28:59

I'm going to do a whole episode on psychopaths

29:01

in the near future, so if you're interested

29:03

in this and what else is going on in psychopathy,

29:06

please tune in for that one. But

29:08

for now, I'm going to get back to normal brains.

29:11

Given the fact that we simulate

29:13

futures and understand how

29:15

we might feel in those futures,

29:18

there's an interesting trick that we can use

29:20

to improve our own decision making,

29:23

and that is to put ourselves in the

29:25

shoes of future people

29:28

looking back at us. Why

29:31

because this gives us a very good way

29:33

to ethically steer our own behavior.

29:36

The world is full of temptations, and some

29:39

of them aren't that big deal, but some of them

29:41

are worth resisting. And

29:43

one way you can do this is to imagine

29:45

looking back on your choice from some

29:48

future point, when you see how

29:50

all of this played out.

29:52

A friend of mine refers to this as

29:54

the concept of book, bell and candle,

29:57

which means something very particular in the spy

29:59

world. But what he meant was, whatever

30:02

you're considering doing, imagine

30:04

how you would feel about this if it

30:06

were written down in a book that everyone

30:09

could read. Or your action

30:11

causes a bell to ring such that everyone's

30:14

attention turns to what you did, or

30:16

a candle lights up the

30:19

hidden thing you did. How would you

30:21

feel if everybody could see that

30:23

you've done it. The point is

30:25

to put yourself in the future and

30:28

imagine retrospectively that

30:30

you have been caught. How would that

30:32

make you feel? There

30:35

are many ways of placing yourself

30:37

in the future and looking back. A

30:39

colleague of mine as an epidemiologist named

30:42

Gary Slutkin, and many years ago

30:44

he started working with gangs in Chicago

30:47

to see if he could reduce the violence. Now,

30:49

there are many ways of thinking about intervening

30:53

to reduce violence, including tougher

30:55

laws or longer prison sentences, but

30:57

Gary started thinking about this differently.

31:01

He noticed, for example, that these gang

31:03

members don't act badly in front

31:05

of their grandmothers, but they do in front

31:07

of their peers, and so they're

31:09

clearly able to assess a situation

31:12

from different points of view. So

31:15

he had an amazing idea, which was to employ

31:18

former gang members who had previously been

31:20

incarcerated and who wanted

31:22

to reduce the violence in their own neighborhood.

31:25

And they came on board as what he called

31:27

the interrupters, and the

31:29

idea was for them to change

31:32

a gang member's behavior in the moment

31:35

by making them think about the

31:37

future. So he got the interrupters

31:40

to intervene when say,

31:42

a gang member felt he'd been wrong then

31:45

he wanted to go in exact revenge,

31:48

and the interrupter would say something like, hey,

31:51

think about this for a minute. When you go

31:53

to jail for shooting that guy who's

31:56

gonna be with your girl Gary.

31:59

And people think about this as reframing,

32:02

but in the context of this episode, I'm casting

32:05

this as anticipated

32:07

regret. And this kind of time

32:10

travel imagining yourself

32:12

in jail and someone else being with your

32:14

girlfriend was very effective

32:17

in steering people's behavior. It

32:19

forced people to time travel to

32:21

a what if moment that

32:24

otherwise they would not have visited.

32:43

Now Interestingly, I want to clarify

32:46

that our feelings of anticipated regret

32:49

are not always a perfect steering

32:51

mechanism. For us, because we

32:53

often assume that our near

32:56

future counterfactuals are going to be

32:58

more appealing than they actually

33:00

turn out to be in reality. In other

33:02

words, the grass always

33:04

seems greener on the other side

33:07

of the temporal fence. So

33:09

what does this have to do with civil

33:11

wars or why the Balkan nations

33:13

split off, or why the Arab Emirates

33:16

came together, or why England

33:18

split off from the European Union.

