Podchaser Logo
Home
Molecular Neurobiology (BRAIN CHEMICALS) Encore with Crystal Dilworth

Molecular Neurobiology (BRAIN CHEMICALS) Encore with Crystal Dilworth

Released Thursday, 20th July 2023
 1 person rated this episode
Molecular Neurobiology (BRAIN CHEMICALS) Encore with Crystal Dilworth

Molecular Neurobiology (BRAIN CHEMICALS) Encore with Crystal Dilworth

Molecular Neurobiology (BRAIN CHEMICALS) Encore with Crystal Dilworth

Molecular Neurobiology (BRAIN CHEMICALS) Encore with Crystal Dilworth

Thursday, 20th July 2023
 1 person rated this episode
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

Pack lightly, not a chance. In

0:02

the all-new 2024 Chevy Trax, you and your squad have

0:06

the space and versatility you deserve

0:08

and need for your next big adventure. With

0:10

large, modern display screens, wireless

0:12

phone connectivity, and affordable pricing, this

0:15

small SUV is kind of a

0:17

big deal. And with the option to choose

0:19

between stylish designs, like the cool,

0:21

competent Active or the ultra-sporty

0:24

RS, you're sure to find the all-new Chevy

0:26

Trax fits your budget and your brand.

0:29

Life comes with a lot of decisions, and

0:31

it can be hard to know the right path sometimes.

0:33

A therapist can help you map out what you really want,

0:36

so you trust yourself to make great choices

0:38

and feel excited about the future. BetterHelp

0:40

offers convenient, professional online therapy

0:43

on your schedule, however you want it,

0:45

by phone, chat, or video call. Let

0:48

therapy be your map with BetterHelp. Visit

0:50

betterhelp.com slash Stitcher today

0:53

to get 10% off your first month. That's

0:55

betterhelp, H-E-L-P, dot com,

0:57

slash Stitcher.

1:00

Oh, hey, this is 2023 Allie here to say

1:02

hello to all the new listeners

1:04

via Radiolab, oh, hey. And

1:06

the good news is, is that this Encore is phenomenal,

1:09

and it's helpful for literally

1:11

every person with a brain. The

1:14

bad news is, is that we're running an Encore because

1:16

I have been in the ER

1:18

two times this week with pneumonia.

1:20

Have you ever gotten pneumonia?

1:22

I have, once this week. It

1:24

sucks. So I'm in bed for

1:26

the week, I'm laid out. I have not been this sick

1:28

in decades, my apologies. But please

1:30

enjoy this stellar episode for the

1:32

first time or for an Encore. It

1:35

has tons of stuff that we both know that you may have

1:37

forgotten. And pass it along to anyone else with

1:39

a brain, okay.

1:40

Oh, hey, it's the lady who keeps

1:42

candles in her wallet because you never know when you're

1:44

gonna be in a pinch and it's gonna be someone's birthday.

1:47

And then you'll be more excited about singing to them

1:49

than they'll be excited about being sung to.

1:51

Allie Ward, back with another episode of

1:54

Ologies.

1:55

Okay, you like brains? Does

1:57

your brain like brains? It probably does. So

2:00

right now, your soft, squishy

2:02

think lump is just hanging

2:04

out in your head. It's thinking about itself.

2:07

How does it work? What's in there? Why

2:09

do I want to eat Cool Whip out of the tub

2:11

with my fingers? And why aren't I more excited

2:14

about folding my laundry? The answer,

2:17

molecular neurobiology. But before

2:19

we splish splash into your mood juices,

2:22

let's take care of some business up top and thank all

2:24

the folks on patreon.com slash ologies

2:26

for being in the club. I'm sure you all support the show

2:29

and you hear what topics I'm working on first and

2:31

you submit your questions for theologists.

2:34

Thanks to everyone wearing Ologies merch from

2:36

ologiesmerch.com. Thanks to everyone

2:39

who forwards an episode to a friend

2:41

or who subscribes on their devices

2:44

and rates and especially reviews because

2:46

you know I read your words and I pick a

2:48

freshie to put on blast. Such as Dusky

2:50

who says, they're falling back in love with

2:53

life and that theologists have shown me

2:55

the light. Okay, molecular neurobiology.

2:57

Let's get into it. Let's break it down. So molecules,

3:00

the word derives from the Latin for

3:02

tiny mass and neuro

3:04

comes from the Greek for sinew

3:07

or cord or penis because

3:09

neurons are elongated. They

3:11

look like strings or cords or

3:14

I guess penises. Biology

3:16

of course, the study of life. So

3:18

molecular neurobiology, the

3:20

study of the tiny masses that

3:23

bring our dick looking brain cells to life.

3:25

I'm just reading facts here. Now this ologist

3:27

got a bachelor's in biochemistry

3:30

at UC San Diego and later a PhD

3:32

in molecular neuroscience at Caltech.

3:35

She's also a dancer, a gymnast, a violinist,

3:38

a TEDx youth speaker, a tech

3:40

strategist, and a TV host for

3:42

Voice of America, Al Jazeera America,

3:45

Seeker, Discovery News, and more.

3:48

She's an if then STEM ambassador

3:50

for the American Association for the Advancement of

3:52

Science

3:52

and Linda Hill Philanthropies. And

3:55

finally, she is appointed a role model

3:57

to other women and girls in science,

3:59

technology.

3:59

engineering and math fields. She also appears

4:02

on segments of the new CBS show Mission

4:04

Unstoppable where she is known

4:06

accurately as Dr. Brain. I've

4:09

known her for five years and have adored

4:11

her since we first sat down and shared

4:13

a basket of sweet potato fries in 2014

4:16

and I was just straight-up giddy to

4:18

have her on my couch and to ask her

4:20

one million questions about what

4:22

a brain is made of in white matter

4:25

and gray matter and what makes us happy and

4:27

how do antidepressants work and why are some

4:29

substances addictive? What happens

4:31

on drugs and can I have new habits

4:34

and what is anxiety all about and how depression

4:36

works and caffeine hacks that

4:37

may not work? So get ready to

4:40

fill your ears and the thing between

4:42

your ears with all kinds of wisdom from

4:44

wonderful person, neuroscientist,

4:47

your new good friend, and molecular

4:49

neurobiologist Dr. Crystal

4:52

Dilworth.

4:58

Start recording. Can you

5:00

say your first and

5:02

last name please? I pronounce

5:04

it right.

5:11

Crystal

5:17

Dilworth? I know. Do you want me to spell it for you?

5:19

No! Okay. Dr. Dilworth.

5:22

Dr. Dilworth. I

5:24

always like to ask this question. What was it like when you

5:27

like came out of the room from defending and you're like, I'm

5:29

Dr. Dilworth. So

5:30

I came out of the

5:33

room and my committee was still in there

5:36

and deliberating and normally

5:38

that is one of the scariest moments

5:41

in anyone's life because you're not sure what

5:43

they're going to say but I was I

5:45

was pretty sure because

5:47

my committee chair had been like we're just gonna chat

5:49

for a little bit we'll be right out. They came out and they

5:51

shook my hand and they said congratulations Dr. Dilworth

5:54

and I got an entirely new lease on life.

5:56

Oh my god, I'm so excited.

5:59

Like everything.

5:59

change. Did you know

6:02

growing up that you were gonna be a doctor

6:05

or a neuroscientist? I mean, you

6:07

are really good at a lot of things and I think that

6:09

that sometimes is difficult. No, I

6:12

was gonna be a dancer. I was gonna

6:14

be on stage at Lincoln Center just like all

6:16

of the books that I had read about how to be

6:19

a professional ballet dancer had nothing

6:21

to do with science really. And then so

6:24

were you studying ballet

6:27

and then sneaking into chem classes?

6:29

How did it work?

6:29

Who were you cheating on? Scholastically.

6:33

I think the decision to go to grad school,

6:36

I was definitely cheating on my dance classes.

6:38

I was in professional dance school in New York City

6:40

doing the things that you have to do to be a professional

6:43

dancer and I just wasn't fulfilled

6:45

by the experience. I think it's really hard. I was lying about

6:48

my age so that I could be in the school

6:50

to begin with and dancers

6:52

are they're treated like empty

6:54

vessels, right? The choreographer, the

6:57

artistic director, these are the people

7:00

that are filling the empty vessel with the intention.

7:02

And when you have a bachelor's degree in biochemistry

7:06

and you're used to doing independent research

7:09

as an intern in a research lab, being

7:12

treated like you have nothing personally to contribute

7:14

is very difficult. Yeah. And I was

7:17

looking for an opportunity to

7:19

be an adult and to be treated like I had something

7:21

to intellectually contribute. I wasn't getting that

7:23

in my artistic life. So I

7:26

started skipping my classes and

7:28

taking the subway uptown to Columbia

7:31

and attending the chemistry department lectures,

7:33

which is insane. Now that I

7:36

look back on it, now like nobody goes to those lectures voluntarily.

7:38

Like the grad students are only there because of the free

7:40

pizza, but I was actually

7:42

there for the intellectual stimulation,

7:45

which is terrifying and awful. Wait,

7:47

they give pizza out of these things? Otherwise,

7:49

nobody goes.

