Podchaser Logo
Home
Dr. Sanjay Gupta on how to build a better brain

Dr. Sanjay Gupta on how to build a better brain

Released Thursday, 15th April 2021
Good episode? Give it some love!
Dr. Sanjay Gupta on how to build a better brain

Dr. Sanjay Gupta on how to build a better brain

Dr. Sanjay Gupta on how to build a better brain

Dr. Sanjay Gupta on how to build a better brain

Thursday, 15th April 2021
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

Hi

0:03

everyone. I'm Katie Kuric, and this is

0:05

next question. You know, I've

0:08

known and admire Dr Sanjay

0:10

Gupta for years. He's such

0:12

a great guy. He's also a neuroscientist

0:15

and the chief medical correspondent for

0:17

CNN, which, as you can imagine,

0:19

when COVID hit put him at

0:21

the center of the pandemic storm.

0:24

This may be a new pathogen that circulates around

0:26

the world. You're running into a situation where you just

0:28

don't have beds. If those numbers don't

0:30

budge, it's gonna be very hard to get to hurt immunity.

0:33

I think this has added a lot more urgency to

0:35

an already very urgent situation.

0:38

There has been so much to cover. In fact,

0:41

Sanjay also launched a daily

0:43

podcast on the subject. I'm Dr

0:45

Sanjay Gupta, CNN's chief medical

0:47

correspondent, and this is coronavirus

0:50

fact versus fiction. Throughout

0:53

this very long year, Sanja

0:56

has really acted as our guide, helping

0:58

us understand the thorny virus

1:01

and what it means for our health, our communities,

1:04

and our country. And he admits

1:06

it's been as all consuming as you

1:08

might think. All I think about,

1:11

Katie has been COVID. You know, I

1:13

viral transmission, how

1:15

people evaluate risk social

1:18

behavior. But there is some

1:20

joy in getting so head down in something.

1:22

I feel like we live such distracted lives.

1:25

You get a little bit about a lot of things,

1:28

like I really know so much about this virus.

1:33

I also wanted to talk to Sanjay because

1:35

he's got a new book out. Yeah, believe

1:38

it or not, he found time to write

1:40

a book during this crazy year. It's

1:42

called Keep Sharp, Building

1:45

a Better Brain at any Age, and

1:47

it's fascinating. It's also a

1:49

practical guide for better

1:51

brain health, something I'm extremely

1:53

interested in. And don't worry, we do

1:56

get into that, but since I have the COVID

1:58

expert of experts, I couldn't

2:00

help but start our conversation there. You

2:04

must feel like you you have a PhD

2:06

in virology at this point, right, Yeah,

2:09

I mean totally and and and the the irony

2:11

is, Katie, is that this is a novel

2:14

virus, right, So I

2:16

mean novel actually means something,

2:18

which which that didn't really strike me until

2:21

a few months into this either, Like I think because

2:24

you said the PhD in virology. The the irony

2:26

is that that I think people

2:28

who had a lot of knowledge about this. In some

2:30

ways it got

2:32

in their way because it's very hard to

2:34

think about something as novel. You immediately

2:37

want to put it into a box. It's the box

2:39

of stars, it's the box

2:41

of H one, N one, whatever you

2:43

come up with. But this was novel,

2:46

which means that if you try to put it in a box,

2:49

you probably got it wrong. So they

2:51

had to cast aside their preconceived notions

2:54

completely, and that's

2:56

hard to do, right for a scientist,

2:58

it's really hard to do, and and it

3:00

goes against sort of how you think about things. Let's

3:02

get the best experts. And by the way,

3:04

I think there's really really

3:07

value, great value and expertise. Don't

3:09

get me wrong, But what you would

3:11

do is grab the coronavirus

3:13

experts, grabbed the pandemic experts, and that

3:16

was all important. But this virus

3:18

was just behaving in a totally novel

3:20

way. I mean. One of the best examples, as

3:22

you well know, was everybody

3:25

believed that respiratory virus is

3:27

really only spread when you were sick. When

3:30

you were when you had symptoms, that's when you spread.

3:32

And you know, the guidance was, will screen people

3:34

at airports, will tell people to stay home if they're

3:36

sick, which people should do anyway, regardless

3:39

of whether in a pandemic, and

3:41

we should be able to quell this thing. No

3:44

one really believed initially that

3:46

this thing would spread most efficiently

3:49

when people didn't have symptoms. That

3:51

that was that's never really happened

3:53

before, is it. As Dr Fauci has said, never in the

3:55

history of respiratory viruses has

3:57

that happened before. That's novel. I

4:00

mean, you know, remember the story of typhoid Mary.

4:03

She was a silent carrier of typhoid.

4:06

It was so dramatic because she infected

4:08

all these people in this single residents

4:11

in this community and all that, and people

4:13

couldn't figure it out. This is

4:15

like millions of typhoid Mary's in a way

4:17

of a of a brand new disease,

4:19

COVID, So it is it was quite extraordinary

4:22

to sort of see that, uh, see see how

4:24

that all played out. Well, before

4:26

we talk about your your new book,

4:28

keep sharp, because I'm really interested,

4:31

as someone who's sixty four, in

4:33

maintaining my mental acuity

4:37

as i age. I just want

4:39

to ask you one last question about COVID, and

4:41

that is, are we seeing

4:44

the light at the end of the tunnel. Every time

4:46

I feel optimistic, sang, I

4:48

then read something about variance or

4:50

increased cases, and it's

4:53

quite nerve racking, I think for

4:55

the average person who doesn't have

4:58

a medical degree or hasn't been deeply,

5:00

deeply entrenched in the science of this, I

5:03

mean, are we screwed? Are we at

5:05

the tail end of this pandemic? I

5:08

do feel the light on my face, in

5:11

your face. I mean, I do think that the

5:13

the tunnel is is the end

5:15

of the tunnels in sight? I mean, the

5:17

then can I just

5:19

can I just remind people, and I think

5:21

this is such an important reminder that they were having rave

5:23

parties in Wuhan at the end of

5:26

last summer. And I bring

5:28

that up only to say that we talked about the vaccines.

5:30

We talked about the fact that science is now rescuing

5:33

us, which is great, fantastic,

5:36

but so much of this didn't need to happen.

5:38

And I and I and I know that's not your question, Katie,

5:40

but I just feel like I can never answer a question about

5:43

um, about the sort of future

5:46

or being optimistic about this pandemic because I'm

5:49

so it's just so, it's

5:51

so, I'm so angry in so

5:53

many ways. I mean, you know, six people

5:56

died and some of them are my friends. And I've

5:58

seen families, I talk to families still,

6:01

I just it just this may not have even

6:03

been the Black Swan event, right, we think

6:05

of this black Swan event, this really contagious

6:07

virus, which this was, but something that

6:10

has a two to three percent mortality that would

6:12

be awful, that would be the Black Swan event.

6:14

This wasn't even that. There were countries around the world

6:16

that immediately quelled this and measure

6:18

their debts in the hundreds instead of the hundreds of thousands.

6:21

Having said that, we are a society.

6:24

Because we are we

6:26

focus on touchdowns and home runs and knockouts.

6:29

We don't care much for singles and doubles.

6:31

Because we're that society. We waited for science

6:33

to rescue us. And and the vaccines

6:35

will. I think they're really extraordinary

6:38

and really effective. They seem to be pretty

6:40

effective against the variant B

6:42

one one seven, the UK variant,

6:45

because there's a lot of concern about that variant. But if

6:47

you have been vaccinated, or

6:49

if you had the infection in the past, the

6:52

other you know, the the circulating coronavirus

6:54

that should also protect you. So it's really

6:56

I think it's really good. And I think with the warmer, warmer

6:58

weather in the summer, viral transmission

7:01

rates will go down. That would be great.

7:03

I do think, you know, um, we'll

7:05

probably get to hurt immunity over the summer,

7:08

but but it's worth reminding that her immunity

7:10

isn't a sort of destination. Necessarily.

7:13

You can pop in and out of her immunity.

