Podchaser Logo
Home
33. Unlocking Mental Health: The Therapeutic Potential of Psychedelics

33. Unlocking Mental Health: The Therapeutic Potential of Psychedelics

Released Monday, 19th June 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
33. Unlocking Mental Health: The Therapeutic Potential of Psychedelics

33. Unlocking Mental Health: The Therapeutic Potential of Psychedelics

33. Unlocking Mental Health: The Therapeutic Potential of Psychedelics

33. Unlocking Mental Health: The Therapeutic Potential of Psychedelics

Monday, 19th June 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:05

This podcast is supported by Americans for Medical

0:07

Progress and was founded and created through the Michael

0:09

D Hare Fellowship , awarded annually

0:12

to support projects that inform and educate the public

0:14

about the critical role of animal research

0:16

in furthering medical progress . The Fellowship

0:18

honors the late Dr Michael Hare , a renowned

0:21

board-certified laboratory animal veterinarian

0:23

who dedicated his career to scientific

0:25

and medical advancements and who was deeply committed

0:27

to animal welfare and advocacy . Hey

0:42

everyone , and welcome in to this month's

0:44

edition of Labrat Chat . Thanks

0:46

for joining us and , as always , please

0:48

take a second pause . If you can

0:50

go into Spotify , press

0:52

the star rate , review our

0:54

podcast . That really does help . You

0:57

know other people find our podcast . If

0:59

you can go into iTunes or Apple podcasts

1:01

and actually write a written review , that's even

1:03

better . Wherever you can write and review the

1:05

podcast on any platform that you're using , we

1:07

do appreciate it . It really does help . And

1:09

again , our email . If you ever have , you know , suggestions

1:12

, comments , just for us personally , danielle and I will

1:14

respond to them labratchat at gmailcom . So

1:17

thanks everyone again for

1:19

tuning in . Today we

1:21

have an exciting guest . His

1:23

name is Dr Alejandro Tirado-Pacheco

1:26

. He's from Oregon Health Sciences University

1:28

. He's a postdoc up there , a

1:31

lab that focuses on want to know

1:33

if the lab focuses on it . But what we're talking to

1:35

him today about is the

1:37

use of kind of psychedelics

1:39

and how that can benefit mental health

1:42

. Especially with the last month , may , being mental

1:44

health awareness month , we thought it'd be

1:46

a cool idea to actually talk

1:48

to someone that does research in that

1:50

field and how it can possibly benefit

1:52

us and how the use of animals help , you know

1:54

, accomplish that goal . So , if you will , dr

1:57

Tirado-Pacheco , just tell us a little bit . You

1:59

know about yourself . We got you interested

2:01

in science and research and then

2:03

in this kind of field , particularly

2:06

if you would just tell us a little bit about

2:08

that .