7:52

Yeah. Most of those, like weekly

7:54

lectures are accompanied by some type of bribe.

7:57

So why neuroscience? Well,

7:59

Well, I guess I was

8:02

always interested in people and

8:04

their behavior because maybe

8:07

as a homeschooled kid that didn't

8:09

have like a really diverse

8:11

social network. I mean, I had

8:13

a social network, but not the diversity

8:15

that you wouldn't see in like public school

8:18

for instance. Some people's

8:20

their behavior seemed unfathomable to me.

8:22

Like, I just don't understand. Like what is this

8:24

programming and how does it work? And

8:26

so I thought, oh, maybe I would study history.

8:29

Maybe I would study sociology or psychology.

8:33

And my dad said, no. He

8:35

was like, that's not a real science.

8:38

None of those are real sciences and

8:40

you have to choose a real science. What

8:43

was your dad? Did your dad study science? Yeah, his

8:45

background is in physics. My mom's background

8:47

is in microbiology. That was what they

8:49

understood. Okay. Um, and

8:52

I don't think they were afraid of all the things the parents

8:54

are afraid of like, she's never going to get a job. She's

8:57

going to be destitute. She's going to move back home. I live with

8:59

my mom right now, by the way. So

9:03

FYI, best laid plans of

9:05

my sin men. Yes,

9:08

but you're like an international traveler and you're

9:10

like about to move to Sumatra. It's

9:13

true. Yeah. There

9:15

are reasons also that I decided

9:17

to move in with my mother to help take care of her

9:19

as well. But, um, you know, like

9:22

I said, this is their plan. Um,

9:25

but so brains, like, do you start with

9:27

molecular biology? When you decide,

9:30

okay, I want to figure out how this weird big lump

9:32

of stuff in my bone bowl

9:35

in my head works, like, where do you

9:37

even begin with that? You start with like neural anatomy.

9:40

Do you start with chemistry of it? Like

9:42

for me, the Eureka moment was

9:44

I was taking organic chemistry because,

9:46

you know, typical freshmen, O chem,

9:49

what everybody has steak. And I was also taking

9:52

biopsychology, which is the closest I

9:54

could get to a psych class and still like

9:56

have this be approved. Um,

9:59

and I should. I can't

10:00

like clarify that I started

10:03

college really young. So I was probably 14

10:06

or 15 at this time. So

10:08

my parents were still approving my course

10:11

load. So I was restricted in what

10:13

courses I could take based on their approval.

10:16

Oh my God. Wait, I've known you for four years and I

10:18

did not know that you started college at 14 or 15.

10:21

Yeah. What? Yeah.

10:24

I started at a junior college, which, you know, your first two years

10:26

are the types of courses you're able to take

10:28

there. And I transferred to a four year college

10:31

much later. Oh my God. I don't

10:33

think I was wearing a bra at 14. I

10:36

wasn't either. Your parents

10:38

would have obviously like help you figure out what courses

10:40

you're going to take. So bio psychology.

10:43

Yeah, it's kind of backfired because I was in bio psych

10:45

and they laid out in

10:48

the book and in the lecture the pictures

10:51

of the different neurotransmitters, the

10:53

chemicals in our brain that sort

10:55

of determine the brain functionality that translates

10:58

into behavior. And I would just learn

11:00

from my organic chemistry class how to

11:02

identify the critical chemical

11:04

functionalities like that's an oxygen

11:07

group, like an OH group hydroxyl. That's

11:09

a benzene ring and sort of start to understand

11:12

how those things sort of fit with our

11:14

biology. And that was like the aha

11:16

moment. If you're like, quick

11:18

word, what is a benzene ring? It's

11:21

not an oil gang, but more elementally

11:23

it's six carbon atoms that are joined

11:25

in a ring with a hydrogen atom stuck

11:28

to each. And she was like, ah,

11:30

my brain loves this stuff. Now

11:32

her bachelor's in biochem and

11:34

so far most of her college courses were

11:36

more generally about the human body

11:39

chemistry and didn't focus on the

11:41

thinky parts of the human body as much.

11:43

So I didn't really

11:46

go back to focusing on the brain until my

11:49

senior year. So all of my upper division

11:51

electives were in neuroscience and that's

11:53

when I was like, this is how

11:55

I want to apply these things. What?

11:58

Okay. What is the difference between?

12:00

neuroscience, neurology, neurobiology,

12:03

molecular neurobiology. I feel

12:06

like if you don't work with brains, you're like, oh,

12:08

just kind of call it a neuro something

12:10

or other. Like what are those different

12:12

fields mean? Okay, so I'm

12:15

going to take you on a little journey. Okay. Story

12:17

time. I am a first year

12:19

graduate student. I have not yet chosen

12:21

a lab. I am real, I'm at Caltech.

12:24

Every single person that I'm meeting is smarter than

12:26

me. I am incredibly intimidated

12:28

and anxious and like really, really need

12:30

to do a good job. Otherwise I'm going to fail life. And

12:33

I go into my first meeting with the

12:35

professor that's going to be my PhD

12:37

thesis advisor, but I don't know that at the time. And

12:40

I'm trying to impress him with how smart I am. And

12:42

I tell him I'm really interested in

12:45

neurology and I'm really interested

12:47

in brains and the things

12:49

that brains do. And he's like, I'm going to stop you right

12:51

there. I'm going to let you finish. Oh, God. If

12:54

you're interested in neurology, then

12:56

you should be going to medical school. We

12:58

don't do neurology here. Neuroscience

13:01

is the science behind the brain and we

13:03

do research on how the brain works

13:06

and we get PhDs. And

13:09

that's the type of science that we can do on

13:11

the brain here. Are you still interested? Oh

13:13

my God.

13:14

And so that is the difference. I

13:17

would have had immediate reactive

13:19

diarrhea and just excuse myself from

13:22

life. I wouldn't have been like, oops. But

13:24

you know when you're so anxious and your

13:27

whole fight or flight system is engaged and

13:29

you're kind of too numb to it and you're just like,

13:31

okay, take the hit and keep rolling, keep going,

13:33

keep going. There's nothing you can do about it.

13:36

So what is a neurologist exactly?

13:39

Okay, they are physicians, medical

13:41

doctors with MDs, probably

13:43

stethoscopes, I don't know, who treat

13:46

neurological diseases and disorders

13:48

that affect the nerves and spinal cord

13:51

and of course the brain. So you can show

13:53

up in their office and say,

13:54

please doc, fix me. That

13:56

is a neurologist. I honestly don't know

13:58

about this stethoscopes. I just made that up. They

14:01

might not even need them, but you get the general aesthetic.

14:03

And a neuroscientist

14:05

studies the science behind how

14:07

the brain works and why the brain

14:09

works. So you have to have knowledge of some

14:11

of that stuff, but it's

14:13

mostly like hypothesis driven

14:16

investigation. Mm-hmm. Okay,

14:18

so

14:19

a brain. What is it? What

14:22

is this big? Is it mostly fat? What

14:24

is it? Is it proteins? What is it made

14:27

of? Yeah, it's all those good things. All fats and

14:29

proteins and carbohydrates all smushed

14:32

together into a collection of different

14:34

types of cells. There's like 80

14:37

billion neurons and they're all sort

14:39

of smushed together. And there's different

14:41

types of those neurons, those brain cells,

14:43

and they're clustered together in different areas.

14:46

And those different areas have specific

14:49

functions that all have to work together.

14:50

And that's sort

14:53

of what we think of as

14:55

the orchestra of the

14:57

brain as an organ, but that's like not

14:59

even it. But wait, there's more. There's

15:02

like a whole other layer of cells.

15:04

We call them glial cells or astrocytes

15:07

that help those neurons to function.

15:10

So it's not just neurons, but there's like a whole

15:12

other set of support cells.

15:15

And they're not even really support cells because they're doing really important

15:17

stuff. And what do the astrocytes and glial cells do?

15:20

They do so many things. So

15:23

my favorite

15:23

type of support cell is

15:26

the cell that creates myelination

15:28

around electrons. So that's like little

15:31

wire insulators to help the

15:33

electric part of the signal go faster

15:36

down the axon of the neuron. So

15:38

I can like send, if I'm a cell and

15:40

you're a cell, I can send my message to you like way

15:43

faster because of the insulation. And

15:45

that's actually one of the last parts

15:47

of brain maturation. So when

15:49

we talk about brains like not being fully cooked

15:51

until our mid-20s

15:53

and we're still developing, one of the last

15:55

things that happens is

15:57

that installation process goes in.

15:59

in that prefrontal cortex area, which is

16:02

so important for decision making. So

16:04

glial cells are a support cell, and

16:06

glial means glue, because

16:08

it was thought that glial cells just kind

16:10

of held all the neurons together, kind

16:13

of like a bunch of mashed potatoes around a pile

16:15

of yarn. But they do so much more than that,

16:17

and there are different types that do different things.