7:15

So if not enough people get vaccinated over

7:18

you know, the next few months, then

7:20

going into the colder weather again in the fall,

7:23

we could see resurgences. It's

7:25

quite disturbing when you hear about the

7:27

people who are refusing to

7:29

get vaccinated. Many of

7:31

them are white men, Uh,

7:33

in this country. I think you see

7:35

the impact of politics on

7:38

that number, not only in terms

7:40

of the response to the pandemic,

7:42

but now to the response to the

7:44

vaccines. Uh. That

7:46

must be quite disturbing

7:49

for you too, it is for me. Yeah.

7:52

I mean there's been no not

7:54

a single part of this entire pandemic

7:57

that hasn't been politicized in some way.

7:59

I mean that I guess now you say that's in

8:02

in April of two one, and it's

8:04

obvious, right, everyone knows that, but started

8:07

starting off covering the story,

8:09

um and now all the way now to the

8:11

vaccines, even every single component

8:14

has been politicized in some way. So it is

8:16

it is disturbing the anti vax

8:18

movement. And you may not even remember this, Katie,

8:20

but I actually did a segment on your your show

8:23

years ago about anti

8:25

vaccination movement at that time around

8:28

h one and one. But it's been around

8:31

for a long time, the anti vaccination movement,

8:33

and it's sort of you know, it simmers it. We

8:36

saw measles outbreaks in Brooklyn and

8:38

and Disneyland and Minnesota.

8:40

This seems bigger though you know that

8:43

was a particular group, uh,

8:46

and it really dealt primarily with

8:49

childhood vaccinations and

8:51

now this has expanded

8:53

to um, you know, these

8:56

adults who I

8:59

don't know for one reason or another. I think you

9:01

can understand people of color and

9:03

the terrible history of Tuskegee

9:06

and some of the ways that people of color have

9:09

been abused in scientific research

9:11

in the past, and this kind of deeply

9:13

ingrained mistrust of the medical

9:16

community. So I think you can appreciate

9:18

that. But this is, you know, this

9:20

is a whole other ball

9:22

of wax, isn't it? Yes, it

9:25

really is. And and you know we're we're seeing

9:27

some of this for or really seeing it come

9:29

to light, I should say, you know, in a

9:31

in a pretty dramatic way. Now. I think I think

9:33

I was reading the statistics this morning, Katie Kaiser

9:36

Family Foundation, forty of

9:39

those in rural areas who say they

9:41

absolutely will not take the vaccine.

9:43

It's not any question of hey, I want more information,

9:45

I want to see how this plays out. They're just saying,

9:47

out of the gate they absolutely not take it. And

9:50

what said, what is the explanation? That's

9:52

the curious thing, Like you said, with some people

9:55

who are vaccine hesitants, it is concerns about

9:57

safety or mistrust or you

10:00

know, uh my my grandfather

10:02

was experimented on as part of

10:04

Tuskegee, you know, things like that. With

10:06

this, I think it's almost an extension

10:09

of this pandemic. Isn't even

10:11

real. It's not a hoax. Why would I take a vaccine

10:13

for something that's a hoax.

10:16

I'm not scared for safety

10:18

of it. I just don't. I think the whole thing is

10:20

sort of you know, the scam demic sort

10:22

of thing. So I don't know if that's

10:24

the case. For it's a huge percentage of people were

10:26

talking about here, so maybe there's a some heterogeneity,

10:29

you know, some variety of opinions there.

10:32

But bottom line, if if, if

10:34

the numbers stay that high, we're not going

10:36

to get to hurt immunity based on vaccinating

10:38

adults alone, which is it's

10:42

it's it's so. I mean, gosh, we wait for science

10:44

to rescue us. We don't do the basic public health

10:46

practices, and then when when this truly extraordinary

10:49

scientific achievement occurs

10:51

in the form of this vaccine, people

10:53

don't take it. You know, if you

10:55

are a Martian coming to planet Earth and saying,

10:57

so, let me get this straight.

11:00

So you didn't do anything about the virus.

11:02

You waited, created this amazing medicine, and

11:04

then you don't take the medicine to it.

11:07

Just it doesn't make any sense. It's a head scratcher

11:09

for sure. When do you think

11:11

Sanjay will be able to go

11:14

about our daily lives without mass

11:17

I think it'll be this summer, Katie,

11:19

I I really do. I mean, I know that there's people

11:21

who are painting a more dire prediction

11:23

around that, but you know, we for

11:26

no other reason alone with the warmer weather, and

11:28

then you're gonna really see the blunting

11:30

despite the vaccine hesitancy that we're talking about.

11:32

You are going to see a significant blunting of

11:35

people who are getting very sick, people who are dying.

11:37

And we know that the the vaccine does

11:39

seem to have good evidence that it stops or decreases

11:42

transmission. So I think we're really going to

11:44

get to a pretty good point. I think you'll still

11:46

see masks around, you know, in

11:49

in Hong Kong after Hong Kong

11:51

really wasn't a mask wearing country until after

11:53

Stars and then there was this huge psychological

11:56

impact. That's why they went to mass

11:58

so early in Hong Kong, but they became a mask

12:01

wearing culture. I think you will

12:03

see people who are just frightened still want to

12:05

wear masks in public places. I think

12:07

in flu season, you know, the colder months,

12:09

I think you'll see more masks. I think that may

12:11

become a a larger part

12:13

of our culture. Not a dominant, but I think

12:15

a larger part of our culture. That's interesting

12:18

because I remember being in Tokyo maybe

12:20

gosh, gosh, maybe ten

12:23

ten or twelve years ago and going on

12:25

the train to Kyoto and seeing

12:27

everyone wearing masks and thinking this

12:30

is so weird. Why are they wearing masks?

12:32

And now, of course I understand,

12:35

and you're right. I think when people are on places

12:37

like public transportation, if they're

12:39

in a closed spaces with lots of

12:42

strangers, Um, it actually

12:44

makes sense, doesn't it. I mean,

12:47

you know, one thing we saw, as you may remember,

12:49

is that the flu numbers were way

12:51

down this past season,

12:54

and that you know that that wasn't because of any

12:56

increase in in vaccination or anything.

12:58

That was because of just public health behavior. It's

13:00

always worked, you know. I don't

13:02

know there's a metaphor for this, Katie, right, I

13:04

mean we I don't know. I guess

13:06

it's true in our lives, like we'd rather just take a

13:08

pill for weight loss rather than go exercise,

13:11

and we always want the convenience. And

13:13

and this is this is another

13:15

example of that. We I

13:18

I was struck. And again, we can talk about

13:20

COVID all day long, but the but the I'll

13:23

never forget these these mask researchers from

13:25

Harvard, a guy named Abera Kuran. He

13:27

basically was doing all this modeling all along.

13:29

We were talking to him. He told me that if

13:31

for four weeks, and this is back, you know, October

13:34

November of last year, if for four weeks

13:37

everybody just wore a high filtration

13:39

mask when they went out in public, that's

13:41

it. For four weeks, if everybody did that,

13:44

it would have ended the pandemic. Are you kidding?

13:46

That's incredible. The virus

13:49

would have nowhere to go. It couldn't

13:51

find a willing host. I

13:53

mean, you know that that that sort

13:56

of that sort of concept is something that's

13:58

more than a hundred years old. I

14:01

just I just don't quite you know, I don't

14:03

know. Maybe I'm just being naive, But but you

14:05

hear that and you think it's amazing, right, I think it's

14:07

amazing. And yet we also

14:09

know both of us that in the United

14:12

States that couldn't happen. It just wouldn't

14:14

happen. I mean, human being

14:16

survived and thrived as a species

14:18

because we're reciprocally altruistic.

14:21

There's a reason that it feels good to do good.

14:23

Why should it feel good when I do something

14:25

nice for you? I mean, what purposes that serve my

14:27

evolutionary tree? I don't know, But the

14:29

reality is that it does feel good to do good. We

14:32

encoded that in some way in our DNA

14:35

and then people can't be bothered to wear a mask

14:37

to save tens of thousands of lives. I

14:39

just I'll that may be

14:41

one of the greatest mysteries of all out of

14:43

this whole thing. Well, I just

14:45

want to say, on on behalf of the

14:48

American public, thank you for your

14:50

coverage of this UM.

