Podchaser Logo
Home
Ep 60: Number Bonds and Tape Diagrams

Ep 60: Number Bonds and Tape Diagrams

Released Tuesday, 10th August 2021
Good episode? Give it some love!
Ep 60: Number Bonds and Tape Diagrams

Ep 60: Number Bonds and Tape Diagrams

Ep 60: Number Bonds and Tape Diagrams

Ep 60: Number Bonds and Tape Diagrams

Tuesday, 10th August 2021
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:01

Hey fellow

0:01

mathematicians. Welcome to the

0:04

podcast where Math is

0:04

Figure-Out-Able. I'm Pam.

0:09

And I'm Kim.

0:10

And we make the case

0:10

that mathematizing is not about

0:14

mimicking steps or rote

0:14

memorizing facts, but it's about

0:17

thinking and reasoning; about

0:17

creating and using mental

0:21

relationships. That math class

0:21

can be less like it has been for

0:25

so many of us and more like

0:25

mathematicians working together.

0:30

We take the strong stance that

0:30

not only are algorithms not

0:35

particularly helpful in

0:35

teaching, but that mimicking

0:39

algorithms actually keeps

0:39

students from being the

0:42

mathematicians they can be. We

0:42

answer the question, if not

0:47

algorithms and step by step

0:47

procedures, then what?

0:51

So in the last two

0:51

episodes, we began a

0:53

conversation around models and

0:53

modeling, right, and how it's

0:56

important to consider the

0:56

progression of modeling First,

0:59

the model the situation, then

0:59

the model of student thinking,

1:03

and then using that model as a

1:03

tool.

1:06

And in this

1:06

progression of modeling, notice

1:09

that it's not about any model.

1:09

It's about models that can

1:13

become tools, important tools.

1:13

So in this episode, we're gonna

1:19

get specific about a couple of

1:19

recently popular models that we

1:23

think should be de-emphasized

1:23

and what we can do instead. So

1:29

y'all, I often get asked about

1:29

what I think about number bonds

1:34

and tape diagrams, or sometimes

1:34

called strip diagrams, and how

1:40

they play out in some current

1:40

textbooks. People wonder about

1:43

that. People are really curious

1:43

about how they fit. I think

1:48

people might be noticing that we

1:48

don't use those two models... at

1:54

all.

1:55

At all. So Pam,

1:55

let's start with number bonds.

1:58

What is a number bond? And will

1:58

you describe a number bond for

2:02

the listeners?

2:03

Okay, so I think a

2:03

number bond is a well

2:06

intentioned idea of helping kids

2:06

focus on the relationships at

2:10

hand, focus on what's sort of

2:10

happening in the problem. And so

2:15

Often, it

2:15

looks like circles and there's

2:20

two circles that are sort of the

2:20

parts. And those circles are

2:24

kind of connected to this

2:24

larger, the whole. And so

2:28

there's like a big circle,

2:28

that's the whole, and two

2:30

circles that are the parts. And

2:30

so - like explaining this over a

2:35

podcast is tricky. So Kim, pick

2:35

a number.

2:38

65.

2:39

Okay, so a number

2:39

bond for 65, could be I put 65

2:42

in sort of the total place. And

2:42

then I kind of draw two lines

2:46

that come off of that circle,

2:46

and then I draw two smaller

2:48

circles. And I could say 60 in

2:48

one and five in the other. Or I

2:53

could do a different number bond

2:53

where 65 in the total. And I can

2:56

have 50 in one and 15 in the

2:56

other. Or I can even have one

3:00

where I have 65 in the total and

3:00

I could have 70 in one and five

3:05

in the other because 70 minus

3:05

five is 65. And then I might

3:08

have to play around with which

3:08

one's bigger and all the things.

3:11

Okay, so it's like sort of like,

3:11

how are these numbers related to

3:15

each other? Alright, so let's

3:15

also talk about the other one.

3:17

So we talked about number bonds

3:17

and what they look like. Kim,

3:20

what's a tape diagram? Or strip

3:20

diagram?