2:09

Yeah , well , first of all , thanks a lot for

2:11

having me on . It's really exciting to be able to talk

2:13

to the public about this research . I

2:15

don't get the chance to do that a lot , so I'm really

2:17

really glad to be here . So I guess , yeah

2:19

, my story is a bit of a bit

2:22

of a wandering path to get to where I am

2:24

today . I grew up in Europe and

2:26

I was really into science from a young age and

2:28

the system , the way it works in Britain

2:30

, which is where I went to university , is that you apply to university

2:33

when you're very young , like 16 , 17

2:35

, and you don't have a major . You just apply

2:37

for something . And so I applied for a physics

2:39

program and you go in and you

2:41

only do that . So I started this physics

2:43

program and two years in I

2:45

was basically ready to quit because

2:48

you know , as you can imagine , choosing

2:50

a career when you're 17 years old , the chances

2:52

that that works out are pretty slim . But

2:55

then I got introduced to these ideas

2:57

about graph theory and dynamical

2:59

systems , so all these like mathematical

3:01

methods that you could use to model things

3:03

in the world . The problem

3:05

I had with physics was it was too abstract . I couldn't

3:08

really relate what was going on in the math

3:10

to the real world . But when I

3:12

got introduced to these ideas and especially to the fact

3:14

that you can model something like the brain using

3:17

these mathematical tools , that was really fascinating

3:19

right , that you can use these tools to think

3:21

about cognition , behavior and even

3:24

emotion , and so that was kind of my entry point

3:26

into neuroscience . So I

3:28

did a computational neuroscience master's

3:31

at University College London

3:33

And it was actually a physics master's

3:35

, but I did a computational neuroscience thesis in

3:38

a lab over there , peter Latham's lab

3:40

And I decided I would go to grad

3:42

school for neuroscience . And so I ended up

3:44

going to Brandeis University , which is

3:46

in Massachusetts , for my PhD . And

3:49

that was another choice that was dictated by the difference

3:51

which might be interesting to your listeners , differences

3:53

between the European and the American system

3:55

. In the American system you come into grad

3:58

school and you have a couple of years of classes

4:00

and you get to sort of explore

4:03

different labs and then choose what you're going

4:05

to do . In a lot of places in Europe , the way

4:07

it works , you as you find a professor

4:09

and you sort of ask them

4:11

. You know I would like to work with you on this

4:13

topic , so you have to really have a good idea of what you

4:15

want to do . But because I came in from the

4:17

sides basically and you know I didn't have

4:19

any biology background , i applied

4:22

only to the US because I wanted that

4:24

opportunity to explore different topics

4:26

And I thought I was going to do computational neuroscience

4:28

but I ended up falling in love with

4:31

experiments , especially experiments

4:33

with freely behaving animals . I just love

4:35

the techniques and what you can learn from those . So

4:38

I joined Gina Torrigiano's

4:40

lab at Brandeis . Over there I studied

4:42

sleep and plasticity , so how brain

4:45

plasticity is affected by sleep . So

4:47

, as you can see , kind of none of this has anything

4:49

to do with mental health or psychedelics

4:51

, but the way I thought about it , i always wanted

4:53

to work with psychiatric disorders And the way I thought

4:55

about it was my PhD was an opportunity to

4:57

learn neuroscience and

4:59

learn techniques and then apply those

5:01

later on in the next step , and

5:04

so that's what I'm doing now . Like you said in

5:06

your introduction , i am a postdoc in Bita

5:08

Mogadam's lab in the Behavioral Neuroscience

5:10

Department , ohsu . What we're

5:13

studying is broadly . The lab

5:15

is interested in the neurobiology of

5:17

psychiatric illnesses . Bita

5:19

has , you know , 30 years of experience working

5:22

in this field and she's done a lot of work

5:24

with schizophrenia , and when

5:26

I contacted her about working in her

5:28

lab , she offered me this project

5:30

that she was interested in to pursue research

5:32

on psychedelics and especially psilocybin

5:34

, because of the really promising

5:36

data in humans , and we can talk about that later

5:38

. But yeah , so now that's what I'm studying , looking

5:41

at psilocybin and major depressive

5:43

disorder .

5:44

Yeah , it's crazy that you apply to a university when you're 16

5:46

, 17 , and you have to pick what you want to do . I

5:48

mean , it's kind of similar , i guess , in a way , in

5:50

the American system You go

5:52

to college when you're 18 . And if you have a scholarship

5:55

I think a lot of scholarships , state-sponsored scholarships

5:57

require you to pick a major by

5:59

the end of your first semester or first

6:02

year or something in order to keep your scholarship . And

6:04

so even then , like 18 , 19 , you

6:06

don't know what you wanted . It took me , i mean , look , i went back

6:08

to school at 32 . So

6:10

I didn't know . It took me a while to like actually

6:12

get into a career or some

6:14

sort of job that I was interested

6:16

in and then to explore that further , so

6:19

then break it down and figure out what I really wanted to

6:21

do within that field . And so it's kind

6:23

of crazy that at 16 , 17 , that

6:25

you're forced to try to make that decision .

6:28

Yeah , it's pretty wild . And the thing is

6:30

there's no major or minor , so you just

6:32

go in , and so I took no college

6:34

level classes in biology , chemistry

6:36

, anything else . That wasn't even science

6:39

, like just physics . So I really think

6:41

it's limiting .

6:42

I don't know . that sounds kind of nice , though I just remember in my

6:44

undergrad having to take like sociology

6:47

and Greek mythology and you're just like I

6:49

don't want to learn this . This is so far better

6:51

.

6:51

Greek mythology is pretty cool . I like Greek mythology

6:53

. I took it because I thought it would be cool . It's

6:55

not cool , it's not when you have to write a paper , yeah

6:57

right , when you have to write a five-page paper .

6:58

I immediately regretted that decision . But

7:00

I have a quick question just about , like

7:03

, what psychedelics are , because when I think

7:05

about that I think of mushrooms . So I'm sure , well

7:07

I don't know . Are there other means

7:09

of where the compounds come from ? I guess , what

7:11

exactly is psilocybin , some of the

7:13

other psychedelic compounds that you work with ? where do

7:15

they come from ? How do you even obtain

7:17

them ? Well , i imagine they're like controlled

7:20

substances , but maybe you could just talk a little bit about

7:22

the compounds themselves .