16:20

We won't go into all of them, but the

16:22

astrocytes are starry shaped,

16:24

hence astro, and they give nutrients

16:27

to neurons, they help repair damage, and

16:29

the oligodendrocytes insulate

16:32

the neurons in the brain by laying down this

16:34

fatty, cozy padding called myelin,

16:37

which is like rubber around an electrical

16:39

cord, or a snuggy that protects

16:41

you from live wires. Now if you have multiple

16:44

sclerosis, like my mom, aka

16:46

our dear Fancy Nancy, who taught you the

16:49

best insomnia hack ever in the Somnology

16:51

episode, the immune system of folks with MS

16:53

likes to eat away at that myelin and cause

16:56

nerve and signaling troubles. Just a side note,

16:58

thank you to all the neuroscientists

16:59

and neurologists working to find a cure for

17:02

MS. We appreciate it, and I want

17:04

to interview you about it please. Now why

17:06

is it important for these diva neurons

17:09

to be so supported and so insulated?

17:12

What do they look like? What do they do? Now

17:14

neurons themselves, those

17:16

are long and have fingers at one

17:19

end, kind of. Can you explain what a neuron

17:21

is? They can be long, they can be short, but

17:23

the critical parts of the neuron are

17:26

the cell body, which is where all

17:28

the good stuff happens, just like a normal

17:29

cell, and the axon,

17:32

which is sort of like that long wire

17:34

that connects one end to whatever

17:37

other cell it wants to talk

17:39

to. There's projections both from on

17:42

each side of the neuron.

17:45

Those would be considered like dendrites, and

17:47

those dendrites create the connections,

17:50

which we call synapses, that

17:52

are how cells talk to each other.

17:54

So it's like sort of the main parts.

17:57

So neurons, they're a cell with a sometimes-

17:59

long axon to reach out to other cells,

18:02

and little fingery dendrites at the end.

18:04

And you may remember the dendrology

18:06

episode with Casey Clapp about

18:09

trees. So just think of those little branches

18:11

at the end of the neurons, those are dendrites. They

18:13

also kind of look like if a bird had a bunch

18:16

of toes and then those toes had

18:18

toes. That's your brain. Okay, so how

18:20

are all these neurons just chit-chatting, gabbing,

18:23

they're shooting the shit, running the show up there,

18:25

what are they doing? My favorite part of

18:27

neuroscience is the fact that neurons

18:29

use both electricity and chemicals. Oh.

18:33

As like communication. Okay, ooh, tell

18:35

me about this. So the

18:37

really important part of neurons is

18:40

that there's like all of these little gates that

18:42

are like regulating the ions

18:44

flowing in and out of them. And like ions are like

18:46

magnesium, calcium, sodium, these are

18:48

really chlorine, these are really important. And they're

18:50

just like constantly moving back and forth.

18:53

But because all of those ions are charged,

18:55

you get like a little electrical field. From

18:59

each of the different cells. And

19:01

so if I wanted to pass a

19:04

signal to you, it would start

19:06

as an electrical field

19:07

that goes all the way down my axon due

19:09

to opening and closing, opening and closing in the

19:11

ions. But then it gets the end and I can't

19:13

transmit electricity to you because there's a little

19:15

gap. And so what

19:18

does a cell do? The cell is like, okay, crap, we

19:20

have to communicate to the Alli cell. She

19:23

likes serotonin. We're gonna release serotonin

19:26

into this little gap. And so

19:28

that's when the electrical signal gets

19:31

converted into a chemical signal, which

19:34

you can read because you speak the

19:36

serotonin language because you have little

19:38

proteins on the ends of you and

19:41

like the end of your synapse and

19:43

you are catching all of those little serotonin

19:45

molecules and bringing them into you. And

19:48

when there's enough of them, it generates

19:50

another electrical signal that

19:53

you can send. Oh my God, and how

19:55

many cells are doing this

19:58

all the time in our brain lumps?

19:59

So I don't know how many cells

20:02

would be active at a given time because that really depends

20:04

on what we're doing. But if you think there's like 80

20:07

billion neurons and then there's like

20:10

estimate like 100 trillion synapses.

20:13

Oh my God. Because it's not necessarily one

20:16

synapse per or two synapses

20:19

per cell. You can have more

20:21

connections. So this

20:24

is a lot. Yeah. We're talking

20:26

the final number is a shit ton.

20:28

Maybe many zeros. Yeah. Okay.

20:31

And so neurotransmitters. This

20:34

is like a chemical messenger that

20:36

cells are sending to each other. And

20:38

what are the main

20:40

neurotransmitters? I know we hear about dopamine and

20:42

serotonin and maybe norepinephrine. But

20:44

take me through some of the players here. Yeah.

20:47

So I think dopamine is like the media darling

20:50

of the neurotransmitter

20:53

world. You have a lot

20:55

of specific chemicals

20:58

like the three that you mentioned

21:00

that are involved in a lot of behaviors.

21:02

But then there's other types of

21:05

messengers as well. So we have

21:08

small peptides like

21:10

we would say oxytocin, which

21:12

it's not necessarily a formal neurotransmitter

21:15

but it's really critical in modulating

21:17

brain function and behavior for

21:19

instance.

21:21

Oxytocin you may have heard is a neuropeptide,

21:24

not to be confused with Oxycontin, which

21:26

is an opiate. But oxytocin can

21:28

promote bonding and feelings of comfort

21:31

and attachment with partners and members

21:34

of a group or with babies. And

21:36

yes, it does increase when you pet

21:39

a dog, which is why you probably

21:41

would not follow around an unfamiliar

21:43

goose in a park and pick up its poo, but

21:46

you would for your dog and not think twice. Now

21:48

on to more neurotransmitters. We

21:51

use

21:51

acetylcholine a lot. So that's

21:53

a neurotransmitter that I studied

21:55

because of its relation to nicotine,

21:58

which I'm sure we'll get to.

21:59

Acetylcholine is really important because it's like

22:02

the fast acting neurotransmitter

22:04

in the brain. So if you need to get

22:06

a cell to respond right away, acetylcholine

22:08

might be the way to go. And it's so fast acting that it's

22:10

used in the body as well to like help

22:13

with muscle contractions. Oh my God. Is

22:15

it like the text message of neurotransmitters? Yes. Get

22:18

at me. Just send me a text.

22:20

Your phone is blowing up. And so

22:22

acetylcholine, can that do

22:25

more than just make you happy or alert? Is

22:28

that, can that send all kinds of messages to you?

22:30

Yeah, it can. So if you think we

22:32

talked about the brain being groups

22:35

of different types of cells and

22:37

each of those cell groups probably has like different

22:39

layers of cells as well. So the complexity

22:42

in the brain is really, really difficult

22:44

I think to imagine. Each

22:46

of those different functional groups of cells or

22:48

different parts of the brain have connections

22:50

to one,

22:51

if not many, many others.

22:53

And they're all talking to each other. That's why I kind

22:55

of call it the orchestra because they're all

22:57

working together. And if you think about each

23:00

different system, like maybe the string system is

23:02

dopamine system and the brass

23:05

section would be your norepinephrine.

23:08

Everybody sort of is talking to each

23:10

other, but in different languages. And it might be

23:12

that I'm a cell that releases acetylcholine,

23:15

but you don't have any receptors for that. So

23:17

you can't see my signal, but somebody

23:19

else can. Did you use

23:21

that metaphor in your PhD defense? Because

23:23

I think it slaps. You should have. I

23:26

didn't. I don't. The orchestra of

23:28

the brain. I'm sure

23:30

it's not original. I think it's

23:32

pretty good. I'm going to look it up and I'll tell you if anyone else

23:34

has used it. Okay, so other folks

23:37

have used this and it turns out because it's a really

23:39

apt good analogy. Also when

23:41

it comes to working on brains, Crystal

23:44

used data from rodent brains to

23:46

try to extrapolate what was happening

23:48

in human brains, including I

23:50

guess her own.

23:52

Did you ever have any existential crises

23:55

when you were like, my brain is studying brains,

23:58

brains on brains on brains? Does it ever freak you out?

23:59

No, I think there's the

24:02

Carl Sagan quote, like we are the way for

24:04

the universe to know itself. And

24:06

so I think that that's kind of

24:08

how I feel as a neuroscientist.

24:11

Do you ever think about certain

24:14

reactions you have to life or certain like,

24:16

if you're having a down day or an up day, are you ever

24:18

thinking about like your orchestra, like you're like horn

24:21

sections going off right

24:23

now? Absolutely. You

24:25

do? Yeah, of course. Does that help you at all when

24:27

you're relating to other people thinking, okay,

24:29

well, this is not just this person's a jerk or

24:31

maybe this person isn't being sad

24:34

for effect. Like do you think about

24:36

them as like a concert of chemicals ever?

24:38

Yeah, I think obviously I can.

24:41

That's when I'm thinking like rationally thinking

24:44

and using that prefrontal cortex

24:47

to try and compose a logical

24:49

flow around why somebody is behaving

24:52

the way that they are. And

24:54

everyday life, it's usually more

24:57

emotion driven, reactivity

24:59

driven. That's how our brains evolved is to react

25:01

to external stimuli, not necessarily to

25:04

think and problem solve about them as

25:06

the first thing, right? Because you don't want

25:08

to be like, is that a snake? I'm not sure if it's a snake.