3:23

So a tape diagram

3:23

is a rectangular box, maybe,

3:26

with a couple of empty spaces

3:26

where students would take

3:31

numbers from a problem they're

3:31

given. And they are supposed to

3:35

put the numbers in the correct

3:35

spot. So like, if it was a

3:38

missing add-in problem, they

3:38

might fill in one part and the

3:42

total from the numbers in the

3:42

problem. And then they need to

3:46

find the other part. It's a way

3:46

to organize information from the

3:50

problem.

3:51

Yeah, exactly. So I might have the total kind of outside the rectangle. I've seen

3:53

where I might have one

3:57

rectangle, and like you just

3:57

said, the parts are inside and

4:00

the total is kind of outside the

4:00

rectangle. I've also seen it

4:02

where the total is its own

4:02

rectangle and then the parts are

4:06

kind of a rectangle below that

4:06

cut in pieces, and you sort of

4:10

put the parts in that second

4:10

rectangle that is the same size

4:14

as the total. Okay, so let's

4:14

talk about how those two models

4:21

are, in a way, a model of the

4:21

situation. So we've talked about

4:27

our modeling context that we

4:27

like, where kind of the

4:30

beginning step is a model of the

4:30

situation. And so in a big way,

4:34

we feel like number bonds and

4:34

strip diagrams are models of the

4:39

situation. They are an attempt

4:39

to get the relationships out

4:43

there to make them visible. So

4:43

which one's the part? Which

4:47

one's the whole? Oh, do you have

4:47

two parts and you need to find

4:50

the whole? And so both of those

4:50

models can sort of help kind of

4:55

get that understood. If you

4:55

think about a problem solving

5:00

step-by-step procedure -which we

5:00

don't advocate - but often that

5:03

first thing is understand the

5:03

problem, right?

5:06

Yep.

5:06

It's like important.

5:06

And why is that the first thing?

5:09

Well, because it's important to

5:09

understand the problem, or

5:12

you're probably not going to

5:12

solve it correctly. So of

5:15

course, we need to understand

5:15

the problem. And so this is a

5:18

well, my number bonds and strip

5:18

diagrams, are well intentioned

5:21

attempts to say to kids,

5:21

"Understand the problem". Where

5:24

it can go awry is when we demand

5:24

that kids draw it every time. If

5:28

I'm a kid who already understand

5:28

what's happening in the problem,

5:32

I don't think it's very helpful

5:32

to demand that I draw the number

5:35

bond or the strip diagram. It's

5:35

just like an extra step that

5:38

doesn't help me; it's not

5:38

improving my understanding. If

5:41

I'm the kid who doesn't

5:41

understand what's going on in

5:44

the problem, it might help to

5:44

draw the number diagram or the

5:48

strip diagram, it might help to

5:48

do that. But often, it also

5:51

becomes like sort of this extra

5:51

thing to do. It's like, how big

5:54

do I draw the rectangle? And

5:54

does it need to be tall?Kids

5:57

like to pay attention to all

5:57

this weird stuff that may not

6:01

have to do anything with the

6:01

problem itself. So it kind of

6:04

becomes like this extraneous

6:04

thing that may or may not be

6:07

helpful. So do we want kids to

6:07

understand what's happening?

6:10

Absolutely. So also another

6:10

thing that can kind of get in

6:13

the way. So think CGI, have we

6:13

ever done a podcast episode on CGI?

6:18

Surely we've mentioned it.

6:19

If we haven't, we'll

6:19

do one soon. But if you're not

6:21

familiar with Cognitively Guided

6:21

Instruction, and we really

6:24

appreciate Cognitively Guided

6:24

Instruction - which is referred

6:27

to as CGI - because they did a

6:27

lot of really groundbreaking

6:30

research that then has allowed

6:30

us to build on that research.

6:34

But sometimes we might have the

6:34

opportunity to get a little bit

6:37

mixed up here. In CGI, there's a

6:37

strategy called direct modeling.