14:52

I think you're so measured and

14:55

uh so eloquent and

14:58

and honestly calming in

15:00

a way, and I just really

15:03

appreciate all the fantastic

15:05

reporting you've done throughout this pandemic.

15:07

So on behalf of

15:09

a grateful America, I would like to

15:11

say thank you, Sancho Gupta, Well,

15:14

Katie, thank you, thank you, And

15:17

that obviously means a great

15:19

deal in particular coming from you, So I

15:21

appreciate that you know, you get

15:23

it. You know, I mean, you're you are

15:25

the standard obviously by whom we all

15:27

measure ourselves. But also you know

15:29

you were all in these black holes, right

15:32

I mean, I don't know where you are right now. As I

15:34

said, I'm in this tiny little closet. I don't you

15:36

don't get any feedback. Sometimes it's

15:38

been really dispiriting because you

15:41

think, Okay, I'm a medical reporter in

15:43

the middle of a pandemic, that that is my you know,

15:45

that's a job. And

15:47

at the same time, the country in which I'm reporting

15:50

arguably did the worst in the world. I

15:52

mean, I know I keep taking this in that direction,

15:54

but it's just so dispiriting. Did I

15:57

did anyone listen to me? I mean,

15:59

if if, if you're the medical reporter and

16:01

presumably people are are counting on you

16:03

to provide knowledge hopefully that

16:06

will inform how they behave and

16:08

then we do the worst in the world. That's

16:10

that's you know, I'm going to need to reflect

16:12

on that. I think, you know, in

16:15

the years to come, like what is the real

16:17

impact here? One could argue that maybe it

16:19

would have been worse, who knows, you know, but

16:21

it's pretty bad. Well, don't

16:23

get too dispirited, because I think a

16:25

lot of people listen, relied on you,

16:28

and actually acted. So

16:31

even though the track record was bad, your

16:34

information was good and important.

16:36

So thank you. I appreciate that.

16:40

When we come back Sanja and I find some

16:42

optimism and believe it or

16:45

not, brain health, that's right after

16:47

this, let's

16:58

talk about keep sharp because moving

17:01

forward, I think many

17:03

people like me really

17:05

are interested in how to keep our

17:08

cognitive and mental health

17:11

at the top. And I think you

17:14

know, certainly, one big

17:16

change in medicine is that we

17:20

as patients are not passive.

17:23

It turns out, Sang, that there is a

17:25

lot of things that we can do to keep

17:28

our brains in shape, just like

17:30

we can do to keep our bodies

17:32

and our organs and other things

17:34

in shape. And that's why you

17:36

broke keep Sharp. But you have a very

17:39

very personal connection to

17:42

this, I guess, well, obviously because

17:44

of your specialty, but particularly

17:47

about Alzheimer's dementia and

17:51

are failing brain power that

17:53

happens as we age. Tell me about

17:56

that. Well, when I was

17:58

when I was twelve thirteen

18:00

years old, my grandfather, my

18:02

my mother's dad, who I was very

18:05

close to, um developed,

18:08

you know, signs of dementia.

18:10

He had had a a stroke earlier in

18:13

his life that had recovered and

18:15

was now developing. You know, just

18:18

these these um

18:20

periods of time where he he really wasn't aware

18:22

of what was going on. He would sometimes,

18:25

uh make a joke that no one

18:27

else was in on, you know, and it was all these things

18:29

that that I remember really being struck by

18:31

as a kid, because you look at adults and you're

18:33

not used to seeing brain power start

18:35

to diminish. And it was the first time I saw really

18:38

specific things like he could he could

18:40

still um right, but

18:42

he couldn't really read. It was it was all

18:44

these things that became really fascinating

18:47

in a way for me in terms of just how does the brain

18:49

work like that? But also to see

18:51

it in a loved one to wonder, is

18:54

that how genetic is that? Is? That? Is

18:56

my mom going to develop those symptoms? While I one

18:58

day all of that and then you know, fast

19:00

forward, you know, thirty forty years later, and

19:03

and we're still worrying

19:05

about the exact same things, and

19:08

and I haven't really made a lot of progress in terms

19:10

of being able to deal with that. So that was

19:12

that was a large part of what I think inspired

19:15

me to to to write the book What

19:17

has Happened over the last forty years?

19:20

Both from a, you know, a pharmaceutical

19:23

standpoint, but also more importantly, I think from

19:25

a from a lifestyle behavioral standpoint

19:27

in terms of what we know and how

19:29

did that influence you to go into neurology?

19:32

No, you know it's funny, Um, it

19:35

didn't. I actually when I started medical medical

19:37

school, I thought I was going to go into pediatrics,

19:39

and then I did a neurosurgery rotation,

19:42

uh during my third year of med school, and I just sort of

19:44

fell in love. So I came to it quite late. But

19:48

I was always interested in the brain because of my

19:50

grandfather, and so it felt like a very natural

19:52

fit. Let's talk about

19:55

the numbers. Because seven

19:57

million Americans have some evidence

19:59

of pre clinical Alzheimer's disease,

20:02

and by two sixty one

20:04

new case of dementia will be diagnosed

20:06

every four seconds. SANJ,

20:10

what the heck is going on here? Well,

20:12

this is this will become the most

20:14

dominant neurodegenerative disease

20:17

of our time. I think that

20:19

that part, I think is is pretty

20:22

well established at this point. But there

20:24

was two things about the statement that you just made

20:27

that I thought were really important

20:29

in terms of what we can potentially do about it.

20:32

As you point out, there's probably about forty seven million

20:34

people who if you were to look at their brains, they

20:36

would have objective evidence of plaques

20:39

and tangles and things like that, but

20:41

also have no symptoms. That's

20:43

the pre clinical time, right, so

20:46

out a little well,

20:48

it's you know, but I think this is ultimately

20:51

good news, and I'll tell you why. If you

20:53

look at patients with Alzheimer's disease

20:55

and and and able to retrospectively

20:58

look at their lives and their scans and their brains,

21:00

you find we now know that that Alzheimer's

21:03

starts in the brain decades before

21:06

people develop symptoms. Decades so

21:08

you're starting to see the kindling and then

21:10

even plaques and tangles. But the

21:12

fundamental point that neuroscientists really

21:14

started to focus on was almost

21:17

the the the analog of that,

21:19

meaning, Okay, so now you've established

21:22

that you can have a brain that has plaques and tangles

21:24

but still functions normally. So

21:26

why don't we focus on that side

21:28

of things instead of saying, hey, look, let's

21:31

get rid of the plaques and tangles, and we have spent

21:33

billions of dollars testing drugs to do

21:35

that that haven't really worked. What

21:38

if we say, instead, we have established

21:40

that a brain with plaques and tangles can function

21:42

normally. Let's figure out why

21:44

and see if we can basically

21:46

make that an aspiration. Do

21:49

you still have objective

21:51

evidence of Alzheimer's in your brain. Yes?

21:53

Is it consequential? No, because

21:56

you know you you are able to still have

21:59

normal cognitive function, memory, judgment,

22:01

all the things that you associate with a healthy functioning

22:03

brain. The metaphor, in some ways,

22:05

Katie would kind of be like a heart bypass surgery.

22:08

You've got a block blood vessel. Now

22:11

you you go in there and you bypass

22:13

that area of the blockage with a new blood

22:15

vessel. Do you still have heart disease? Yes?

22:19

Is it? Is it? Is it causing

22:21

you some dysfunction? No, because you're getting enough

22:23

blood flow now to the heart. If you can think

22:25

about that same metaphor

22:27

for the brain, Yes, you have plaques,

22:30

but there's so many ways to build all these

22:32

new pathways in the brain to your

22:34

destination, that little blockages

22:37

due to the plaques becoming consequential.

22:40

So is there a tipping point? You know you talk

22:42

about these tangles and plaques in

22:44

your brain. Um, is

22:46

it just a slow growth

22:50

of plaque or slow accumulation

22:52

of plaque and increase kind of

22:55

tangles that then lead

22:57

you from being perfectly functioning?