7:24

Sure , i guess I'll start with what they are

7:26

. There's three main classes of

7:28

psychedelics and they're just like , biochemically

7:31

slightly different , but I'm not a biochemist

7:33

myself , so those things don't really mean much to me

7:35

. But essentially all of them are very closely

7:37

related to serotonin and

7:40

they act on serotonin receptors

7:42

in the brain . And then they have slightly different pharmacological

7:45

profiles . You know , some are natural

7:47

compounds , like psilocybin , like

7:49

you mentioned , is found in mushrooms . Another very

7:51

popular one is DMT , or dimethyl

7:54

tryptamine , which is found in

7:56

some species of toads and in some

7:58

plants as well . And then a lot of them

8:00

are just synthetic compounds , for example

8:02

LSD , the most famous one of them

8:04

all . So the interesting thing is a

8:06

lot of them are controlled substances

8:09

and they are like at the highest level , So

8:11

they're schedule one substances . So psilocybin

8:13

is schedule one , lsd is schedule one , and

8:16

so , yeah , it takes a while to get them

8:18

. You have to have a license with a DEA

8:20

Luckily I don't , but my boss does And

8:23

it takes a long time to get that and then

8:25

it takes a long time to get the compound . The

8:27

National Institutes of Health has a drug supply

8:29

program through the National Institute

8:31

on Drug Abuse . So basically the

8:34

idea is we want to study these

8:36

quote-unquote drugs of abuse to

8:38

understand them better . They can give you small

8:40

amounts of these drugs when you request them . There's

8:42

a lot of paperwork involved and you have to obviously justify

8:44

why you want it , what you're

8:47

going to do with it and how this is going to benefit

8:49

the public . There are other compounds

8:51

that are actually not scheduled , even though they're

8:53

extremely similar . So one of them is DOI

8:56

, and if you look up psychedelic research , you'll

8:58

see this DOI pop up all the

9:00

time , and that's because you can just buy it , and so

9:02

I don't know what it feels like to take it . I don't

9:04

know how similar it is to LSD or psilocybin

9:07

, but it's freely available and it's kind

9:09

of used as a proxy for psychedelic

9:11

effects . I personally think it's not

9:13

exactly the same , but we can get into that if

9:15

we want to later on .

9:17

Is that also something that comes from mushrooms

9:19

? That's just like a synthetic compound someone's made

9:21

.

9:21

That's synthetic . And actually I should mention

9:23

the psilocybin that we get and that everyone

9:26

gets is at this point synthetic . So it's

9:28

made in a lab , because when

9:30

you extract it from mushrooms you get a lot

9:33

of impurities .

9:34

Okay , So it's a process of extraction

9:36

. You can't go out and just find the mushroom

9:38

and eat it and get the psilocybin effects

9:41

. It's kind of like , I don't know , I guess like cocaine

9:43

. If you will , You got like there's a process to make it into

9:45

cocaine And I guess there's a process to make

9:47

psilocybin . I'm not trying to give information on how to

9:50

make it , or process it .

9:51

You're teaching people how to do that .

9:52

Well , my biochemistry book in school detailed

9:55

the process of how to make crack

9:57

detailed . Oh my gosh that's hilarious . It's

9:59

like the protocol on how to do it Probably

10:01

no longer in publication And I didn't try it

10:03

just for the record .

10:04

Either way , people should not go out and just eat mushrooms

10:07

, because you have no idea .

10:08

Yeah . So I guess if they're scheduled one drugs

10:10

, if it's something that you could just go out and

10:12

get in nature , or if it takes a pretty

10:14

like advanced process to actually get

10:16

the compound out of it , You know

10:18

, for lab research we want to make sure it's a

10:20

pure compound , so we use the synthetic version

10:23

.

10:23

But yeah , it's naturally available . And I should mention you

10:25

know it's been used for millennia by people

10:27

all over the world , especially in

10:29

what is currently Mexico . So in

10:31

Mesoamerica people have been using

10:34

psychedelic mushrooms for religious

10:36

and other important cultural ceremonies for

10:38

a long , long time .

10:40

Good to know . It seems like

10:42

there's definitely a lot of positive use

10:44

for it . I think it seems like when I was a kid

10:46

I remember talks about mushrooms

10:48

and how people would hallucinate

10:51

. I just remember conversations about if you

10:53

took them you're going to hallucinate and go jump off a building

10:55

and all the side effects and how awful they are

10:57

and how they need to be banned from society

10:59

. But now it's nice that there's kind of maybe

11:01

the little bit of shift coming on in society

11:04

and finding a positive use

11:06

for it . And I think in order to get to that understanding

11:09

, research needs to be done , and I think that's kind of where

11:11

you guys come in . I know there's lots

11:13

of human research in there out there as well

11:15

, but in order to really get a good understanding

11:17

, i think it's important that we do some of the

11:19

animal research . So if you would

11:21

, can you just tell us a little bit about your research

11:24

? Which animal model do you guys use

11:26

once you get that synthetic psilocybin

11:28

compound In order to study

11:30

it ? you've had a lot of findings , but what's

11:33

like a broad spectrum approach

11:35

to some of the findings that you've been

11:37

able to discover using that animal model ?