25:10

I'm going to keep walking towards it until I'm absolutely

25:13

sure. Ouch. Now I'm dying,

25:15

right? That's not how brains, our

25:18

brains work. So I think when I can

25:20

take a step back and think rationally

25:22

about like, why is this person yelling at me? It

25:26

is helpful, but that's, you know, I'm human

25:28

just like everybody else. And that

25:31

prefrontal cortex, that's right behind

25:33

our forehead. And that's

25:35

the kind of meteor chunk that's evolved

25:37

more recently.

25:38

Yeah, I think that's what

25:40

we'd like to think of as one of the

25:42

differentiating parts of human

25:45

brains versus other animal

25:47

brains. And I want to make a comment about animal brains

25:49

in a second, but it's our ability

25:52

to extrapolate, to use

25:55

logic and reasoning to come up with

25:57

creative solutions to problems.

25:59

to not just react and to think

26:02

about downstream effects. That's what

26:04

the prefrontal cortex helps us do. But

26:07

what I was gonna say, which is

26:09

one of the myth busting things, I was like,

26:11

I'm gonna talk to Allie about neuroscience.

26:14

What do I want people to know about neuroscience? Okay,

26:17

the pop culture reference to the

26:19

lizard brain. Yes, yes, okay, okay,

26:21

okay. Let's debunk this flim

26:23

flam.

26:24

Really bothers me. Okay, I

26:26

was gonna ask about it. Because it's often

26:29

one used incorrectly. Okay. It's

26:32

usually when people say, don't listen to your

26:34

lizard brain. It's just

26:36

lizard brain, baby. I think what they

26:38

mean is, don't listen to

26:40

your limbic system. Okay. Or

26:43

your midbrain or the center

26:45

of your brain in which emotions

26:48

are generated and relevant.

26:52

But I think when that quote, and

26:54

I am blaming Sagan again for this,

26:57

when the Sagan quote of there

26:59

is an alligator brain around which

27:01

everything else is wrapped was put

27:03

out there, he meant something even more

27:06

basal, like your brain stem and the

27:08

parts of the brain that control respiration

27:11

and heartbeat and those

27:13

types of really, really basic biological

27:15

functions. But the fact of the matter is,

27:17

is that lizards and reptiles

27:20

actually have something similar to a cortex.

27:22

They do? Yeah, they do. Nothing

27:25

like the giant white

27:27

matter that we have. It's nothing

27:29

like the big prefrontal cortex

27:32

that you would see in primates, but it's

27:34

something that

27:35

evolves similarly. And when I say evolved,

27:37

I mean, the gestational period.

27:40

And you see very similar wiring

27:43

there. So. Poor lizards.

27:46

They're really getting a bad rap. Oh.

27:48

Also, are alligators even lizards? Back

27:51

in the serology episode, I asked lizard

27:53

expert, Aaron McGee about it. Alligators,

27:56

they are lizards. No. No.

27:59

No.

27:59

I just realized I was like, how big does a lizard

28:02

get? Is an alligator, why isn't an alligator a lizard?

28:04

I'm sorry. I'm

28:06

just not. Just not. So,

28:09

so much horsepucky flim flam debunked

28:11

all at once. So you mentioned white matter and gray matter.

28:15

What is the difference? So white matter

28:17

is all, is basically the wiring. Okay.

28:20

So when you would, you would say

28:22

like, there's a pathway

28:24

between two brain regions

28:27

and that's the white matter. It's the connections

28:30

and gray matter is like more

28:33

the cell bodies and the gooier, the

28:35

gooier

28:35

stuff. Okay. Put it that way.

28:37

Is there a skin on it? Kind of, is there like an apple skin on a brain?

28:40

Not the way that you are describing

28:43

it, but we do have a barrier between

28:45

the brain and the blood

28:48

system that provides the glucose

28:50

and the other nutrients to the brain. And

28:53

that's the blood brain barrier is critically

28:56

important to protecting the brain from

28:58

all the things that we're exposed to. Can

29:01

more things leak through that blood brain barrier than

29:03

we realize? Are we finding that out? Probably.

29:05

We used to think that it was impenetrable. And

29:08

now we know that there's evidence of a lot

29:10

more sort of transmission through

29:12

that barrier than we think.

29:15

But it really does protect us. I mean, think about like

29:18

all of the pharmacologicals that you've ever taken

29:20

in your life and some of them can

29:22

slip through and that's good

29:25

because we need them to regulate our behavior. And

29:27

some of them are kept out by that

29:30

barrier, which is great because they could be potentially

29:32

toxic. Okay, well getting

29:34

back to neurotransporters.

29:35

Oh, no. I wanted

29:38

to ask if you'd ever touched a brain before. So

29:40

I had to get us off course. I have touched a

29:42

human brain. It's

29:44

very delicate. Like you don't want to make

29:46

a lot of really fast, and they're preserved brains.

29:49

I haven't like touched a brain of a person,

29:51

which some neurosurgeons

29:53

have. I cannot speak to what it's like

29:56

to touch a live brain, but one

29:58

that's been preserved and formality.

29:59

to hide. It's very delicate.

30:02

It is as gooey as

30:04

you think.

30:06

When you're holding it, if you're anything like

30:08

me, there's an oppressive sense

30:10

of responsibility that

30:13

happens when you're thinking about the life that

30:15

that brain was really responsible for guiding.

30:18

I don't hold it for very

30:20

long. I held it and

30:22

I felt the profound

30:24

nature of what I was doing and then I gave

30:27

it back to the technician. What

30:29

was the setting here?

30:29

What was the setting? Yeah.

30:33

Honestly, this was in undergrad at a science

30:35

fair. And

30:37

that was just one of the really cool exhibits.

30:40

Like there was mouse brains and a

30:42

human brain and you know, like other

30:44

brains that you could just kind of like touch and play

30:46

with. Wouldn't it be crazy if you donated your brain to

30:48

science and they're like, you're just gonna go to science fairs. You're

30:50

gonna recruit some people. You'd be like, okay, sweetie.

30:53

Shake some hands, kiss some babies, not

30:55

shake some babies and kiss them hands. I almost said that

30:57

backwards. Okay, so neurotransmitters,

31:01

serotonin, dopamine. What

31:04

do they do? Do they have different roles in

31:06

terms of our emotions? They have very, very different

31:09

roles. Dopamine, I'm going to start

31:11

with because this is everybody loves

31:13

a good dopamine story.

31:15

I love it. And

31:17

without dopamine, we really wouldn't be motivated

31:20

to do anything. So it's really interesting

31:23

in computer science when they talk about computers

31:26

having rewards so that you can teach it

31:28

like artificial intelligence system

31:30

that

31:31

you're on the right track, keep going.

31:35

We have similar rewards. You're on the right

31:37

track, keep going. And dopamine

31:39

is how our brains have been

31:41

evolved to receive this reward.

31:44

So it makes us feel happy,

31:46

but not really happy. More just like

31:48

pleasure, like things are good.

31:50

And anything that you would do that

31:53

would keep you alive elicits a dopamine

31:56

response. So eating,

31:58

drinking, sleeping. and hanging

32:00

out with your friends, anything that you might enjoy,

32:03

you get a little bit

32:05

of a dopamine hit. So

32:07

it's basically to keep us doing things that

32:09

are going to keep the human race

32:12

alive. Okay. Basically.

32:14

Whereas serotonin is more nuanced.

32:17

It's not just pleasure, but it's mood

32:20

and it's sleep. And it's

32:23

really helping to modulate the way

32:25

that those little dopamine hits are interpreted

32:28

by the larger orchestra. If

32:31

that makes sense. Yeah. And

32:33

what happens when

32:34

they get off? Is there not enough

32:36

to send a signal to the next neuron?

32:39

Is there too much? And

32:41

why does it seem like

32:43

a very slim percentage of people have

32:46

a good balance? I feel like, or maybe

32:48

it's just living in LA or internet

32:51

culture, but I feel like everyone's

32:53

like, oh yeah, my neurotransmitters

32:56

are whack. I'm no minor. So I

32:58

mean, unfortunately I want,

33:01

I want to respond to you philosophically.

33:04

Like

33:06

when we first sequenced the human

33:08

genome, the lead researcher

33:11

on that project was the genome that

33:13

they chose to sequence. Does that mean

33:15

that he's the most normal genome

33:18

and every other genome is going to be compared

33:20

to his maybe it

33:22

was an arbitrary center for

33:24

science to pick. Right?

33:26

Okay. So quick aside, there was the publicly

33:29

funded human genome project and the

33:31

first public genome came mostly

33:33

from a single anonymous male donor.

33:36

I think this would be a sperm donor from

33:39

Buffalo, New York, but then aside

33:41

privately funded genome research

33:43

project was launched by geneticist J.

33:46

Craig Venter, who later admitted

33:48

that his DNA was among

33:50

the first donor pool to be fully sequenced,

33:53

tossing his own genetics into a research

33:55

project was

33:56

later addressed in the journal science

33:58

in an article bearing the

35:59

except for sleep. So

36:01

they just didn't do anything. But like, people

36:03

have probably experienced this before. Like

36:06

you're in a really good early stage

36:08

of your relationship. You don't

36:10

eat, you like your sleep's kind of disrupted.