23:00

You know, maybe some memory issues right

23:02

that once in a while, you you know you're not

23:04

quite as sharp as you were as you when

23:06

you were younger. But where you

23:08

kind of fall off or it's just gradual

23:11

build up of this gunk in your brain, it's

23:14

it's it does seem to be a pretty gradual

23:17

build up, and you can tolerate

23:19

a significant amount of build up

23:21

before you I guess as you as you say, fall

23:23

off. You know, so what exactly

23:25

then pushes people over?

23:27

It's not all of a sudden you have an exponentially

23:30

more plaque, and that leads to the problem.

23:32

Maybe for different people it's a different inflection

23:34

point. But the brain is

23:36

actually quite resilient. I mean, that's the thing

23:39

that came out of this. Even with a lot of plaque and

23:41

tangle, you could actually

23:43

be doing fairly well. I mean the occasional

23:45

memory lapse, like you say, which is probably do more

23:48

to inattention than even anything organic

23:50

in the brain, but other than that doing

23:52

pretty well. And you see societies around

23:54

the world where arguably brain

23:57

function not only

24:00

is it good, it may be improving as

24:02

you get older, which is so incredible,

24:05

and that I think is one of the hopeful things

24:07

about this book, that that our

24:09

brains can get sharper and better as

24:11

we age and dementia is not necessarily

24:15

an inevitable, you know, consequence

24:17

of old age. So um,

24:19

you know, I remember reading Sanjay

24:22

how your brain like by

24:24

the time you're twenty three or twenty

24:26

four and then your prefrontal frontal

24:28

lobe and all this thing that has to stuff

24:31

that has to do with judgment, Like after

24:33

that, your brain really stops

24:35

absorbing and growing

24:38

and changing. I mean that was sort of

24:40

what I always thought, and then it was downhill

24:42

from there. But but this

24:44

book is really

24:47

cause for celebration in some ways, right,

24:50

right, absolutely, you know I

24:52

was told the same thing. Right, You've got a certain number

24:55

of neurons in your brain, and then

24:57

you're going to drain the cash as you go through

24:59

life. Certain things is like drinking alcohol and

25:01

things like that are gonna kill more brain cells. You're never going

25:03

to get them back. I think that's what our parents

25:05

told us to keep us front drink. It

25:08

works well to some extent, but the

25:10

the you know it, but that

25:12

that part of it is not true, and

25:15

that that may be one of the most fundamental new

25:18

things that we learned. And by the

25:20

way, you'll appreciate this, Katie, I'm

25:22

in some ways this book, I'm acting

25:24

as translator. I go to

25:26

these neuroscience meetings because that you

25:28

know, I live this bifurcated life between

25:31

medicine and media. But I'm still going to these neuroscience

25:33

meetings and they're talking about these fascinating

25:35

developments, and yet that hasn't really

25:37

gotten to the to the lay public yet.

25:40

So it's about a ten year gap in

25:42

some ways. Keep Sharp is to just accelerate

25:44

that that knowledge tree. But one of

25:46

the things that they've been talking about is exactly what

25:48

you mentioned, which is neurogenesis.

25:51

Everyone's heard of neuroplasticity, which

25:53

basically means you can recruit neurons,

25:56

brain cells from other areas of the brain to do new

25:58

functions. This is actually

26:00

growing new brain cells, and

26:02

we were told throughout our lives that it basically happened

26:05

twice. You know, when you were a baby in

26:07

your brain was still forming, and maybe

26:09

after an injury like a stroke or traumatic

26:12

brain injury, there may be a process of neurogenesis

26:14

that occurs. But what these

26:16

these neuroscientists have have really

26:19

I've been writing about and focused on for some time

26:21

is that at any age, a healthy

26:23

brain can continue to grow new

26:26

brain cells. You really can't say that

26:28

about any other organ in the body. So

26:30

it's quite incredible. The stem

26:32

cell surges, the various growth factors,

26:34

all these things that converge to

26:36

allow you to grow new brain cells at

26:39

any age. That to me was

26:41

deeply inspiring. It's super exciting.

26:44

But let me dis backtrack for one

26:46

moment, just so I understand the difference

26:48

between neuroplasticity and neurogenesis.

26:52

Um, explain it like I'm a fifth

26:54

grade Okay, So you

26:56

know, when you think of neuroplasticity, it's more like your

26:59

brain is is like plastic it's it's can

27:01

be molded. So let's say there's

27:03

been an area of your brain where someone

27:05

had an injury or a stroke or something. You could sort

27:07

of mold another part of the brain

27:09

to to fill the game, compensate, compensate,

27:12

Yeah, exactly. You know. Take so if it

27:14

was motor strength, for example, on the right

27:16

side of your body that was affected, cells,

27:20

brain cells that normally don't do motor

27:22

function, they're not responsible for motor function, could

27:24

be recruited to do that sort of work.

27:26

Or a sense. You know, even if you lose a

27:28

sense, other senses can start to become

27:31

heightened or even create, right.

27:33

You hear that with blind people, you know, in

27:36

terms of a heightened uh

27:38

sense of I guess all kinds of

27:40

senses, right, that compensate for the fact

27:42

that you can't see exactly and

27:45

that and that is a that is a It's an

27:47

amazing concept. It's what sort of gives

27:49

real birth to physical therapy,

27:51

to cognitive therapy. We're using

27:54

these therapies to basically recruit neurons

27:56

from other parts of your brain to do something. The

27:59

human is so amazing,

28:01

isn't it. It continues

28:03

to wonder and delight me every day.

28:06

And I've been thinking about this for forty

28:08

years, you know, I love it. Um

28:11

Neurogenesis is the growth of

28:13

new brain cells. So this

28:16

this, this, this flies in the face of

28:18

what we were all told when we were young, that you only have

28:20

a certain number of brain cells and that's it. This

28:22

is basically saying you can create new brain cells

28:24

at any age. The metaphor I think that may make

28:26

it more more accessible,

28:29

is right

28:31

now, our COVID life

28:34

is kind of like how our brain operates. And what

28:36

I mean is that you probably are at home.

28:38

You may drive to the grocery store. Maybe

28:40

you drive to a couple of different places and

28:43

you but you're not You're you're mostly in in

28:45

just a small, small square sort

28:47

of area of place. You know

28:49

how to get to all those places really well. You could drive

28:51

there with your eyes closed, no problem.

28:54

But but you you're not traveling

28:56

around the rest of the world. You're not even traveling

28:58

around the rest of the state right now. That's

29:01

kind of how our brains are. We use our whole brain, but

29:05

the time we're using our brains.

29:08

That's the thing. If you start to actually

29:10

do things to inspire neurogenesis

29:12

in your brain, it's kind of like building new cities and

29:14

visiting those new cities in your brain. It's

29:17

it's it's a it's a little bit of a simplistic metaphor,

29:19

but it makes me really happy to think about because

29:22

visiting other places in your brain is an incredibly

29:25

joyous thing. You start to see

29:27

patterns that you would have otherwise missed. You

29:29

connect dots, you're thinking is clearer,

29:33

and and and that's the whole concept of

29:35

of of what neurogenesis can do for

29:37

you. When we come back Sanche's

29:40

advice on how to keep our brains

29:42

strong, especially as

29:45

we age.

29:57

We'll take me on a trip, do

30:00

and how can I visit these

30:02

new places in my brain? Because

30:04

this is really exciting And

30:06

um, I know your book has a lot

30:09

of recommendations for ways

30:11

that we can encourage neurochantesis.

30:14

So what do I do? Son J? Yeah,

30:17

so you know. The way I'll tell you the way

30:19

that I wrote the book was I took

30:21

all these these neuroscience concepts and try to make

30:23

them accessible and also help you set

30:25

up a substrate for your brain in terms

30:28

of, you know, how you nourish yourself, how you

30:30

rest your brain, things like that. So the basics

30:32

are there, but your question is more about

30:34

taking the trip and building the new brain cells.