11:40

Yeah . So we use rats in the lab

11:42

and there's a very specific reason

11:44

, and the reason is that we are interested in

11:46

the behavioral effects of psilocybin

11:49

. So I'm going to back up a little and tell

11:51

you a bit about the human data , because that will kind of bring it

11:53

into context why we're doing what we're

11:55

doing . There's a lot of phase one clinical

11:57

trials and even a few phase two clinical

11:59

trials that show incredibly promising

12:02

results for psilocybin . And to kind

12:04

of tell you what that means , incredibly promising

12:06

. I'm talking about two doses

12:08

of psilocybin three weeks apart having

12:10

a stronger effect in ameliorating

12:13

symptoms of depression than a

12:16

six-week course of a standard medication

12:18

, which would be a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor

12:20

, you know , like prozac or lexapro . That's crazy

12:23

, it's very striking , it's

12:25

fast-acting and it's long-lasting . So

12:27

people have looked up to six months after

12:29

one or two doses and you still see

12:31

an effect . Yeah , that's incredible

12:34

. And it's really important to sort of

12:36

underscore the fact that , while

12:38

accessorizer are amazing and I can

12:40

probably say that they've saved countless

12:42

lives They have a lot of side effects and

12:44

adverse effects and you know people

12:46

end up having to take this medication every day

12:48

. So it's you know you get sort of problems with people

12:50

taking it . We've been on the hunt for

12:53

a better treatment for depression

12:55

for six decades or so , so

12:57

it's really cool that we now have a few options like

12:59

ketamine is one , and now psychedelics are emerging

13:01

as a really promising option .

13:03

Ketamine- is one I didn't know that Yeah , we

13:05

use ketamine all the time . I don't know it was used in

13:07

the mental health realms . I thought

13:10

it was more just in the realm of like just an addictive

13:12

kind of like cocaine , heroin

13:14

type of drug . But I guess it has use as

13:16

well . I know that's not the focus of your research

13:18

, but that's just not that interesting .

13:20

Yeah yeah , ketamine is interesting because

13:22

the different doses do very different things . So

13:24

at high doses it's an anesthetic I know

13:26

it's used in animals for anesthesia

13:28

but at low doses it has

13:31

this weird antidepressant effect

13:33

, and so it's actually pretty close to FDA

13:35

approval , i think Awesome .

13:37

Okay , that's cool . Yeah , i mean , we use it quite a

13:39

bit and cats that want to try to kill us while

13:41

at work , when they're hissing at you , you can score

13:43

a little bit in the mouth . It calms them down

13:46

a little and they do a little anesthetic properties

13:48

, like you said . But yeah , that's cool , good to

13:50

know as well .