36:13

All you want to do is like read your text messages

36:16

over and over and over again, or check

36:18

your phone to see if you've got another one. Your normal

36:20

function is disrupted because you've

36:23

got dopamine floating

36:25

around in there at levels that you're totally not used

36:27

to. And there's probably some oxytocin in there

36:29

as well, like really fucking things

36:32

up. Oh my God. Wait. So then

36:34

at what point does that decline?

36:36

Is

36:36

that like the two year period of like,

36:39

I'm over this. Yeah, I think we get,

36:41

we get used to it. Yeah. Right. And

36:43

then we can sort of like mellow out and

36:45

become more normal. Um,

36:48

the rats, some of them died because they

36:50

didn't, they didn't eat or

36:52

really do anything because they were just

36:54

super happy pressing

36:56

that lever for their dopamine

36:58

hit. So don't do that. Just pathologically

37:01

fulfilled. Oh no. Okay. Side

37:04

note. I read one article that estimated

37:06

four years was when dopamine starts

37:09

to wane, but

37:10

I really should ask a psychoneuroendocrinologist

37:12

or perhaps a biological anthropologist

37:15

about it. But if things are starting

37:17

to feel a little

37:18

stale with a partner, some

37:20

researchers think that doing scary

37:22

or novel things together, like,

37:24

I don't know, zip lining or going

37:27

to haunted houses or Costco

37:29

on a Saturday, those things can

37:31

get those new romance brain juices

37:34

squirting again. Okay. What happens if

37:36

you don't have enough dopamine? If you don't have enough

37:38

dopamine, it depends on what

37:42

parts of your system are disrupted.

37:45

But

37:45

most of the classical symptoms

37:48

that we see for like ADHD

37:51

or depression or, you know, even anxiety

37:53

and in some cases usually

37:56

have to do with disruption of the dopamine system. That's why

37:58

it gets all the media attention. The classic

38:00

depression is lower levels of

38:03

dopamine, which means that you're

38:05

just not having the same response

38:08

that someone that doesn't have depression to your

38:10

dog or to normal

38:14

things in your life that would normally make you

38:16

happy. You're tired, you're lethargic,

38:19

there's a lack of motivation and

38:21

you just aren't getting pleasure from the task

38:24

that you normally would. So it's like

38:26

that grayness, that lack of

38:29

color,

38:29

everything sort of seems blah. That

38:32

would be what it would be like to not have enough

38:34

dopamine in your system. And is that

38:37

because the dopamine

38:39

isn't being produced at high enough levels

38:41

or it's just like not making the jump between

38:43

the neurons?

38:46

That's something that is sort of on

38:48

an individual basis, but I would say

38:50

overall, it's probably your system

38:53

isn't able to produce enough dopamine.

38:56

So there's all these little packages of

38:58

the dopamine chemicals that are sitting at the

39:00

terminals like near the synapse, like just

39:02

ready for the signal, like release us, we're

39:05

ready to go. All systems are go. And

39:07

someone that has lower numbers

39:09

of those little vesicles, those little dopamine

39:12

packets, like even if the cell was like, okay,

39:14

go release all the dopamine

39:16

that you have, is going to be a lower

39:18

level, less packages of

39:20

dopamine released than

39:23

what we would consider to be a normal

39:25

cell. And then what about serotonin

39:28

then? Does

39:31

serotonin play a big part in depression

39:33

and anxiety and ADHD as well? Yes.

39:36

And all of the other things that all of us have?

39:39

The problem with the orchestra is that you

39:41

can't just remove one section, right?

39:47

Like they all work together. So yes,

39:49

they're definitely, serotonin is definitely implicated

39:52

in

39:53

pretty much

39:54

like everything. You know, our predefine is implicated

39:56

in pretty much everything, but it's just a matter of like, what

39:59

is the major contribution?

39:59

So

40:01

serotonin and depression, we're

40:04

used to thinking about SSRIs,

40:07

which are selective serotonin reuptake

40:09

inhibitors, which that's

40:12

the medication that we are giving

40:14

to people with depression. And

40:16

why is that? We want more

40:18

serotonin to be floating around

40:21

in that synapse, in that space between the

40:23

two cells. We want it to be sitting around

40:25

longer. So that signal to

40:28

continue elevating your mood is a

40:30

bigger signal. So there's usually

40:31

these little Pac-Mans that live

40:34

in that space between the two cells that

40:36

collect all of the leftover molecules

40:38

and bring them back into the cell. Like,

40:40

okay, we don't need you anymore, so you're

40:42

going to come and live back in the cell again. And

40:45

if we inhibit those little Pac-Man

40:47

collectors, then we get

40:49

more chemical in that synapse.

40:52

And that

40:53

raises the probability that the

40:55

next cell is going to have a prolonged

40:57

signal from that neurotransmitter.

41:00

Okay, so by cock-blocking the

41:02

neurotransmitter gobblers, there

41:04

will be more in the spaces between the neurons

41:07

to deliver messages. Kind of like if you canceled

41:10

a neurotransmitter's lift and you

41:12

just kept him at the party longer. You're like, sorry,

41:15

what can I say? We love having you around. You're

41:17

great at conversation. We love the signals

41:19

you send. This is a question that I

41:22

have had for years, but I feel like I read somewhere

41:24

that neuroscientists don't really

41:27

know how antidepressants work. Yeah,

41:29

no. Is that true? There's

41:32

so many medications and like, I'm

41:34

sorry, big pharma. There's so many

41:36

medications that we don't actually understand

41:39

the molecular mechanism for, but if

41:41

it works and the side effects aren't too

41:43

bad, we're just like, just

41:45

put it out there and it'll help people. Oh

41:47

my God. Okay, so we don't totally

41:50

know how SSRIs work.

41:52

We just know that a certain percentage

41:55

of people when they take them are like, feeling

41:57

better. We know exactly

41:59

how. they work. We

42:02

don't know why they work. Like

42:05

we don't know why keeping

42:07

the serotonin or the norepinephrine

42:10

or the dopamine around in

42:12

the synapse and increasing

42:15

the signal leads to the behavioral

42:17

changes because the level of complex,

42:20

we can ask those molecular questions. Like that's

42:22

the level I like to look at because it's a lot more concrete.

42:25

We can get answers there, but the

42:27

multiple layers of complexity from like, well,

42:29

which cells

42:29

are getting the attenuated signal and

42:32

what brain regions are those cells in? Oh,

42:34

but it's this brain region, but it's only those brain layers

42:38

of that brain region. And

42:40

what are those particular

42:42

active regions doing

42:44

when they're working in concert? And

42:47

how does that map to the genetic

42:49

background of this individual and

42:52

the external stimulus? And why does

42:54

that mean that giving this SSRI four

42:56

weeks later, this person is willing to get off their couch?

42:59

We don't know. Yeah. I always wondered about the lag

43:01

time there because that is the toughest. And

43:03

I know that this is like a psychopharmacology

43:06

question, but that is the toughest. If it's like,

43:08

hey, you're depressed, take this thing,

43:10

man, six weeks, there's

43:12

a 20% chance you might feel better. Like

43:14

you could have a lot of faith and lucky for me,

43:17

like, you know, well, not lucky for me, but I tried

43:19

a few different medications for anxiety and depression

43:21

before I found one that worked. I've mentioned this

43:23

in another episode, but I tried a genetic

43:26

test to see which anti-anxiety or

43:28

antidepressants would work better for

43:30

me. And I ended up going with something

43:33

that was recommended. It was an SNRI, but

43:35

you should do your own research. I did a ton

43:37

of reading and I decided I didn't have much to lose.

43:40

And I tried a company called GeneSight, which has

43:42

a sliding scale. It's super affordable. They are

43:44

not a sponsor, but it helped me out. But

43:46

your mileage definitely may vary. Anyway,

43:48

let's move on to Crystal's research

43:50

on nicotine addiction. And what did

43:53

you learn about how addiction works

43:55

having studied

43:56

the mechanisms behind nicotine?

43:59

For nicotine, it's super crazy. When

44:03

you're exposing yourself to nicotine, you're actually

44:05

changing the way that proteins

44:07

in your brain are expressing. So

44:10

they're like, oh, cool.

44:12

I really like this. I would like it

44:14

again. I would like it in a specific way. So

44:16

I'm going to change the way that I'm making the proteins

44:19

in my cells so that they are

44:22

better able to bind and

44:24

respond to this drug that

44:26

I have now been exposed to and know exists in the

44:28

world. And so what does nicotine do? Does

44:31

it wedge itself where a

44:33

different neurotransmitter should be? Yeah.

44:36

So nicotine looks a lot like acetylcholine.

44:39

So the receptors that bind nicotine

44:42

also

44:42

bind acetylcholine. They

44:44

are called nicotinic acetylcholine

44:46

receptors. Of course they are. They like dominated my

44:49

life for five years. But

44:52

what looks similar to a protein might

44:54

not actually look similar to us.

44:57

So acetylcholine is the one that's responsible

44:59

for those muscular contractions. It's

45:02

super fast acting. Scientists

45:04

also think it may affect memory and attention.