30:37

So after you sort of you know, make sure and

30:39

it's not challenging to get to the right sort

30:42

of place in terms of your diet, you

30:44

know, uh, and and the amount of rest

30:46

that you need. That's important. But

30:49

the biggest I think difference with

30:51

growing new brain cells versus how we typically

30:53

think about strengthening our brain is

30:56

that you don't necessarily want

30:58

to just keep doing the same things

31:00

over and over again. The whole practice makes

31:02

perfect sort of teaching the

31:05

killing drill sort of teaching that a lot

31:07

of schools focus on. It's important

31:09

to to understand and and be

31:11

able to learn concepts. But

31:14

that's kind of like those roads I was talking about

31:16

that you travel so well, that's like getting even

31:18

better at traveling those same roads. Now

31:20

you can really do with your eyes closed. Now you know

31:22

it's it's it's totally second nature

31:24

to you. But

31:27

if you were to do different things,

31:30

totally different things, things that get you out of your

31:32

comfort zone a little bit, a

31:34

totally different sort of hobby, that's

31:37

when you're starting to actually build

31:39

some of these new brain cells, create some of these new cities,

31:42

create some of the new roads, whatever,

31:44

whatever metaphor you want to apply to it. That's

31:48

that's a much better way

31:50

to sort of do that versus

31:52

the practice makes perfect. So if practice

31:55

makes perfect, change is what's going

31:57

to build the neurogenesis. It's

31:59

going to build the resili and redundancy

32:01

in your brain. So I played the

32:03

piano. Should I not

32:05

focus as much on the piano because I thought

32:08

about taking lessons even though

32:10

I took for ten years and I played by ear,

32:12

But I enjoyed the piano, And we actually

32:14

have a beautiful piano that

32:16

Jay and I bought each other for our birthdays

32:19

back in the day. Yeah, and but

32:21

but should I learn how to play the guitar or

32:23

the viol The violence sounds just horrible

32:26

if you're not good at it. But what do you

32:28

mean, should I try a new instrument? Yeah?

32:30

You know, so I asked a lot of neuroscientists

32:33

about this, because one thing about writing a book like this

32:35

is that it affects everybody, right, So even

32:37

the guys and gals who are who are

32:39

doing all this research, they're thinking about what to

32:42

incorporate into their own lives. And there are

32:44

a couple of things that sort of jumped out at me. One

32:46

is that something new is I think really

32:48

important. That's that's that that is

32:50

a key, But something that you can

32:52

also use your hands with

32:54

that you're actually activating your motor motor cortex

32:57

as you're doing, seems to be even more beneficial.

32:59

So so an instrument is great, um

33:02

painting, Try a new

33:05

one because a new one? Yes, I mean I mean

33:07

the piano again. I

33:10

want to be careful here. I did this Bill

33:12

Clinton. I was talking to him about brain

33:14

health the other day and he got on my case

33:16

because he said he loves crossword puzzles, and he's

33:18

like, so you tell me crossword puzzles are not good for No,

33:21

No, I'm not saying don't do those things.

33:24

But understand what you're accomplishing. You

33:26

are you are. You're paving those roads

33:28

really really well on your brain, and that is great, there's

33:30

great value in that. But if

33:32

it is true that you can build all these new roads,

33:35

and the question you're asking me is how to do that, then

33:37

it would be it would mean doing something different.

33:40

So I'm not saying stop playing the piano,

33:43

keep driving those roads, but if you want

33:46

to start going on these trips around

33:48

your brain, doing something different,

33:50

and preferably doing it in a way

33:52

that maybe even a little uncomfortable. So

33:55

if you're painting, and I just bring up painting, because

33:57

this is the one that came up set

34:00

all times among these neuroscientists. Learn

34:02

how to paint. I'm a terrible artist. Learn how to paint,

34:05

do whatever you can, and do it with your non dominant

34:07

hand. Yes,

34:10

this was another. In fact, they went so far

34:12

as to say that tonight at dinner, when you're eating

34:14

your dinner, try eating your meal with

34:16

your non dominant hand. And just see

34:19

what happens. And it's really interesting,

34:21

Katie, because we think of building the brain means

34:23

reading books and gaining new knowledge,

34:26

and that's true, but in terms of actually

34:28

creating neurogenesis, it's more like you think

34:30

about a physical workout. I'm gonna do something

34:32

different and I'm going to actually

34:35

now focus the less

34:37

side of my brain, which normally isn't

34:39

doing motor function. That's delicate or

34:41

fine. On actually doing that sort of stuff,

34:44

it has real relevance because again, you're

34:46

you're actually building these roads in these cities in

34:48

your brain, and that's fun. Try

34:50

it. It's fun. But on a more

34:52

practical level. To your original question, Let's

34:55

say one day the road

34:57

that you drive so well becomes

35:00

locked by one of these amyloid plaques

35:02

that we're talking about, some of these tangles. Right

35:05

now, you know that road really well, but you

35:07

know what, you don't really have other roads to

35:09

get from point A to point B. If

35:11

you've been building all these roads by

35:14

painting with your left hand and spilling your food,

35:16

but by eating with your non dominant hand, whatever

35:18

it might be, you're actually building roads.

35:21

This gets back to the bypass analogy.

35:23

Do you still have flacks and tangles in your brain.

35:26

Yes, So are these the cognitive

35:28

reserves that you're talking about, Yes,

35:31

the cognitive reserves, the cognitive

35:33

resiliency, which is often they often

35:35

use these terms interchangeably, but

35:37

that's exactly it. We have the capacity

35:40

to to have significant cognitive

35:42

reserve. We're barely tapping into

35:44

that. If you look at societies around the world

35:47

where people are living into their nineties and hundreds

35:49

and have hardly any dementia.

35:52

The presumption now is that if you were to image

35:54

their brains, they might have plaques and

35:56

tangles. If you're doing autopsy,

35:58

they may be diagnosed with all Heimers, because

36:00

that's how Alzheimer's was diagnosed, was that autopsy.

36:03

But the truth of the matter is that during their lives

36:06

they had perfectly normal cognitive function. Before

36:08

we talk about your twelve week program, I'm

36:10

just curious in terms of diagnostic

36:13

advances and and therapeutic

36:16

advances. I mean, will

36:18

we get to a point where someone can have

36:20

a brain scan and say, Okay, here's

36:22

the status of your tangles and plaques,

36:24

and here's what you need to do. Because brain

36:27

imagery, you know, I've ways found it so

36:30

interesting even when you talk about like antidepressants

36:33

and you know, serotonin reuptake

36:35

inhibitors or whatever they're called ss

36:37

is. Yeah,

36:40

that that you know, there was never a

36:42

way until recently to kind of measure

36:45

how the brain was reacting. They would just

36:47

kind of it would be very anecdotal,

36:49

you kind of throw it against the wall to see what sticks.

36:52

And now we have so much better

36:54

brain imagery. So will

36:56

that translate into dimension

36:59

all time person and preventative

37:01

strategies that we could follow. I

37:03

think, I think so. I mean, we're we're making a lot

37:05

of progress on brain imaging, and

37:07

you're absolutely right. I mean, the brain has long been sort

37:10

of considered this black box only

37:12

measured by its inputs and its outputs. You

37:14

really couldn't get a good

37:17

idea of its internal machinery. But

37:19

now we can. I mean, I

37:21

don't know that we're at the point yet where we can determine

37:23

degree of severity of

37:25

of dementia based on a scan, and

37:28

as I think we will. We think we'll get to that

37:30

point. I think we'll

37:32

get to the point where we can very quantifiably

37:35

measure the burden

37:37

of plaques and tangles and other things in the brain.

37:40

But well, but Again, what I think is so extraordinary,

37:43

Katie, is that you could have two people

37:45

with the exact same scan essentially and

37:48

very different clinical pictures.