6:43

Direct modeling is all about

6:43

acting out what's actually

6:46

happening in the problem, a good

6:46

thing, right? Another well

6:49

meaning like, let's model what's

6:49

going on in the problem. That

6:53

can be a very necessary thing

6:53

for kids to do when they are

6:57

beginning problem solvers. They

6:57

need to like actually act out

7:00

what's happening. And then they

7:00

use that acting out to help them

7:03

solve the problem. But that can

7:03

be different than a model of the

7:07

situation. Because it can be...

7:07

how do I say this? Direct

7:14

modeling is where you're

7:14

actually like acting out the

7:17

problem, what's actually

7:17

happening in the problem. Strip

7:20

diagrams and number bonds are

7:20

less of acting out the problem

7:25

and more of a model of the

7:25

relationships at hand, or a

7:30

model of the relationships that

7:30

are happening in the problem.

7:33

But what's interesting is, all

7:33

of those are a model of the

7:38

situation. In our framework,

7:38

whether I'm acting out the

7:43

problem, to understand it, and

7:43

to solve it, maybe just say to

7:47

understand it, whether acting it

7:47

out of direct modeling, or I'm

7:50

drawing a number bond, a strip

7:50

diagram as getting the

7:53

relationships out of what's

7:53

happening in the problem, we

7:56

would consider all of that the

7:56

model of the situation.

8:01

Right.

8:01

That's sort of the

8:01

beginning of our kind of

8:04

modeling scenario here, or the

8:04

way that we think about

8:07

modeling. So it might be true

8:07

that a student needs to act out

8:11

the problem, directly model the

8:11

problem with actions or drawing

8:15

what's actually happening. And

8:15

that's fine. And we would call

8:18

that a model of the situation.

8:18

But we can also be more abstract

8:22

in general, like the number bond

8:22

and strip or tape diagram, where

8:26

it's more of a graphic of the

8:26

relationships involved, it's

8:29

less the action that's

8:29

happening. Either way, these

8:32

models of the situation set up

8:32

the relationships. What they

8:36

don't do is represent what

8:36

you're actually doing to solve

8:39

the problem. How you're actually

8:39

using the relationships to compute.

8:44

Yeah.

8:44

So let me get a little bit more specific. I know I'm doing a lot of like, not

8:46

very exampled discussion here.

8:49

So let's get examples. Let's get

8:49

to some examples. So what's an

8:53

example of a number bond and how

8:53

it can be helpful, and maybe

8:58

less helpful than we might have

8:58

been led to believe? So here's a

9:04

problem. Let's say, "Hey, we're

9:04

reading. We like to read. Kim

9:07

and I both read, reading's good.

9:07

We're on page 195 of the book.

9:13

And there are 303 pages in the

9:13

book. Hey, how much do we have

9:18

left to read?" That could be a

9:18

great problem, right? There's

9:21

the problem. We need kids to

9:21

first understand the problem. We

9:24

don't want kids flipping a coin,

9:24

and just grabbing the numbers

9:28

195, 303, and like adding them

9:28

or multiplying or dividing. We

9:32

don't want that to happen.

9:33

Sure.

9:33

We want them to actually understand what's happening. So kids could draw a

9:34

number bond or a tape diagram

9:40

with, let's do a number bond

9:40

first. They could put 195 in one

9:43

little circle and 303 in the

9:43

total circle, and then a blank

9:50

in the other sort of

9:50

part-circle, the other smaller

9:52

circle. Like, that's how they

9:52

can sort of say, "Okay, I know

9:55

what's happening. I've got 303

9:55

in the total, that 303 is the

9:58

total number of pages and I'm at

9:58

195. So that's part and then I

10:02

have this other part that's the

10:02

pages I have left to read." I

10:05

could also do that on a tape

10:05

diagram. I could have the whole

10:08

rectangle be 303. And then I

10:08

could have that second rectangle

10:12

where I've cut it and I've put

10:12

195 in part of it and I need to

10:16

find the other part of that

10:16

second rectangle, the empty part

10:19

to find. But now what?