13:51

Yeah , so going back to I guess the reason

13:53

I talked about all that is because psychedelics

13:56

have an interesting history . You know they were

13:58

used in the 50s and 60s in research

14:00

. Then they you know research stopped because

14:02

the control substances act made them

14:04

schedule one substances . And now we have all

14:07

this human data , but we have a real gap of

14:09

knowledge in terms of the biological mechanism

14:11

, and that's where the animal research comes

14:13

in . We want to understand why are

14:16

they so effective ? right , what is happening

14:18

in the brain when you take psilocybin

14:20

, and you know later on you feel better . And

14:23

so to do that , we have to be able to

14:25

see an effect . Is the animal actually

14:27

feeling psilocybin and perhaps does

14:29

it have an antidepressant like effect ? and

14:31

then what is happening in the brain ? So we have to be able to

14:33

use techniques that

14:36

you wouldn't be able to use in humans , that are

14:38

invasive , that you can do in animals . And

14:40

so in our research , what we really sort

14:43

of noticed is that in the polyclinical trials

14:45

you know you might have heard this phrase set

14:47

and setting in terms of psychedelic use , where

14:49

you know the mental set , so your expectations

14:52

coming in your current mood , your history

14:54

, your genetics , that plays a part , and

14:56

also the environment plays a part , right , so

14:58

you're the peers who is present

15:00

during that experience , the kind of support

15:02

you have , even just the physical

15:04

environment . All of that really affects

15:07

the experience and the outcome . And

15:09

so in these clinical trials people are

15:11

usually in a very , very safe feeling

15:13

environment . They're usually on a bed with relaxing

15:16

music and sometimes eye shades

15:18

, and there's usually two support

15:20

staff who are often trained

15:22

psychologists or therapists , and

15:25

the idea is not to give , like it's non-directive

15:27

, supportive therapies , what they call it , so it's not really

15:29

like therapy session , but there is that

15:32

sort of support . And so we thought maybe

15:34

what's happening is that the effect

15:36

of psilocybin is to sort of prime

15:38

people to get the most out of that

15:40

experience , and so really the effects while

15:43

the drug is on board are important

15:45

to study . We decided to do that

15:47

in rats And broadly what we found is

15:49

that , very surprisingly , rats

15:51

can be better at some tasks

15:53

that require cognitive flexibility

15:56

, so the ability to switch between

15:58

different behavioral strategies depending

16:00

on the environment , while they

16:03

are essentially high on psilocybin . And

16:05

you might ask , what does that have to do with depression

16:07

, what you've been talking about ? Well , cognitive

16:09

flexibility first of all , is associated with

16:12

greater mental health . Cognitive

16:14

inflexibility , so kind of rigid thinking

16:16

and rigid patterns of behavior

16:18

, are seen in a lot of psychiatric

16:21

disorders , including depression and

16:23

anxiety disorders , and higher

16:25

cognitive flexibility , which is measured

16:27

in humans with tasks very similar to the ones we

16:29

use in rodents , is associated with greater

16:32

mental health and also better responsiveness

16:35

to treatment for depression . So

16:37

there's a very strong link there , and

16:39

basically the idea is that the

16:41

hypothesis that we have , and others that propose as

16:43

well , is that psychedelics promote a

16:45

state of higher flexibility that then

16:47

allows people to break out of

16:49

ingrained patterns of thought , of

16:52

ingrained sort of negative perceptions

16:54

of the self or the environment

16:56

. Our research in animals sort

16:59

of supports that idea And also

17:01

, well , currently and in the near future

17:03

, we're trying to also understand how this is happening

17:05

, so that we can first of all get

17:08

an understanding of that process in the brain

17:10

which would be great And also perhaps

17:12

design compounds that can

17:14

better target the specific

17:16

therapeutic action that we're looking for , as

17:18

opposed to something like psilocybin , which is

17:20

perhaps having a lot of effects that are

17:23

maybe undesirable or even just unnecessary

17:25

.

17:26

I have a question about why rats would

17:28

be the best animal for this , Because , now that I'm

17:30

thinking about it , all the places I've worked

17:32

where there is like a psychology department or

17:34

something , it seems like rats are always the go-to model

17:37

And I didn't know if there was a particular reason . Is

17:39

it maybe just because their brains are bigger than mouse

17:41

brains ? or maybe they can learn different

17:43

tasks easier ? But I guess what's the draw

17:45

to rats for this type of work ?

17:47

Yeah , that's a great question , And it really boils down to the

17:49

fact that they're just smarter than mice . This

17:53

is what it is , the task that we use

17:55

. Mice would just not be able to do it .

17:57

Okay , interesting .

17:58

Were you guys planning on using , like higher

18:00

level species , like you plan on going into

18:02

monkeys or something like that to do this

18:05

, or is that something that's not necessary ?

18:07

I think it could be interesting , but our

18:09

lab is not equipped for that . I

18:11

wouldn't be surprised if someone started

18:14

doing research with psychedelics

18:16

and monkeys . I think the things

18:18

you can learn from all the different species

18:20

are a little bit different , you know , and so all

18:22

levels of research are valuable . But

18:24

even in mice people do a lot of research with psychedelics

18:27

and they just look at different things . So

18:29

for us , with the behavioral focus that we have

18:31

, rats are a really optimal choice .

18:33

That kind of lends itself to my next question , because we like

18:35

to ask our guests to kind of get their

18:37

take on this . In your opinion , is there a

18:39

way to conduct this research , or maybe

18:42

preliminary research , using computer

18:44

models or organs on a chip or any

18:46

other alternative , without using animals

18:48

? We just kind of like to get different opinions

18:50

on that .

18:52

I think , for what we are specifically trying

18:54

to do . What we're trying to understand is how

18:56

does the brain produce a certain behavior

18:59

and how does this external agent

19:01

, this drug , change brain activity

19:03

? that then changes the behavior , and so if you

19:05

don't have the behavior part , you're really

19:07

sort of losing a whole level of analysis

19:09

. So I don't think we could do the research we're doing using

19:12

a computer model or a brain in a vat

19:14

. Essentially , that said , i

19:16

think that those types of techniques or those

19:18

types of models are really valuable , because

19:21

our goal with animal research is always

19:23

to keep the number of animals that we're

19:25

using at a minimum . We always want to try

19:27

to minimize the number of animals and that can be done with careful

19:30

study , design and repeated measures

19:32

, designs and stuff . But of course , if

19:34

your question is answerable

19:37

without using an animal , then I

19:39

think it should be , and I think it would

19:41

be really cool to see some psychedelics research

19:43

done in , for example , brain

19:45

organoids , which I don't know maybe for some

19:47

of the listeners who are not familiar , what

19:49

these are is . You can take human

19:51

stem cells and then differentiate

19:54

them into neurons in a dish And it

19:56

becomes essentially a human

19:58

brain in a dish . Obviously it's not a real

20:00

human brain , it's not the same thing , but

20:02

it's been used to study human brain

20:04

development from a genetic standpoint , and

20:06

I think something like that would be great to answer some

20:09

questions about what psychedelics do , for

20:11

example , to specific cell types

20:13

that are found in a human brain but not in the brain

20:15

of a rat .