37:50

One person may be completely debilitated, obviously

37:53

having dementia, and the other person may be functionally

37:56

cognitively normal. And again,

37:59

I look, do

38:01

I don't want to I don't want to have flax

38:03

and tangles in my brain. But mostly what I don't

38:05

want to have is the cognitive dysfunction

38:08

that comes with that. It's a different way

38:10

of thinking. It really is, like I think,

38:12

again, we focus so much on making someone's images

38:15

look better or whatever, and what

38:17

the person really wants is them to be

38:19

better, and there are ways to do that,

38:21

you know, with lifestyle changes. I mean, I'm

38:23

a neurosurgeon saying this, by the way, just remember

38:26

that because I'm a specialist. That's that's what I

38:28

was trying to do. And yet I'm now becoming

38:31

increasingly convinced that these types

38:33

of changes that we talked about in this book

38:35

really can can prevent you

38:37

from developing the symptoms

38:40

I was going to say. So you're saying that brain scans

38:42

are just part of the story. It's sort of

38:44

like it's half of the story

38:46

because even with them, you could

38:49

have these cognitive reserves

38:52

developed and be very

38:54

asymptomatic. Well, let's talk about

38:56

this twelve week program. Sharp

38:59

take us through this deeps because I'm all

39:01

ears. It's it's twelve

39:03

weeks where I basically based

39:06

on how I think your brain is going to change

39:09

and and react to things that you're now

39:11

doing that are different or new. Um,

39:14

it all sort of builds on itself. I start

39:16

off by really making sure you get the basics

39:18

right, and I'll tell you it's it's not

39:21

that complicated. There are a few big

39:23

messages in terms of the overall getting

39:26

it the substrate right, and as

39:28

you might guess, diet nourishment

39:30

is one of them. But but the big the

39:32

big takeaway here is I think generally people

39:34

know what a healthy diet is, and

39:37

for those who don't, there's some information in there about

39:39

what's specifically healthy for the brain. There

39:41

are some distinctions between the brain and the

39:43

body in this regard. One is one

39:45

is sugar. Um. You know, we we

39:47

we talk a lot about sugar, and people know that they shouldn't

39:50

need too much sugar. We used to get sugar,

39:52

you know, twice a year when fruit fell from

39:54

the trees. Even honey was protected

39:56

by the bees. And now we're eating a hundred

39:58

and thirty pounds a year on average of sugar. But

40:01

what was what was a learning point for me

40:04

was that the brain is exquisitely sensitive

40:06

to sugar. So typically you eat a

40:08

lot of sugar and you think, well, that's being

40:11

absorbed into cells. I have a lot of energy, whatever

40:13

it might be. These these are these are a lot of calories

40:15

that are now providing the energy the brain.

40:17

As soon as sugar levels get beyond a

40:19

certain point, and it's a pretty narrow range, the

40:22

receptors basically shut down. So

40:25

you could run into a situation where you're taking

40:27

in a lot of calories, a lot of energy,

40:30

and starving your brain at the same time. And

40:33

that that is a situation that leads

40:35

to a whole a whole cascade

40:37

of events that you can pretty easily

40:39

avoid. So that's you know, as

40:42

much as I talk about in the first few

40:44

weeks of what to do, there

40:46

are several things that you're

40:48

told not to do just to avoid and

40:50

that's that's more than half the battle,

40:53

and they're not that hard to do. I also

40:55

try to make the case for things like sleep,

40:57

which you've read a lot about I've read a

40:59

lot about, but reminding people just

41:01

how metabolically active the brain is

41:04

during sleep, and this wonderful conversation

41:06

that I'm having with you right now will be encoded

41:08

into my hippocampus if I get good sleep

41:10

tonight, so that twenty years from now I can

41:13

recall this and remember it. A lot

41:15

of times people say that they can't remember

41:17

something. It's not that they can't remember that, it's

41:19

not that they forgot it, it's that they never

41:21

actually stored it in their memory centers

41:23

in the first place. So these

41:26

are strategies to help that. But

41:28

then, you know, sort of the midpoint of the book is

41:30

really about and the

41:32

the evidence based things that we know improve

41:34

brain health. Starts off by asking

41:37

you to define what you think a healthy brain is.

41:39

What is a healthy brain? We know what a healthy

41:42

heart is, it pumps a certain amount of blood

41:44

out with each beat. What is a healthy brain?

41:46

You know? And and I spend a little bit of time

41:49

talking through talking

41:51

the reader through how they define that, because

41:53

it is different for different people. Robert

41:56

Zapolski, who is this evolutionary biologist,

41:59

I was interviewing him, and he and her forget. He

42:01

said to me that a healthy brain is a

42:03

is a is a brain that has a bigger circle

42:06

of you, is what he said,

42:08

which basically means you let more people into

42:10

your circle. Now why is that relevant. Well,

42:12

it's relevant in you know, ancient

42:15

times because you were more likely to be protected

42:17

by the group. But now it's this idea

42:19

of what true connection

42:22

does for for protection

42:24

of the brain. And and and now to

42:26

your earlier point, measurable. You

42:28

know, a lot of what we talked about is based

42:31

on objective data that we couldn't collect some

42:34

time ago. But I will tell you something fascinating

42:36

because I find this this topic really interesting.

42:38

But there's this loneliness researcher named Stephanie

42:41

Cacciope. She's an oregan. I know. I

42:44

at the in Chicago. Her husband

42:46

was one of the pre eminent scientists

42:49

about memory, and then he died, died,

42:51

I know. And now she's an oregan. And I talked

42:54

to her from time to time. It's

42:56

been tough, as you might imagine. And she's by herself

42:58

an Oregon, which you know, has a loneliness. Researchers

43:00

through this pandemic has been such a

43:03

significant thing for her. But she said

43:05

this thing to me that I'll never forget, and

43:07

and and I talk a little bit about it in the book, which

43:09

is we talk about connection, like right now,

43:12

you and I get to zoom and have this call about

43:14

you know this, this conversation that's very

43:16

interesting to me. Most connections

43:19

that we have with friends, maybe even

43:21

family to some extent, are pretty cursory.

43:24

How you doing, I'm doing fine. How you doing, I'm

43:26

doing fine. You know. It's it's how

43:28

do you get to a level of more profound

43:30

connection, because it wasn't.

43:32

As you've well heard, it's not about the number of connections

43:35

you have. It's about the quality. But what does that

43:37

mean quality? And one thing Stephanie

43:39

said to me was a

43:42

sort of shortcut to building the

43:44

quality and the high intensity connection

43:47

is to be vulnerable, to ask

43:49

for help, to share your problems,

43:52

which is totally counterintuitive to how

43:54

I think about things. I would rather not burden somebody

43:57

with things. But I took it

43:59

to heart. And I was talking my parents, who are in their late

44:01

seventies in Florida through this pandemic,

44:03

and we were having those conversations how you

44:05

do and how the girls. That was the conversation

44:07

for months, and I said to them, I

44:09

asked them a question about a problem

44:11

I was having one of my cars that my

44:13

wife's car had some smoke coming from the

44:15

hood. They're both engineers, and

44:18

for days, Katie, we started to have these

44:20

really interesting conversations about cars,

44:22

about their history of being interested in engineering,

44:25

in all this stuff, figure out

44:27

the way to build the meaningful

44:29

connection um that

44:32

that is. That is probably one of the most critical points,

44:34

and and they're there pretty easy

44:36

ways to do it. I think the point

44:38

is connection, deep connection

44:41

is good for your brain, yes, and good

44:43

for you, and I think you know it's good

44:46

for you in general. So the last

44:48

part of the twelve week program, I'll just tell you quickly,

44:50

is more about what we started talking about initially,

44:53

which is, then, how do you create Now

44:55

that I've primed your brain for neurogenesis,

44:58

giving you all the right amounts of the right

45:00

hormones, not too much epinephrin, but

45:02

enough oxytocin, and all that sort

45:05

of is happening by going through the first

45:07

few weeks of the program, Now how do you build

45:09

the new brain cells? And that gets to

45:11

a lot of what we're talking about in terms of that cognitive

45:13

reserve. You know, actually, um

45:16

uh, doing these different types of activities,

45:19

doing similar activities in a totally

45:21

different way, doing things with different

45:23

people, doing them in at different times, eliminating

45:26

certain things completely from your regiment

45:28

for a while, adding in something totally unrelated.

45:31

It's it was fascinating to me. I

45:33

tried it. I based this entire

45:36

thing on my conversations with these neuroscientists

45:38

who all tried it and written

45:40

about it and published it in journals. It's

45:42

fun, It's a fun ride. I was gonna

45:44

say, so, give me some ideas real

45:47

quickly before we go about things I could

45:49

do. Should I take a pottery class, Should

45:51

I learn Italian? Should I pick

45:53

up the guitar? What should I do? I

45:55

think that, you know, I

45:58

think the two big ingredients are it's would

46:00

be something you really haven't done before. This isn't

46:02

about trying to again build

46:04

a two lane highway where you're used to driving one.