45:07

And crystal produced from her purse two

45:10

molecular models, as one does. So

45:12

she showed me that nicotine is a double

45:15

ringed molecule with two nitrogens. And

45:17

acetylcholine has one nitrogen

45:20

that's crowded with methyl groups, which

45:22

are three hydrogens bonded to a carbon.

45:24

But in a nutshell, crystal describes both

45:27

molecules

45:27

as having similar friends, aka carbon

45:29

atoms, that give them kind of an analogous

45:32

bulk when it comes to fitting into the

45:34

same receptors. So they look

45:36

similar enough to the receptor that

45:38

it responds in the same way. Got it. So

45:41

it's like when you're doing a puzzle and you find a piece that doesn't quite fit, but you

45:43

can jam it in and then it fucks everything else up? Pretty

45:46

much. It's exactly like that. I should have just said

45:48

that. That's exactly what's happening. And

45:51

so how do some people who might

45:53

be, say, predisposed to that

45:55

kind of addiction, how do

45:57

they have a better chance at beating

45:59

it? they want to if they're like, I'm

46:02

done with you vaping. I'm done with you like

46:05

cigarettes. Like what do they do? Well, beating

46:08

addiction is really challenging because

46:10

you have like a learning and memory component. And

46:12

then you have a chemical dependence component,

46:14

especially for smoking because

46:16

you'll be like, oh, I'm done. I

46:19

don't smoke anymore. I'm

46:21

successful. And then you'll have one

46:23

drink too many. And suddenly

46:25

you've got a cigarette in your hand and you're not exactly sure

46:28

why. And there's a chemical reason for that.

46:30

But there's also a learning and memory component.

46:32

Like you definitely beat your

46:34

addiction to smoking at work

46:36

and at home. You did not beat the addiction to smoking

46:39

at the club. In the club, we are all family.

46:41

Because you've learned that

46:44

you have three drinks, then you go outside and you have a smoke.

46:47

So it's those behaviors that

46:49

can really hang

46:51

up recovery. Nicotine is actually one of

46:53

the most addictive substances. So

46:57

I was listening to the addictionology

47:00

episode that you did. And

47:02

yeah, there's a lot of really, really terrible

47:04

withdrawal symptoms. Like withdrawing from alcohol

47:07

is potentially lethal. So you need to be careful.

47:09

We've seen media depictions of withdrawal

47:12

from heroin, for instance, which

47:14

looks like it's the one where everyone's like, oh

47:16

my god, I'm being attacked by bugs and my skin

47:18

is itchy. And I need to get my skin off. Like that's

47:21

awful. You're not going to get that if you try and quit smoking.

47:24

But once you go through those really,

47:26

really awful terrible withdrawal periods,

47:29

you have a really good chance of not

47:31

doing those drugs again. Whereas with nicotine,

47:34

it can come back really at any time. OK,

47:37

so what can one do? Is

47:39

there any promise when it comes to

47:42

meditation and mindfulness and breathing

47:44

exercise? Can you retrain your brain

47:46

through healthier behaviors at all?

47:49

Yeah, you can definitely retrain your brain. You

47:52

can also, through meditation, mindfulness,

47:55

and cognitive behavioral therapy, reduce

47:58

the reason that you're smoking.

47:59

at all. So we see smoking

48:02

behaviors, especially with addicts,

48:05

but yeah, like people that are addicted

48:08

to nicotine, oftentimes

48:11

are in response to other things. So schizophrenics

48:14

have a very specific smoking behavior.

48:16

We think they're trying to self-medicate veterans

48:18

come back, not even with PTSD, but just that

48:20

have come back from really traumatic experiences.

48:23

Possibly they're smoking in the

48:26

characteristic way that they smoke to reduce

48:28

activity in their amygdala.

48:29

So you may remember

48:32

the amygdala from the two-part pharaology

48:35

episode, and it is a little brain nugget that

48:37

I like to think of as the screaming

48:39

almond of terror.

48:43

So some folks may self-administer

48:45

nicotine to appease their shrieking

48:48

almond. Does it solve problems? No, not

48:50

at all. It only makes worse. Blame your almonds

48:53

and then try to outsmart it. There's

48:55

lots of different

48:58

indications that could cause somebody to

49:00

smoke heavily that would make quitting harder.

49:04

Is that at all the same when it comes

49:06

to anxiety or depression or ADHD?

49:08

Are there any kind of situational

49:11

triggers that might affect our levels of neurotransmitter?

49:14

Yeah, that's a problem. So we have that

49:16

learning and memory, as I was saying, component.

49:19

So we've learned something is dangerous

49:21

to us, even if it's not, then

49:24

our bodies are going to continue to respond to it that way.

49:26

And you have to retrain.

49:28

No, that's not like some people

49:30

are scared of dogs. Some people are scared

49:32

of people. You

49:35

are snakes. My mom

49:37

is terrified of snakes. And

49:40

it was my brother-in-law and he is like a six

49:42

foot four heavy metal guitarist with hair down to his

49:44

waist. And he can't if he sees a snake

49:46

on TV, he's like, turn it off. Yep.

49:48

My mom is exactly the same way. She gets like the

49:50

chills because and then she

49:52

like runs out of she runs out of the room. And

49:55

so if she decided that was something that

49:57

she wanted

49:58

to learn to not be afraid of,

49:59

afraid of, there are ways through

50:02

like overexposure and other therapeutic

50:05

methods that I don't know anything about, but I know exist

50:08

to

50:10

rewire the brain. Probably

50:12

that direct signal of snake

50:16

fear is never going to really go away,

50:18

but you might be able to add a layer of regulation

50:21

like snake checkpoint.

50:24

Okay, I'm going to react in a different way

50:26

instead of I'm going to react with my

50:29

fear response. And will your neurons

50:31

form new pathways? Will they kind

50:33

of make new channels? You are referring

50:35

to neuroplasticity. I did it. Yes, you

50:37

did. Yeah, you

50:40

can definitely create new connections.

50:43

We're doing that all the time. If we couldn't do

50:45

that, we wouldn't be able to learn anything new and we

50:48

wouldn't be able to teach babies all the things that they need

50:50

to learn in order to be competent humans.

50:52

I mean, I've got to assume that there's some

50:54

of those out there. So

50:57

yes, through using particular

51:00

pathways, particular connections in the brain,

51:02

you can make those connections stronger. You can

51:04

recruit other connections to

51:06

make that pathway larger. I like

51:08

to think of it as like you start out with like a hiking

51:11

trail that you were told was a trail

51:13

and Google maps doesn't really have it on

51:15

there and you have need a machete and you're kind of like

51:17

hacking through it. It's a jungle in here.

51:20

But if you walk that trail many, many

51:22

times, it eventually becomes much easier

51:24

to use and it can

51:26

eventually become like a six lane

51:29

super highway. That's very, very fast

51:31

to go down. And that's the preferred method because our brains

51:33

are really lazy. They don't want to do

51:35

new things. They don't want to think about

51:37

anything. They just want to react because that's how we

51:40

stay alive. And so if you can train

51:42

your brain that taking the path that you

51:45

want it to take is actually easiest and

51:47

allows it to be the most lazy, then

51:49

you can influence the path that it chooses to

51:52

take without you cognitively having to control

51:54

it all the time. So practice

51:56

makes a habit kind of. Yes. Well,

51:59

that's good to know.

51:59

I have a gym membership that I have not used

52:02

in a month and I'm like, okay, can

52:04

I ask you questions

52:07

from patrons? Yeah. Okay. Now,

52:10

before we dive into the questions that you submitted on Patreon,

52:12

a few words about sponsors who make it possible

52:14

for Ologies to donate to a different cause each

52:17

week. Now Crystal is once again an if

52:19

then STEM ambassador for the Lida Hill

52:21

Foundation, which works with a few nonprofits. So

52:23

that's already amazing. But she chose

52:25

her donation to go to the Gina Davis Institute

52:28

on Gender and Media, founded by

52:29

Gina Davis, who has said,

52:32

what our children see sets the framework

52:34

for what they believe is possible. So

52:37

the Gina Davis Institute on Gender and Media

52:39

is the only research-based organization working

52:42

from within the entertainment industry to

52:44

improve gender balance, to reduce stereotyping,

52:46

and to create diverse female characters

52:49

in entertainment and media for kids 11 and

52:51

under. So thanks, Dr. Dilworth. A

52:53

donation will go to them and that is made possible

52:55

by some sponsors of the show, which you may hear about now.

52:59

I have plates from East Fork and

53:01

I love them. They are lovely. Listen,

53:04

did I ever think that I would fawn over dishes?

53:07

No, but then there's East Fork dishes

53:09

and you're like, number one, this company is cool as

53:11

hell. Okay. They

53:13

pay a good wage. They've got a factory in Asheville, North Carolina.

53:16

They have such beautiful bowls

53:19

and mugs and they're durable,

53:21

which is good because I bump into things. They

53:23

also have adorable little cake plates. They have everyday

53:26

bowls. They have a mug that took 10

53:28

years to perfect. When I go to pour

53:31

a chai tea in the morning, I'm looking straight at

53:33

those mugs and I'm like, you're coming with me, baby. They're

53:35

a B Corp. They're climate neutral certified.