20:16

I've been asking , speaking about the brain and looking

20:18

at changes in cell type , and maybe you said it and

20:20

I mentioned . But when someone takes psilocybin

20:23

, like in these clinical trials , and then they look

20:25

at the brain , or when you guys are looking at your rats , are there

20:27

permanent changes to the brain

20:29

that psilocybin causes , like give

20:31

the long lasting kind of effects

20:33

that are like ? are they permanent changes

20:36

to the brain or are they just something that they're temporarily

20:38

changes to the brain that alleviate the signs

20:40

of depression , that then eventually

20:43

reverse back to normal cell

20:45

types , or should just have changes to do

20:47

with like synapses or just psilocybin just have like

20:49

a long half life in the body that's causing

20:51

the side effect that we see , if you know , alleviating

20:53

signs of depression .

20:55

Yeah , that's a fantastic question And it's not

20:57

a settled one . There are definitely long

21:00

lasting changes for psilocybin . So one

21:02

of the things that has come up we mentioned

21:04

ketamine before and this is true also for ketamine

21:06

. These compounds cause plasticity

21:09

. They cause growth of synapses

21:11

, essentially in the brain . When people have looked

21:13

and that can be something that's fairly long

21:15

lasting We don't have the data

21:18

to look , let's say a year after administration

21:20

but it does seem to be a persistent increase

21:23

in the potential number of synapses in

21:25

some parts of the brain after psilocybin

21:28

, after ketamine . Whether

21:30

that is directly related to

21:32

the depressive symptoms , it's hard

21:34

to confirm for sure , but that's

21:36

definitely a leading hypothesis about why

21:38

. The other option , which is kind

21:40

of like our angle , is a bit more that

21:42

this plasticity is perhaps a direct

21:44

correlate of the increased flexibility And

21:47

then it's kind of a resetting of the ability

21:49

to make new connections , and so maybe

21:52

you can think of it as rewiring

21:54

your brain in a way . So instead of

21:56

being stuck in this loop of negative self

21:58

perception and perception of the environment , you can

22:00

rewrite all that and come

22:02

to a different state after that experience

22:04

. Perhaps it's not a lasting physical

22:07

state , it's a physical state that lasts

22:10

a little bit and allows you to change

22:12

other things , but it's also possible

22:14

that there are changes that are , for example , gene

22:16

expression , that are very , very long lasting

22:19

, and we just don't know yet . So I would

22:21

say it's an open question , but it's definitely

22:23

something that people are looking into and

22:25

that is a very promising avenue for research

22:27

.

22:28

Yeah , absolutely , and we always

22:30

like to help our listeners kind of understand

22:32

the potential applications of the

22:34

research that you're doing . Maybe

22:36

it's like when we have guests on the show and how

22:38

that's going to affect our society

22:41

, and so you've said a lot already about how I

22:43

think your research has helped advance our understanding

22:45

of the use of these psychedelics , particularly

22:48

psilocybin . Do you think in our

22:50

lifetime even which I mean I think we're

22:52

all relatively young , but do you think

22:54

in our lifetime we're going to see these compounds

22:56

being accepted as a powerful

22:58

therapeutic tool , something that doesn't require

23:01

people suffering from PTSD

23:03

or depression to enroll in a

23:05

clinical trial , like , do you think psychiatrists

23:08

will eventually be able to use it in

23:11

practice , like regularly , outside of

23:13

clinical trials ? That's what I'm getting at , i guess

23:15

.