10:21

Yeah.

10:21

We've represented

10:21

the situation, the scenario,

10:23

we've got it, we understand

10:23

what's happening. But now how do

10:26

we find that missing part? Okay,

10:26

so Kim, how might the kids solve

10:30

that problem?

10:31

Right, and here's

10:31

the important part, right? So if

10:33

I'm thinking about 303 minus

10:33

195, I might say 303 minus 200,

10:42

and get 103. And then I've

10:42

subtracted too much. So I can

10:46

add back that five, to get 108.

10:49

Bam, and you use a

10:49

nice over strategy.

10:52

Right.

10:52

You thought about

10:52

that problem as subtraction, and

10:55

you subtracted a bit too much.

10:55

And then you add it back, nice,

10:58

super. That's the strategy.

10:58

We're focusing on how students

11:02

are solving that problem. And

11:02

that was a very nice,

11:05

sophisticated strategy. Super.

11:05

So now as a teacher in our sort

11:08

of way that we think about

11:08

modeling, now it's time for our

11:12

second thing, where I would take

11:12

what Kim just did and I would

11:15

model that. I would make it

11:15

visible. I would draw an open

11:19

number line. And I would start

11:19

at 303. And I would draw this

11:22

big jump back of 200. And I

11:22

would say, "Where did you land

11:26

again?" Land on 103. And then I

11:26

would say, "Kim, what did you do

11:30

again?" Depending on how much

11:30

I'm working with the student.

11:33

And she says, "I subtracted too

11:33

much." And so I'm like, "How

11:37

much too much?" She said, "Five

11:37

too much." So then I would draw

11:40

back. The jumps shouldn't have

11:40

been as long so I would back up

11:44

five? And then okay, so what is

11:44

that? 103 and then up that five.

11:48

So now we're at 108. And I would

11:48

draw that 108 and I would go,

11:52

"Hey, does this represent what

11:52

you just did?" Look at it, check

11:55

it out the relationships you

11:55

just used, now they are visible.

11:59

We can use this open number line

11:59

to make that visible. Cool. Then

12:03

over time, I can ask that kid,

12:03

"Hey, you've seen me represent

12:06

your thinking, you've seen me do

12:06

that a few times. Now the next

12:10

time you do that over strategy,

12:10

I want you to represent your

12:14

thinking, now you make your

12:14

thinking visible, just like

12:17

we've done, cool." Then over

12:17

time, and with lots of

12:20

experience, that model becomes a

12:20

tool. And kids might actually

12:24

use that model to solve that

12:24

problem. That's our hope. That's

12:27

our modeling sequence. Okay, so

12:27

Kim, what if a kid used a

12:31

different strategy? Same problem.

12:33

Sure. So a couple

12:33

of other strategies, right? So

12:36

here's one. What if I found the

12:36

difference between 195 and 303.

12:42

So that might look like the

12:42

teacher drawing a number line.

12:46

So let me describe what I would

12:46

do first. So if I had 195, then

12:50

I could add five to get to 200.

12:50

And then I could just add 103,

12:56

to get to 303. So add five to

12:56

200, and then add 103 to 303.

13:02

And then that would look like a

13:02

teacher drawing the number line

13:05

with the 195 on one end, and the

13:05

303 on the other end, and making

13:11

those hops that I just

13:11

described, kind of on top of the

13:14

number line to show the plus

13:14

five and the plus 103, which is

13:19

the total of 108.

13:21

Excellent. And you would write all that out. The teacher would model that

13:23

student's thinking, represent

13:27

that student's thinking. Sometimes people call it annotating student's thinking, I

13:29

don't really like that so much.

13:33

Maybe we'll bring that up in a

13:33

minute. I like representing the

13:37

student's thinking using that

13:37

number line, because we know

13:40

that's an important powerful

13:40

tool. But you said a couple

13:43

strategies. All right, give me another one.

13:44

So how about you

13:44

give me one? Maybe two.