53:38

Also people at East Fork listen to ologies. So

53:40

you know, they're one of us. I love my East Fork

53:42

bowls so much that I use them

53:44

to feed the crows. That's how much

53:47

I love the crows and that's

53:49

how much I love East Fork. I put some peanuts

53:51

out there. I honestly go, I

53:52

give the crows access. To

53:57

the best dinner planes we have. collection

54:00

today at eastfork.com and

54:02

get 15% off with my promo

54:05

code, Ologies. So that's eastfork.com,

54:08

promo code Ologies. They have beautiful glazes.

54:10

Okay, bye-bye.

54:11

What's a game where absolutely no one wins?

54:14

The waiting game. Don't do it. When

54:16

it comes to hiring, don't wait for people

54:18

to find you. Find them first with

54:20

Indeed, because when you're hiring, you need Indeed.

54:23

What is Indeed? So it's a hiring platform where

54:25

you can attract, you can interview, and you can hire all

54:28

in one place. Instead of going to a bunch of different job

54:30

sites, searching for candidates, hoping

54:32

you find someone with the right skills, they

54:34

have this hiring platform that can help

54:36

you do all of that. They have instant match. And

54:39

with that, over 80% of employers

54:41

get really good candidates whose resume on

54:43

Indeed matches their job

54:45

description, and it happens like poof the

54:47

moment they sponsor a job. No waiting. You

54:50

can start hiring now with a $75 sponsor

54:53

job credit to upgrade your job post

54:55

at Indeed.com slash Ologies.

54:58

Offer is good for a limited time. So claim your $75

55:00

credit now at Indeed.com

55:02

slash Ologies. Just go to Indeed.com

55:05

slash Ologies, and you can support the show by

55:08

saying that you heard about it on this podcast. So that's

55:10

Indeed.com slash Ologies. Terms

55:12

and conditions apply. Need to hire? You

55:15

need

55:15

Indeed. There are so

55:17

many reasons to learn a new language. Whether you're

55:19

going to travel, you're going to order something

55:21

in another language, maybe flirt or

55:23

find a bathroom. Teaching your brain

55:25

how to communicate with other people, it's the best,

55:28

but getting started can be a little overwhelming. But

55:30

Rosetta Stone, they break

55:32

down your new language into these bite-sized

55:34

pieces, and they focus on practicing for

55:37

real conversations. Rosetta Stone knows

55:39

how to do this. They have an immersive method

55:42

that helps you learn more intuitively. They

55:44

also have a true accent feature, and it helps you

55:46

perfect

55:46

your pronunciation. They also have a Rosetta

55:48

Stone app, so you can learn anytime, anywhere,

55:51

even if you only can do it for 10 minutes at a time.

55:53

Do you really care what your sister's college roommate

55:55

is doing with her life? No. Spend that

55:57

time learning another language. Rosetta Stone.

55:59

like we got an app. Come say hi. And I

56:02

am proud to report I have more

56:04

words in my brain than ever before.

56:07

Also my friend Pablo's from Mexico and he said that my accent's

56:09

pretty good. Bueno. And now for a limited

56:11

time our listeners can get Rosetta Stone's

56:13

lifetime unlimited subscription. That

56:16

means access to all 25 of

56:18

their languages from Dutch to Arabic

56:20

to Italian forever for 40% off. So

56:23

visit rosettastone.com

56:26

slash ologies rosettastone.com

56:28

slash ologies Rosetta

56:29

Stone. How language is learned. Listen

56:32

BarkBox believes in a couple of things.

56:34

One everything's better if there's a dog around

56:37

and two podcast ads can get

56:39

kind of annoying but this one is about your dog so

56:41

it's a good. One of my favorite holidays

56:43

is called Wolfanute and it's a celebration of dogs

56:45

and wolves and BarkBox kind

56:47

of makes every day Wolfanute. So each

56:50

month BarkBox delivers a new collection

56:52

of original toys that's themed

56:55

to you and your dog. I got one recently

56:57

that was all movie themed. There was a squeaky

57:00

toy that looked like popcorn.

57:02

I love BarkBox. We get it every month

57:04

for Grammy. She gets new little squeaky toys

57:06

that she chases around. Sometimes you throw them

57:09

and then she just runs over to it and then she comes back

57:11

without the toy and you're like that's not how fetch works but that's

57:13

okay. We've also given BarkBox to so

57:15

many people. Our neighbors, our loved ones.

57:18

Also how many times have you had to go to a baby shower,

57:20

get presents for people's weddings. What about your friend

57:22

who just got a dog? Don't they deserve

57:24

some presents and treats and toys? BarkBox

57:27

is such a good gift for friends

57:29

who have dogs.

57:29

BarkBox, we love you. You

57:32

can sign up now at BarkBox.com slash

57:34

ologies for an exclusive offer. This

57:37

ad is now over. Let's go back to petting our dogs.

57:40

Okay let's get to your molecular neurobiology

57:43

questions shall we? I have questions

57:45

from Petron. And also this definitely

57:47

warrants like a psychopharmacology follow-up

57:50

because absolutely I think that given

57:53

all of the the response to

57:55

and all the questions you definitely need like a psychiatrist.

57:58

Yes!

57:59

This podcast is not intended

58:02

to diagnose or treat. Okay,

58:04

a lot of folks had

58:07

questions about this. Jack, Jennifer Alvarez,

58:09

Elise, Anna Thompson, Grace Lauren,

58:12

Rachel Thompson, Panic,

58:14

or Paynik, either way. Donald

58:17

McGregor, Pandora Two, Rebecca, Lynn Weisselberg,

58:20

Juliana, Aria Sal and Penny

58:22

Lee, and generic Nicky, all asked

58:25

about ADHD. Jack said very,

58:27

very plainly, I have ADHD.

58:30

What exactly is wrong with my neurotransmitters?

58:33

And so yeah, all those folks, and I'm curious about

58:35

it too, because sometimes I'm like, do I have ADHD? Maybe

58:38

I do. I don't know. So classically,

58:40

ADHD is described as a disruption of

58:42

the dopamine system. But I

58:45

think that there's a lot to be said for the

58:47

involvement of other neurotransmitters.

58:50

We like to talk about serotonin, but I actually

58:52

think that norepinephrine is more

58:54

relevant to ADHD specifically,

58:57

because norepinephrine is responsible

59:00

for attention and alertness.

59:03

So when your norepinephrine

59:05

system is working, you're

59:08

awake and you're alert. When

59:10

it's really activated, it's telling

59:12

you there is something you need to pay attention to right

59:14

now and be very awake and maybe run. You

59:17

never know when you may have to jam. And

59:19

so

59:21

the attention needed to perform and

59:23

complete a task

59:24

was associated with norepinephrine

59:27

sort of cycling. So there's

59:29

a lot and then there's not so much and then there's a

59:31

lot and then there's not so much. It's like just

59:33

enough to kind of keep you on

59:35

task and motivated. And

59:37

that motivation also comes from dopamine. But

59:40

when it's tonic, when it's just kind of like

59:42

at an okay level and just kind

59:44

of like plugging along, there's really no

59:47

reason for you to maintain attention

59:49

because it's not telling you to do

59:51

so. And so you're like,

59:53

huh, I want to feel good about something. And

59:55

then when you go looking in search of dopamine because

59:58

dopamine in ADHD.

59:59

people is a little bit lower. And so

1:00:02

they're constantly looking for stimulus

1:00:04

that's going to pop that up so that they can feel

1:00:06

good.

1:00:07

Oh, wow. Why do you think so many people

1:00:09

have ADHD or are getting

1:00:11

diagnosed with it? And I know so many people who

1:00:13

are diagnosed later in life, like, why do you think

1:00:15

it's so prevalent? I don't have

1:00:18

a good answer for that. There's a lot

1:00:20

of discussions people have about

1:00:22

our technology training us to have ADHD, about

1:00:25

the fact that we have declared a thing, kind

1:00:28

of promoting the diagnoses

1:00:31

of it, and not being

1:00:33

able to compare to 20 years ago to know if

1:00:35

it really is increasing in prevalence because

1:00:37

we've just started diagnosing it. This

1:00:39

is an argument that's used for a lot of things.

1:00:42

So I don't really have

1:00:44

a good answer for you. But

1:00:46

I think that it might be that we're just becoming

1:00:49

aware of our differences. And

1:00:51

ADHD is a way for us

1:00:53

to label those differences. Sometimes

1:00:56

that's a good thing. But it isn't

1:00:58

always. I know that there's a lot of really

1:01:00

smart, really, really energetic

1:01:03

and curious kids that get diagnosed with

1:01:06

ADHD. And it might just be because

1:01:08

we have a hard time handling

1:01:10

that level of energy and curiosity.

1:01:13

All right, quick aside here. We ended

1:01:15

up doing a three-part episode

1:01:18

on ADHD with the Russell Barkley.

1:01:21

We did it in 2022.

1:01:21

And people, it's

1:01:23

a banger. It's linked in the show notes.