46:06

This is about getting to you know, to

46:10

Italy instead of staying in New

46:12

York or or going somewhere even different in

46:14

Argentina, you know, totally different.

46:16

If you can do something that involves your

46:19

your your hands, like pottery or painting.

46:21

Even better, that was something that came up over

46:24

and over again. And then the second

46:26

ingredient, I guess, and this is a little bit

46:28

more vague, is that it's

46:30

it's good if it makes you a little uncomfortable.

46:33

I And I know that sounds almost euphemistic

46:35

or too easy or too simple, but the

46:38

whole point is that when you start

46:40

to release certain hormones

46:42

in the body, like some stress

46:45

hormone, stress can be good. It

46:47

really helps that process of neurogenesis.

46:49

So a little bit of discomfort with something totally

46:51

new, preferably using your hands, that's

46:53

a pretty good prescription. Before

46:56

we go, can you tell me about

46:59

foods that are healthy brain foods?

47:01

I know that you hear about fish, you

47:03

hear about nuts, you hear

47:05

about extra virgin olive oil. Are

47:08

all those things sort of good brain food?

47:10

And what else should I be eating other than staying

47:13

away from the cupcakes? Yeah? Nor

47:15

Yes, definitely the sugar thing I mentioned

47:18

already, So I mean that's just that's just a I

47:20

think you could accomplish sev of

47:23

all the other things by basically just

47:26

eliminating added sugar from your diet. But

47:28

I think the adage what

47:31

is good for the heart is good for the brain remains

47:33

true. But I think with the brain there

47:35

are a few a few distinctions.

47:37

One is, if

47:39

an apple a day keeps a doctor away, then berries

47:42

are what's good for the brain. Berries

47:45

really good data around berries. Really start to add

47:47

berries into your diet. I think that's

47:49

one of the big ones. And while most

47:52

of the neuroscientists did not advocate a caloric

47:54

restriction diet, necessarily a

47:56

calorie reduced diet overall to

47:58

the extent that you can do it. We create a

48:00

lot of metabolic byproducts from

48:03

from overeating, and a lot of those

48:05

metabolic byproducts get accumulated

48:07

in the brain. So if you

48:10

can cut down on the amount of energy

48:13

that has to be metabolized in that way, you

48:15

can make a lot of progress. Even though

48:17

berries may be good for your brain.

48:19

You don't believe in this whole idea of supplements

48:22

or superfoods, do you, now?

48:24

I you know. I think super food first of all,

48:26

is a really vaguely defined term. As

48:29

part of this book, I asked a lot of people, and I even

48:31

talked to your friend Mark Hyman about this as well.

48:33

It's it doesn't it doesn't have a really

48:36

objective meaning. There are some foods

48:38

that are maybe better than others. But I

48:40

think the thing about supplements that struck me

48:43

was was the idea that for

48:45

certain people who have deficiencies,

48:48

then supplementing that part of their diets important.

48:51

But you know, Katie, in this country, and

48:53

I'm not advocating this, but in this country, even

48:56

the standard American diet, like if

48:58

you go to a McDonald's, even the

49:00

food is largely fortified, you

49:03

know, with all these different vitamins and micronutrients

49:05

and things like that. That that is a decision that

49:07

our US d A made decades ago

49:10

to fortify food so that people

49:12

wouldn't develop basic nutritional

49:14

deficiencies. So oftentimes we're supplementing

49:16

something that doesn't need to be supplemented. A

49:19

lot of a lot of the approaches

49:22

more in terms of what you're not eating versus what you

49:24

are eating, and that that you know that

49:26

holds up to be true. So berries

49:29

I single out because they are one of these

49:31

foods whose active ingredients are particularly

49:33

good at crossing the blood brain barrier, particularly

49:36

good at creating these scaffoldings,

49:38

you know, for the neurogenesis

49:40

that we talked about earlier, So I

49:43

put that high on the list. But what

49:45

about you know, I see this stuff in the drug store

49:47

and I'm like, Oh, should I be taking like privig

49:50

in or should I be taking what is

49:52

it like? Almost isn't it like jellyfish

49:55

to run derivatives and stuff? And

49:57

I'm like, should I be doing that? Well?

50:00

You know, I the privileged one is interesting because

50:02

you know, Eric Kendell is very involved

50:04

with this, and he's a very prominent neuroscientists

50:07

did a lot of the original jellyfish research

50:10

basically trying to figure out where the memory

50:12

stores were in jellyfish, how jellyfish

50:15

remembered, and isolating those stores

50:17

and basically creating a supplement. It's

50:19

a fascinating idea. I

50:21

don't know that it really works. I mean, it's

50:23

very hard to study this sort of thing. You know, it takes

50:26

decades long studies to prove that something

50:28

like that's improving memory. What we

50:30

do have is is decades long data

50:32

on societies around the world where

50:35

dementia is essentially so rare that it's

50:37

reportable. You know, if somebody developed dementia,

50:39

you'd report that in the medical journal. But

50:41

my my point is though that with these

50:44

we don't need to have the supplements. We know

50:46

it's possible to be done because

50:48

we see it having already transpired

50:50

real time and large societies across

50:52

the world, and in

50:54

those societies, you know, I took

50:57

the neuroscientific data that we had and

51:00

tried to see, are are they in some

51:02

ways applying that unwittingly?

51:04

I mean they didn't read these papers obviously, but

51:07

were they sort of just by default

51:09

essentially following that that right

51:11

diet, following that right amount of movement, following

51:15

the right amount of rest. So movement, for example,

51:17

I'll just tell you this was an interesting one. If

51:19

you look at movement, it's probably

51:22

the only thing that has has

51:24

the longest amount of evidence behind

51:27

it in terms of actually creating neurogenesis.

51:29

All of this is new research, but that that's sort

51:31

of the oldest new research. But

51:33

what was fascinating to me was that

51:36

what does movement mean to people?

51:39

Right? I use the word movement instead of exercise,

51:42

because what they found was that moderate

51:45

movement, brisk walking that

51:47

tended to be a lot better for neurogenesis

51:50

than intense exercise. Now,

51:52

why would that be Well, it turns out

51:54

that when you briskly exercise,

51:57

you're releasing a lot of what is known as brain

52:00

derived neurotrophic factor. That's kind

52:02

of like the miracle grow for for your brain. As

52:04

was described, if you are intensely

52:06

exercising, you also tend to release a

52:08

lot of epinephrin and epinefrin

52:11

is actually a blocker. It's a it's a

52:13

cascade blocker of what b

52:15

d NF, this neurotrophic factor does.

52:18

I know, I'm throwing a lot of language at you, but I'm

52:21

FOLLI. Intense exercise may be great

52:23

for your heart and you know,

52:26

maybe even weight loss, whatever your goals.

52:28

Maybe, but for your brain, intense

52:31

exercise actually is not good

52:33

and you find that can actually be a little

52:35

bit destructive by releasing these stress

52:37

hormones that block the beneficial

52:40

effects that exercise should have on your brain.

52:42

I never knew that, and it

52:45

so like I think. I go for a walk as

52:47

often as I can with Rebecca. Now, that wasn't something

52:50

I did. I was out there thinking, I got forty

52:52

minutes, I'm gonna go hard. That was

52:54

my sort of approach, and sometimes

52:56

I still feel the need to do that. But walking

52:59

is great. Brisk here here,

53:01

here's the best way to do it. If you want to just make it for

53:03

your brain. Take a brisk walk would

53:06

a close friend or family member and

53:08

talk about your problems, and that

53:10

sort of brings all these things together in

53:13

some ways that we've been talking about. Take your take your

53:15

you're very smoothie with you, and you've pretty

53:17

much nailed it. I'm curious

53:20

about social media and the way we live

53:22

our lives. You know, we're constantly distracted,

53:25

we have constant incoming information,

53:27

our attention spans have shortened.