13:47

Okay. So if I'm

13:47

thinking about 195 and 303, and

13:52

the distance between them, I

13:52

might choose to find that

13:55

somewhere slightly different. I

13:55

might put them both on the

13:59

number line, and nudge them up

13:59

both five.

14:03

So now I'm finding

14:03

the distance or difference

14:04

Oh, okay. Lots of

14:04

crossy-outies.

14:06

between 200 and 308, because

14:06

I've moved both of them up five,

14:10

because 308 minus 200, bam is

14:10

just 108. And I'm just there.

14:14

And I'm sort of done. And then

14:14

we call that the Constant

14:18

Difference strategy. So as we

14:18

just talked about this kind of

14:22

Yeah, lots of

14:22

crossy-outies. Again, over time,

14:22

sequence of modeling, I have

14:22

gotten to the point now where

14:26

when I solve subtraction

14:26

problems, I think on an open

14:30

number line. It has become a

14:30

tool for reasoning for me. But

14:34

funny, are you ready? When I

14:34

just did that, if you guys could

14:38

have seen me, I literally had my

14:38

hands in the air. And I put one

14:42

hand where I could see 195 and I

14:42

put another hand where I could

14:46

see 303 and I move them both to

14:46

the right up five. So that I

14:51

could see that 200 and see that

14:51

308 and then I could solve the

14:55

problem. Now, it took me way

14:55

longer to discuss what I did,

14:59

than it did for me to just, like

14:59

my hands were there. And it took

15:03

me way shorter to solve that

15:03

problem, way more efficient

15:07

solve that problem than it would

15:07

have - could you imagine the

15:11

traditional algorithm for 303

15:11

minus 195? Wow, all those

15:15

opportunities for error. and with lots of experience,

15:22

that model becomes a tool and

15:26

then kids, and me, actually use

15:26

it to solve problems. Notice

15:31

that the model that we're

15:31

advocating is an open number

15:34

line, that is a tool worth

15:34

building.

15:37

It is. And I'm going to go back for a second because I think you mentioned

15:39

something that we kind of

15:41

glossed over. And the really,

15:41

really important thing that I

15:44

think you just said, was that

15:44

over time you ask a kid, "Hey,

15:48

when your brain does that, it

15:48

can look like this. Does this

15:52

match what your brain did?"

15:52

Because we want to represent

15:56

their thinking. So we just

15:56

talked about a model that we

15:59

love for subtraction and really

15:59

addition too, and that's the

16:02

open number line. But for

16:02

multiplication, division and

16:05

proportional reasoning problems,

16:05

Pam, what's your preferred

16:08

model?

16:09

Yeah, that's a really good question, because we started talking about strip

16:11

diagrams and tape diagrams. And

16:13

that's often where we see tape

16:13

and strip diagrams, I'm saying

16:17

them like, they're two different things. They're just called those so maybe I should choose

16:19

one, strip diagrams. We see

16:22

those often in proportional

16:22

reasoning problems, where we

16:25

sort of set up what's happening

16:25

in the proportion, and then we

16:28

can kind of think about the

16:28

relationship and those ratios,

16:32

and we can kind of solve them.

16:32

But how do we solve them, that's

16:34

the rub, right? Like, it's just

16:34

the setup, it's just - the strip

16:38

diagram only helps me get a feel

16:38

for what's happening. It doesn't

16:43

then help me use the numbers in

16:43

the structure to solve the

16:46

problem. It definitely doesn't

16:46

model what I do to solve the

16:49

problem, it just sort of sets up

16:49

the scenario, the situation. So

16:52

in Episode 58, we actually

16:52

walked through kind of a way

16:56

that we use ratio tables, we use

16:56

our modeling paradigm to think

17:01

about setting up a ratio table.