23:16

I think that's definitely where the momentum

23:18

is going . Here in Oregon , there

23:20

was a ballot question and it ended up

23:22

resulting in the approval of psilocybin

23:25

as a medication . essentially , it's a

23:27

complicated legal and logistical

23:29

framework , but it's definitely going that direction

23:32

. To answer your question of will it happen in

23:34

our lifetime , i think so , but

23:36

I want to be a little bit cautious . There's

23:38

a great paper from a couple of the

23:40

people that have been doing a lot of the clinical trials , like

23:42

Roland Griffith and David Yead . In

23:45

this little , very short paper they talk about the

23:47

bursting of the psychedelic bubble . Their

23:50

idea is when you have something that has so

23:52

much hype behind it , it creates this

23:54

bubble of expectations . It's kind of like you

23:56

get sensationalists , news articles

23:58

and this is the miracle cure for everything

24:00

and it's going to cure depression for everybody

24:02

. It's our role as scientists

24:04

to be realistic and cautious and

24:06

to not feed into that , because

24:08

what can happen is that you then get a

24:11

pushback when all

24:13

these incredibly high expectations don't materialize

24:15

and then you get very negative coverage

24:18

and very negative landscape for funding

24:20

and for continuing to do this research . This

24:23

is kind of what happened in the 50s and 60s

24:25

. People were so had such

24:27

high expectations on these compounds , which

24:29

were not illegal at the time , that they first

24:31

of all promoted them as they were going to be the cure-all

24:33

for the world and for

24:36

all the ailments in the world . But also

24:38

they did very , very sloppy research and

24:40

that , combined with popular culture , the

24:43

use of these compounds , but definitely the poor

24:45

research , contributed to these

24:47

substances being kind of written off the map

24:49

of research for several decades with

24:51

the Control Substances Act of 1970 . And

24:53

so I think it's important to be cautious about

24:55

our expectations . It's unlikely that

24:57

we're going to ever find a complete

25:00

cure , pharmacological cure , for depression

25:02

. That said , the more tools we

25:05

have in our toolbox , the better . I

25:07

really think that psychedelics are going to be one of

25:09

those tools , and it will take time . Ketamine

25:12

has had kind of a similar progression and it's taken

25:14

more than 20 years to go from the first

25:16

promising research to almost

25:18

FDA approval and there are actually ketamine clinics out

25:20

there . So I think it will happen and

25:22

hopefully we can sort of temper

25:24

expectations and be

25:26

careful with our research and really get these

25:29

compounds accepted as what

25:31

they are , which is another potential

25:33

tool in treating these disorders that are

25:35

very , very hard to treat and that really affect

25:37

people's lives .

25:38

But yeah , i hope that made sense Makes perfect

25:40

sense , and I think it's important too that

25:42

if they do become more widely accepted

25:45

and are able to start being used , that

25:47

they have to be used correctly , right ? Because you talked

25:49

about some of the negative side effects , but we didn't really talk

25:51

about what the negative side effects were , and

25:53

if people were using them irresponsibly or at

25:55

higher doses that were too high

25:57

for the human body to handle , then

26:00

they could have obviously very negative consequences

26:02

as well , right ?

26:04

Of course , safety is a big concern

26:06

with these things . I don't think it's ever going to be

26:08

the kind of medication that your doctor hands you , the

26:10

prescriptions like , yeah , one type

26:13

of acid every three weeks . that's

26:15

just not how it's going to work . More

26:17

likely than not it's going to be the way the clinical

26:20

trials have been done . So there's a preparatory

26:22

sessions and there's a very specific

26:24

sort of protocol for having the

26:26

experience with psychological

26:28

support , and then a debriefing session

26:30

and then another experience . You know it's

26:32

all very , very standardized and

26:35

the support is essential . So I think

26:37

that's probably how it's going to go . And yet

26:39

, when done that way , the adverse

26:41

effects and side effects are actually very

26:43

minimal . That's a good thing , yeah

26:45

, absolutely .

26:47

One question that we always like to ask our guests , and

26:49

I think you might have an interesting answer

26:51

, just based on your unique research

26:53

. How do you go about describing

26:55

to strangers what you do for a living , so

26:58

like if you're at a party or trapped

27:00

on an airplane next to someone like , how

27:02

do you bring up that you work with research

27:04

, animals and , on top of that , psychedelics

27:06

. It's kind of a unique combination .