33:21

So let me answer that by

33:24

going back to one of the great classical

33:26

novels of Chinese literature. It's

33:28

a fourteenth century novel whose

33:31

title translates to Romance

33:33

of the Three Kingdoms, and this

33:35

novel spends eight hundred thousand

33:37

words dealing with the battles

33:40

and plots, both personal and military,

33:43

of different people and groups trying

33:45

to achieve dominance for almost a

33:47

century. It's like Game of

33:49

Thrones before Game of Thrones and without the dragons.

33:52

Anyhow, the key thing I want to note is

33:55

the opening lines, which

33:57

I've always found shockingly in

33:59

sight full. The opening lines

34:01

go like this, the Empire,

34:04

long divided, must unite,

34:07

long united, must

34:09

divide. Thus has it ever

34:11

been? In other words,

34:14

the prediction here is that all

34:17

unified countries are eventually

34:19

going to decide it's better to split up,

34:22

and all divided countries will eventually

34:24

decide it's better to link

34:26

arms into a union. This

34:29

is something that characterizes world

34:31

history. But why does it happen? Well,

34:33

there may be lots of reasons, including economic

34:36

factors and agricultural factors and

34:38

political expediency. But I'm

34:41

going to suggest there's a neural

34:44

factor too, and that has to do

34:46

with people running the counter factuals.

34:49

What would it be like if we

34:52

were together? Wouldn't that be terrific?

34:54

Or we're all tangled

34:56

up in each other's business, wouldn't things

34:59

be better if we were independent?

35:03

And my assertion is that we have a slight

35:05

bias for concluding that

35:07

it would be better whichever way

35:10

we haven't experienced. Why.

35:12

Well, it's because we're not perfect simulators,

35:15

and so we believe that the

35:17

low resolution simulation in our heads

35:20

is actually a good one, even though

35:22

it lacks all the blemishes

35:24

of reality. So we believe

35:27

that everyone will be happy and get

35:29

along. And wouldn't it be great if we were unified,

35:32

or wouldn't it be great if we were finally

35:34

separated and we compare

35:37

our sanitized simulation

35:40

against reality. This is the same

35:42

thing, of course, with relationships. When

35:44

we're young and single, we continually

35:46

fantasize about a partner that we're gonna

35:49

meet. The partner always says the right

35:51

thing, always makes us feel great, never

35:53

has something on his or her mind that

35:55

causes distractions so that they don't

35:58

actually listen to you, but realize

36:00

life is always more complex, for

36:02

worse and for better than our

36:04

imaginations can simulate.

36:08

So now on to the last act we've

36:10

been seeing so far. In all these episodes

36:12

that we are creatures who mentally

36:15

travel to different points in time. We

36:17

constantly simulate ourselves

36:19

in the past for memory, or simulate

36:22

ourselves in the future to steer decision

36:24

making, or we simulate

36:27

possible now's to

36:29

understand what we should have done

36:31

better. And this kind of

36:34

time traveling, if we do it intelligently,

36:37

can allow us to steer our

36:39

lives a bit better than we

36:42

otherwise might. For

36:44

example, as we get better at thinking

36:46

about and interacting with all these different

36:49

temporal versions of ourselves,

36:51

we can actively cultivate

36:53

our ability to simulate these well.

36:56

And this is what we get out of visualization,

37:00

out of actively putting ourselves

37:02

in a detailed future simulation. So

37:05

here's an example. I recently met a new friend

37:07

named Brian Burke who's an LA filmmaker,

37:10

and he'll ask people, hey, do you want to be a

37:12

film director? It's not that hard, and

37:15

the person might say, genuinely, I don't

37:17

think I could ever do that, and he says,

37:19

look, here's all you need to do to

37:21

become a film director. You

37:24

write a ten minute script and then you get your

37:26

cell phone camera and a couple friends to

37:28

shoot it, and then you do all the editing,

37:31

and he says it'll suck for

37:33

sure. So then you do it again.