17:01

And I use sticks of gum. So if I

17:05

have sticks of gum in a pack,

17:05

then I can think about how I can

17:09

model that situation by I start

17:09

putting down in a table. Okay,

17:13

I've got one pack to 17 sticks,

17:13

I've got two packs to how many

17:17

sticks. As kids say those

17:17

numbers, I put them in the

17:20

table, and it's a model of the

17:20

situation. And then as they

17:24

solve problems, I model their

17:24

thinking. I represent their

17:27

thinking using scaling, or using

17:27

lines to sort of, I'm adding

17:32

packs together, so I add the six

17:32

together. Or I'm scaling as I

17:35

double the packs. Or as I

17:35

multiply the packs times 10,

17:38

then I multiply the sticks times

17:38

10. And I represent all that

17:41

thinking on the ratio table.

17:41

That's the model of thinking.

17:45

And the more that we do that and

17:45

through time and experience,

17:48

then that tool becomes a model

17:48

for thinking. Well, I can do the

17:53

same kind of a thing with a non

17:53

unit rate scenario. So if I have

17:57

something like four slices of

17:57

pizza for $5. I represent that

18:02

scenario. First it's a model of

18:02

the scenario, of the context,

18:05

where I put four slices of pizza

18:05

for $5. And then as students

18:10

think about different numbers of

18:10

slices, and the cost or

18:14

different amounts of money, and

18:14

the number of slices of pizza I

18:16

get - as they solve those

18:16

problems I represent their

18:20

thinking using that ratio table.

18:20

And our goal is then to

18:23

transition to where that ratio

18:23

table becomes a tool for

18:28

thinking, in order to think. And

18:28

as students when they see a

18:31

proportion, they think to

18:31

themselves "Oh, well, you know,

18:33

how how does that fit in a ratio

18:33

table? Oh, I can use that ratio

18:37

table as a tool for thinking."

18:39

So in a nutshell,

18:39

these other models that we've

18:42

been talking about are not bad

18:42

or wrong. They're just really

18:45

limited. They can help students

18:45

slow down and evaluate what's

18:50

happening. It's just a format,

18:50

though. And in any case, it

18:54

doesn't help you decide what to

18:54

do with the numbers, like you

18:56

said. But teachers think it

18:56

does. It's like manipulatives

19:00

where we think the math is

19:00

embodied in the manipulative but

19:03

that's only because we've

19:03

already schematized it, not

19:07

because we can now just show

19:07

what we worked out and have it

19:10

magically transfer to our students.

19:12

Oh that's so well said.

19:13

Yeah, teachers

19:13

have probably seen this a ton,

19:16

right? You might have seen the

19:16

student who may or may not be

19:21

able to take the numbers from a

19:21

word problem and figure out

19:23

where to place them in a tape

19:23

diagram. Maybe they can do that.

19:27

But even if they're able to,

19:27

many of them get stuck in

19:30

knowing how they want to mess with the numbers.

19:32

Yeah, even if they get to that point. Now they're there. What do we do next? And

19:34

what we haven't done is help

19:38

teachers know what to do next.

19:38

Well, woolah! We are helping you

19:42

hopefully know what to do next.

19:42

Ask lots of good problems, pull

19:46

out that thinking from kids, you

19:46

represent their thinking using a

19:49

very important model that then

19:49

can become a tool for them to

19:52

solve problems with. So it's

19:52

helpful to figure out what's in

19:57

a problem. Okay, you could do it

19:57

in other ways. But tape

20:00

diagrams, number bonds is a fine

20:00

way, that's fine. But don't get

20:04

caught in demanding that from

20:04

students because number bonds

20:08

and tape diagrams are not

20:08

particularly helpful to know how

20:12

to mess with the numbers after

20:12

that. And they're also not the

20:16

models that we believe will

20:16

become the tools we want

20:20

students to become really

20:20

familiar with, so that they can

20:23

use them as tools for thinking

20:23

and reasoning and mathematizing.

20:27

Alright, so if you want to learn

20:27

more mathematics and refine your

20:32

math teaching so that you and

20:32

students are mathematizing more

20:37

and more, then join the Math is

20:37

Figure-Out-Able movement and

20:41

help us spread the word that

20:41

Math is Figure-Out-Able!

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features