27:09

It's funny because the psychedelics part

27:11

most people think , oh wow , that's so

27:13

cool , so that part is easy . And

27:16

then , yeah , the animal part is . I

27:18

recognize that it's a difficult sort of a delicate

27:20

subject for some people , and so you know , i've

27:22

asked people how they feel about animal research

27:24

, because it's kind of a sensitive subject , and

27:27

, interestingly , most of the time the response

27:29

I get from just people who are not scientists

27:31

at all . It's kind of the same thing that I feel

27:34

, which is that we're making this sort of ethical

27:36

bargain where animal research

27:38

is necessary because we

27:41

are trying to alleviate suffering

27:43

and trying to come up with treatments

27:45

for all these different disorders , And

27:47

even at a basic science level , we need to understand

27:50

the biology to be able to get back

27:52

to the translational level . I

27:55

feel like everyone has an understanding of that intuitively

27:58

. It makes it a little

28:00

bit easier to talk about that , but I find that people

28:02

don't actually , while they have this understanding

28:04

, they don't really know in practice

28:06

what that looks like , and so I'd really try

28:09

to make the point that we obviously try to

28:11

do this , while we do everything in the most humane

28:13

way possible . We have very standardized

28:15

protocols that are checked by veterinarians

28:17

, we use sterile

28:19

technique and anesthesia and all this when we

28:21

do surgeries on our animals , for example . But

28:24

also we try to minimize animal use . So

28:26

we can use statistical analysis and

28:28

careful study design And

28:31

, like you said , potentially alternative model

28:33

organisms or even computer modeling to

28:35

minimize how many animals we use And

28:37

so kind of trying to make the point that we understand

28:39

we're trying to do this in the most ethical

28:42

way possible . I feel like people are really

28:44

receptive and interested when they

28:46

actually learn how it happens in practice .

28:48

Yeah , absolutely , and that's great that

28:50

you make that point to kind of explain all that , because

28:53

once I mean , of course , like you said , try to minimize

28:55

the use , it'd be great not to have to use animals

28:57

, but unfortunately we still need

28:59

those animals to make discoveries that

29:02

are going to benefit both humans and animals . And

29:04

taking the time to explain it I think helps , because

29:06

99% of us have never

29:09

been inside of an animal or

29:11

a research laboratory and seen kind of how

29:13

it goes down or been a part of an eye-cook meeting and

29:15

hear the discussions that they have and how it's all

29:17

about . You have to have humane endpoints to ensure

29:20

they're not suffering and yada-yada . We've had

29:22

the whole eye-cook discussion down here before

29:24

about protocol breakdown and what goes into the protocols

29:26

. So if you want more information on the

29:28

approval process and how that goes , you can go back

29:31

to one of our earlier episodes and hear all of that

29:33

. But it's great that you take the time to do that

29:35

Before we wrap up this episode . Do you

29:37

have any other final statements or thoughts

29:39

about anything that we haven't covered

29:41

or anything you want to reiterate , just

29:43

kind of either whether or not it's about just

29:46

the animal research side of things or about your research

29:48

, or about the use of psychedelics or any other

29:50

topic .

29:51

And , sir , you gave me that opportunity . I would like to make a couple

29:53

points , if I may . One of them is related to your last question

29:56

and it's something I wanted to mention that I

29:58

forgot . I really want to make the point that

30:00

as scientists , we are accountable to

30:02

the public , because most of

30:04

the time , we are funded by taxpayer

30:06

dollars , and so I really appreciate

30:08

what you guys are doing here , and because

30:10

we want the public to see the value of

30:13

animal research , i want people to see the value

30:15

of the research that they're funding and get people behind

30:17

funding for translational and basic science

30:20

, and so thanks for doing this And

30:22

, yeah , i think that's an important part of science

30:24

. Communication is to really make that point

30:26

. And then the other thing I wanted to say is

30:28

that if you're interested in psychedelic

30:30

research , i really would like to

30:32

recommend the MAPS organization

30:34

. So MAPS is the Multidisciplinary Association

30:37

for Psychedelic Science , and they are sort of

30:39

a broad consortium of scientists and

30:41

people who work in field who are conducting a lot

30:43

of the clinical trials . I'm not part of them , but

30:45

I think it's a great thing to look in terms

30:48

of seeing what psychedelic research is about

30:50

, and so if you want to check them out there at mapsorg

30:52

and they're been actually behind

30:54

a lot of the trials with MDMA which we

30:57

haven't talked about but which will very likely

30:59

be approved as a treatment for PTSD

31:01

. So I just wanted to give a shout out to them . Oh

31:03

and sorry . One more thing , if you want to check us out

31:05

our lab is you can look up the Moga

31:07

Down Lab at OHSU .

31:09

We really appreciate your time . I've learned a lot

31:11

. I think all of our listeners have probably learned a lot after

31:13

listening to this whole episode . So

31:16

thank you for you being willing to come on

31:18

and talk to us and break all this

31:20

down . For us And it's , like we

31:22

said , kind of a topic that doesn't seem to be

31:24

discussed a whole lot , but hopefully we start hearing

31:26

more about it , because anything we can do to help benefit

31:29

mental health and especially depression

31:31

and PTSD and all that If we can do anything

31:33

to help those people suffering from that , it's

31:35

fantastic . So thank you for everything that you're

31:37

doing and thank you for taking the time to talk

31:40

to us today . We truly appreciate it .

31:42

Thanks so much , it was my pleasure .

31:44

Absolutely . Thanks , guys . Thanks , be

31:47

sure to go write review our show again If you're still

31:49

listening . Hopefully you made it this far . Write

31:51

review us . It does help us and we will

31:53

catch everyone next time . Thanks everyone .

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features