37:36

You write a new script, and you shoot it again

37:38

on your cell phone, and you edit it again

37:40

and it'll probably still suck. And

37:42

then you do that a third time and you find

37:45

you're getting a little bit better at this, and then

37:47

he says at the end of that you'll be as good as

37:49

most of the directors in Los Angeles.

37:52

And this simple technique of walking somebody

37:55

in detail through a future, this really

37:57

moves and inspires people, especially

38:00

people for whom it had never even struck

38:02

them that they could possibly think of being

38:04

a film director in their internal

38:07

model. They have a job and they're

38:09

doing fine at that, and they've never

38:11

meaningfully considered film directing.

38:13

That's a totally foreign thing that other

38:15

people do. But all it takes is

38:17

someone doing them the favor of

38:20

walking them through some steps

38:22

in the imagination what it would take to

38:24

get there, and suddenly they

38:27

see that it's not impossible.

38:30

And this is why visualization of

38:32

possible futures is so meaningful.

38:35

It fleshes out what the path

38:38

can look like. When someone sees the

38:40

path clearly, then it doesn't

38:42

seem so hard to get started. And

38:45

visualization or imagination this can

38:47

also steer your behavior away

38:50

from certain things that you don't want to do again

38:52

by making them feel real. A

38:55

colleague of Mind named Jack Keene started

38:58

an app to get seniors to exercise,

39:00

and the idea is to use AI

39:03

to show the person a picture of themselves.

39:06

If they do and if they don't exercise,

39:09

they see their body in good

39:12

shape or not in good shape. And once

39:14

it's something they can picture, then

39:16

it is more real. And

39:18

there are lower resolution versions of this.

39:21

For example, in episode nine, I

39:23

outline some ways that we can get good

39:26

at navigating our future behavior,

39:28

and one example I mentioned is for people

39:30

who are trying to lose weight. You

39:33

have them find a picture of themselves

39:35

where they look more overweight than they would

39:38

like to and you get them to stick

39:40

that picture on the fridge

39:42

and that way, every time they go to the fridge

39:44

to graze, that picture reminds

39:47

them of what they want to accomplish,

39:50

and it does so by allowing them to see

39:52

right in front of them their future

39:55

if they don't modify their behavior, and

39:58

getting people to think about possible

40:00

futures, good and bad. This is what coaching

40:03

is about. Sports coaching or life coaching.

40:05

A coach's job is to

40:08

expand your model of what's possible

40:10

and to move you through the next steps

40:12

and get your aim straight on

40:15

who you could be. Generally,

40:18

being able to visualize something makes

40:20

it like a prediction that you can chase

40:22

after, or defend against, or prepare

40:25

for or whatever. It refines

40:27

the simulation and makes it

40:29

feel more real. So

40:33

wrapping up the past three episodes,

40:36

who you are is the sum

40:38

total of layered time

40:41

scales. When you walk down

40:43

the street, you look to other people

40:46

like you're just a person walking

40:48

down the street, but your

40:50

brain is colorful and

40:52

alive with reminiscence

40:55

of your past, simulations

40:57

of a variety of possible futures,

41:00

and all your regrets and reliefs

41:03

that result from simulations of

41:05

hypothetical nows, and

41:08

this rich layering of time,

41:11

this is what makes humans so nuanced

41:14

and complex and fascinating.

41:18

And as we come to learn what is

41:20

happening under the hood, and we get

41:22

better at taking advantage of

41:24

these mechanisms, that gives us

41:26

a small grip on a very powerful

41:28

tool to navigate ourselves

41:31

in the direction of who we

41:34

would like to be. Go

41:41

to Eagleman dot com slash podcast

41:43

for more information and to find further

41:45

reading. Send me an email at

41:48

podcasts at eagleman dot com

41:50

with questions or discussion and I'll

41:52

be making an episode soon in which I address

41:54

those. Until

41:57

next time, I'm David Eagleman, and this

42:00

is Inner Cosmos.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features