53:30

I read a fascinating study a while ago

53:33

that said the part of your brain I think

53:35

it's a hypocampus you can correct me if i'm

53:37

wrong, responsible for

53:39

for creativity. It only

53:42

fires up when you're bored.

53:44

And that's why you have so many great

53:46

ideas when you're in this shower, when

53:49

you're not distracted, or when you're taking

53:51

a long walk and you don't have your phone

53:53

with you. And I'm curious the impact

53:55

of all this mental stimulus

53:58

or stimuli has on

54:01

nerroa genesis and keeping

54:03

our brains healthy. You know, yeah,

54:05

okay, that that that's a It's a great topic and

54:07

I approach it as a person who wrote this book

54:09

frankly, also as a dad three

54:12

teenage girls, because and this is conversation

54:14

topic number one in our household all the time.

54:16

And I'll tell and I'll tell you two things that actually came

54:19

out of a dinner time conversation I recently had.

54:21

Um And I try not to be too

54:23

preachy with with my girls,

54:25

although sometimes I can say I don't use

54:27

this line often, but I can say I did write a book

54:30

about that. The girls hate it when

54:32

I do that, but it's true, and I can use

54:34

that as a wild card to actually get them to listen

54:36

to what I'm saying about the fact

54:38

that when you are distracted

54:41

like that and you think maybe even

54:43

you are multitasking, the

54:45

brain is actually not that good at multitasking.

54:48

It actually requires a lot of energy

54:51

to shift back and forth between things,

54:53

between scrolling through your social media

54:55

feed, trying to have a conversation, trying to

54:57

look your dad in the eye when he's talking to you,

54:59

whatever might be. It's a it's hard

55:01

to transition back and forth between all

55:03

these things. We think we're being efficient, and

55:06

we're not, because the amount of energy it takes

55:08

to actually make the switch is a lot

55:10

higher than we realized. That's kind of novel

55:12

thinking because you know, it's always been about multitasking.

55:15

How many things can I do at the same time. But

55:17

the second thing, which I think is, you

55:19

know I worry about the most, and I think is what

55:21

you're saying as well. Is that leaving

55:23

aside just the content on social media

55:25

for a second, and just the fact that it's

55:28

so incessant, Like you're saying, we

55:31

talk about stress and on

55:34

the brain and on the body. Stress

55:36

in and of itself is not the enemy.

55:38

I mean, in fact, we need stress. I was

55:41

a little nervous to do this podcast with

55:43

you today because I have so much respect for

55:45

you. But it makes me a little stressed because I have that nervousness.

55:47

But it's good. I need that because I prepared

55:49

for this. But the

55:51

problem is that we can't get a break

55:53

from the stress. Social media screens,

55:57

the incessant nature of it make it very

55:59

difficult for us to ever turn the stress

56:01

off. We don't want

56:03

to turn it off completely or never have it. That would

56:05

not be a worthy or or possible

56:08

goal. But we

56:10

don't get breaks from it. And that's

56:12

what I worry about the most. With with my

56:14

girls, myself to some extent, although I'm much more

56:17

aware of it. But that's what I worry about, Katie.

56:19

So you're saying that it's really important

56:22

to put the phones down, put them away,

56:25

even studies that show if it's on a

56:27

table, it's distracting

56:30

by its very presence, because

56:32

you can't have a deep, focused conversation

56:35

with that thing in your line

56:37

of sight, right that that that

56:39

the distraction, just

56:41

the presence of it, whatever it may be, it

56:43

takes you away from from being in the moment.

56:46

And again, I know some of this sounds so euphemistic,

56:48

but and maybe you've heard it all before, but

56:50

now the data is there. I mean, I

56:53

mean, the smartphone has only really

56:55

been around since two thousand five, Katie.

56:57

I mean you think about that fifteen years,

57:00

and it's not that long, and we've

57:02

had some of the biggest behavioral shifts

57:04

ever recorded in human history

57:06

during that time. You talk

57:09

about kind of constant stress, and

57:11

you need stress in them recovery. I guess

57:13

that's because your your brain is producing

57:15

too much cortisolve, right, I mean, the stress

57:18

hormone or is it doing

57:20

a lot of other stuff of physiologically.

57:23

I think I think the thing that that is

57:26

becoming clear is that the absolute

57:28

amount may not be as important

57:31

as how long your selves are are

57:33

sort of exposed to the to the stress hormone,

57:35

you can have these amazingly high spikes.

57:38

And they saw this, uh in people

57:41

fighter pilots, people who are in these incredible

57:43

situations for periods of time, really

57:46

high spikes, so high in fact, that the blood

57:48

vessels in the back of their eyes would change. They would

57:50

have to account for blurinus

57:52

of vision because they're they're they're a benefference

57:54

spikes so high. But when

57:57

they weren't in that situation,

58:00

they had incredibly low levels of

58:02

stress, really high heart rate variability.

58:05

Heart rate variability is a really interesting measure

58:07

of this because if you have high heart

58:09

rate variability, that's good. That

58:12

means that means your your your blood vesseles

58:14

aren't clamped down by all the stress

58:16

hormones. They're they're kind of loose

58:18

and can the variability is good. And

58:21

so it wasn't the spikes in

58:23

in cortisol, epineffer and other

58:25

stress hormones as much as it was them

58:28

staying plateaung at an at

58:30

an unreasonably high level. Interesting

58:33

when it comes to stress. In closing,

58:35

because I've kept you far too long Sunday,

58:37

but I could talk to you all day. Is what

58:40

is the impact of this

58:43

year plus of really

58:45

for many people, this constant

58:47

stress, And how

58:50

is that going to, in your view,

58:53

affect us in terms

58:55

of collective trauma? You

58:57

know, we we have we have pretty pretty

58:59

good data on what these stress

59:01

hormones in prolonged periods of time

59:04

due to the brain um we've you

59:06

know, we're, we're. That's been documented now

59:09

in all sorts of different studies. Nothing

59:11

quite like this, obviously, because this is so

59:13

unique, and that's why I still preface

59:16

by saying I don't know for sure. With great humility,

59:18

I try and answer some of these questions, but

59:21

I think there will

59:23

be an impact. But I think that we've also learned

59:26

that we can grow new brain

59:29

cells, though we can recover from that.

59:31

We can create situations where

59:33

it doesn't become such a incessant

59:36

memory that that it basically leads

59:38

to post traumatic stress, which is a real concern

59:40

as well. There will be people that

59:43

have significant amounts of post traumatic stress, but

59:46

our ability to treat that, to recognize

59:48

it is better than before, and our

59:50

ability to to build new brain cells

59:52

to help compensate. Is better than before,

59:55

so impact significant,

59:57

but solutions you know emerging

59:59

as well. So it

1:00:01

is possible to heal. I hear

1:00:03

you say it is possible

1:00:05

to heal, and and we we we've

1:00:08

seen it before, you know, with other even

1:00:10

other pandemics. A

1:00:15

huge thank you to my friend Dr Sanjay

1:00:18

Gupta, who you can watch on CNN

1:00:21

or listen to on his daily podcast

1:00:24

Coronavirus Fact or

1:00:26

Fiction. His new book, by the

1:00:28

way, is called Keep Sharpe, How to Build

1:00:30

a Better Brain at any age. And

1:00:32

I want you all to know I just ate

1:00:34

breakfast using my left

1:00:37

hand. Next

1:00:44

Question with Katie Kurik is a production of My Heart

1:00:47

Media and Katie Kurk Media. The

1:00:49

executive producers Army, Katie

1:00:51

Curic, and Courtney Litz. The supervising

1:00:54

producer is Lauren Hansen. Associate

1:00:56

producers Derek Clements, Adriana

1:00:58

Fassio, and m Lee Pinto. The

1:01:01

show is edited and mixed by Derrick

1:01:03

Clements. For more information about

1:01:05

today's episode, or to sign up for my

1:01:07

morning newsletter wake Up Call, go to

1:01:09

Katie Currect dot com. You can also

1:01:11

find me at Katie Currect on Instagram.

1:01:14

And on my social media channels.

1:01:16

For more podcasts from I heart Radio,

1:01:18

visit the I heart Radio app, Apple

1:01:21

podcast, or wherever you listen to

1:01:23

your favorite shows.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features