1:01:26

Wow, changed my life. Also,

1:01:28

though, in 2019, I did look this up.

1:01:30

An adult ADHD diagnosis rose 123% between 2007

1:01:33

and 2016. And

1:01:38

the prevalence of ADHD in kids went up 26%.

1:01:41

So many researchers think it's just awareness

1:01:44

of symptoms that's driving more people to get

1:01:46

evaluated for it. And I know so many

1:01:49

folks with ADHD, some diagnosed

1:01:51

in adulthood that just wish they knew

1:01:53

sooner. I just want to

1:01:55

tell you that

1:01:56

in the process of writing this aside, Jarrett

1:01:58

was typing really loudly.

1:01:59

got his keyboard and I got distracted so

1:02:02

I went and got the headphones that I had lost

1:02:04

for about six months but just found and then in the process

1:02:06

I wandered into the kitchen to make a matcha latte

1:02:09

and then I sat back down but I got an email and

1:02:11

so I ended up checking my credit score for a while. Anyway,

1:02:13

okay, yes, ADHD awareness. It's up.

1:02:16

More people might have it than they realize.

1:02:18

And then what about treatment for ADHD?

1:02:21

I know like amphetamine salts are sometimes

1:02:24

prescribed. What is that doing to the dopamine

1:02:26

or what is that helping

1:02:28

level out? So when I

1:02:31

learned about this, which was a while

1:02:33

ago, we were talking about the

1:02:36

use of amphetamines in the concept of homeostasis.

1:02:39

So our entire system

1:02:41

is designed to keep us in a certain

1:02:44

region of activity and alertness

1:02:46

and awakeness. We want to maintain that

1:02:48

homeostasis

1:02:48

because when we get thrown out of it we get

1:02:50

disease and a lot of terrible things. And

1:02:53

so when you take an ADHD

1:02:56

brain and you give it amphetamine, you're

1:02:58

releasing a lot of norepinephrine, you're releasing

1:03:00

a lot of adrenaline, you're releasing a lot of dopamine

1:03:03

and that's like throwing you way up. So in

1:03:06

a way it's compensating for the things that you might not

1:03:08

have enough of but it's also telling

1:03:10

your body, hey,

1:03:11

as a complete system you better pay

1:03:13

attention to what's happening here because something has

1:03:15

gone crazy and it's forcing your system

1:03:18

to level you out. So

1:03:21

if you don't have those discrepancies

1:03:23

though, the lower level of dopamine,

1:03:26

then you've thrown your entire system into a whole other

1:03:28

solar system

1:03:34

which is for some people good.

1:03:36

That's why we love cocaine because we love

1:03:38

a lot of dopamine and we want that to hang around

1:03:40

for a while but

1:03:41

usually ends up in really,

1:03:43

really bad results. Right. But if you

1:03:46

are already low on

1:03:48

the dopamine then it just levels you off to where

1:03:51

maybe a neurotypical person might be. Oh,

1:03:53

that's fascinating.

1:03:55

So the first line of therapy

1:03:57

for ADHD is usually medication? Why is

1:03:59

that? Well, it works in up

1:04:01

to 80% of folks suffering with ADHD if

1:04:04

the dosage is right. But the best strategy,

1:04:06

doctors say, is combining strategies.

1:04:09

So exercise, some supplements like

1:04:12

fish oil and magnesium have

1:04:14

been shown in some studies to improve symptoms and

1:04:16

being around nature every day can also be effective.

1:04:19

Either way, there is no shame in the ADHD

1:04:21

game. Super common and there are

1:04:23

treatments out there. And yes, I want to do a whole

1:04:25

episode on this. But besides, everyone

1:04:28

wakes up and pours themselves

1:04:29

a piping hot cup of stimulants anyway,

1:04:32

right? But one of the interesting things about homeostasis

1:04:35

is that

1:04:36

it doesn't have to be, like it's something that our body

1:04:39

does naturally and it doesn't necessarily have

1:04:41

to be drug related. Although,

1:04:43

like the, there's a really

1:04:45

great story about homeostasis in coffee.

1:04:49

So if you go through the same morning routine,

1:04:53

when you wake up and you go down, you're about to press

1:04:55

the button on your coffee maker, make maybe

1:04:57

the sound of the coffee maker and the sound of

1:04:59

the coffee going into the pot or the cup. Your

1:05:01

body knows I'm about to get some caffeine.

1:05:04

So we'll depress its system in

1:05:06

anticipation of the stimulation

1:05:09

from caffeine. So that's why like replacing

1:05:12

your coffee with decaf is like

1:05:14

a really terrible trip to play on people

1:05:17

because you'll actually get

1:05:19

more depressed

1:05:22

than you would otherwise

1:05:23

because your body has depressed the system waiting

1:05:25

for the stimulant and then it has not gotten

1:05:27

it.

1:05:29

Fuck. Yeah. So

1:05:31

be really careful with your

1:05:33

routine. Oh my God. I

1:05:35

still say we use the routine we have. Caffeine

1:05:38

binds to the thing that makes you sleepy.

1:05:41

It takes the place. So

1:05:43

caffeine interacts with adenosine receptors

1:05:45

and adenosine receptors are just

1:05:48

kind of like open and waiting for the adenosine

1:05:50

to come and it comes and it binds

1:05:52

them. And if enough adenosine binds

1:05:54

enough of the receptors and it's like,

1:05:56

okay, we're sleepy now. We're

1:05:59

going to go to bed.

1:05:59

But the caffeine comes and it like sits

1:06:02

in that binding site and prevents the

1:06:04

adenosine from binding the receptor but

1:06:06

doesn't activate them. So the

1:06:09

adenosine can't get in and the

1:06:11

receptors like waiting for a signal that never comes.

1:06:13

The caffeine is like, haha, you are awake now

1:06:15

forever.

1:06:18

So yes, caffeine, it swoops in

1:06:20

and it takes the seat of the sleepy chemical,

1:06:22

kind of like musical chairs and it blocks

1:06:25

the snoozy feelings. What if

1:06:27

you are staring at the ceiling and

1:06:29

not even the fancy-nancy trick of

1:06:32

thinking of a category like fruits or cities

1:06:34

or Star Wars characters and then going down the alphabet

1:06:36

thinking of things in that category that start with each letter

1:06:39

is working. Is it best to gradually

1:06:42

taper off caffeine? Like

1:06:46

if you need to. If you needed

1:06:48

to, like why would you stop drinking coffee? I don't

1:06:50

understand the question. She

1:06:52

says with a coffee cup. I don't know.

1:06:55

Why would anyone do that? Okay, a lot of people

1:06:58

had questions about the genetic levels

1:07:00

of neurotransmitters

1:07:00

like Radley, Joe

1:07:03

Portfino, Corey Navis, Kinley Wallace,

1:07:05

Andrea, essentially asked,

1:07:08

you know, anxiety, depression, hereditary,

1:07:11

contagious. Radley asked, are

1:07:13

imbalances in neurotransmitters more likely

1:07:15

due to genetics or environment? Speaking

1:07:18

as someone with a whole slew of mental illnesses

1:07:20

and addictive behaviors in my family, including

1:07:22

myself.

1:07:23

And Radley, you're not alone. I feel like most

1:07:25

of us are probably in the same basket. Going

1:07:28

into Thanksgiving, everybody's going to

1:07:30

know that they're in a family of nuts. Absolutely.

1:07:33

We all are. All of

1:07:35

us. There's so many ways that neurotransmitter

1:07:38

levels can be affected. Definitely

1:07:40

genetics is one of them. Definitely

1:07:43

environment is another. And things that we're

1:07:45

temporarily going through can influence

1:07:47

it as well. So like if you've just

1:07:49

experienced a traumatic loss, you

1:07:52

are going to have differences in your

1:07:55

neurotransmitter release. But

1:07:57

that is temporary and it will eventually go back

1:07:59

to what we're doing. but for you as a normal level and you're

1:08:02

able to cope. But some people

1:08:04

that have genetic differences,

1:08:06

what does that even mean? It could mean we

1:08:09

produce different amounts of neurotransmitter.

1:08:11

It could mean that our receptors have different

1:08:14

responses to those neurotransmitter than

1:08:17

a neurotypical response. There's

1:08:19

so many different ways that the

1:08:21

amount or the reaction

1:08:24

to a neurotransmitter can be

1:08:26

affected by genetics or by environment.

1:08:29

So the answer is yes. Yes. That was a very long

1:08:31

yes answer. So genetics can

1:08:33

influence your neurotransmitter levels, for sure.

1:08:36

But before

1:08:36

you blame your parents for everything,

1:08:39

a whole bunch of factors are also

1:08:41

at play. So it's not you, fancy

1:08:43

Nancy. It's me

1:08:44

or dad or how much caffeine I drink or

1:08:47

maybe jet lag or the fact that I have a minute in the gym

1:08:49

in a month. Anyway, what about

1:08:51

SNRIs versus SSRIs?

1:08:53

I know Aurora, Heather Gentry, Gracie

1:08:56

Zeca, Leanne Shister, Rachel

1:08:59

Polivica, and Amelia H.

1:09:01

All wanted to know, do we know why different

1:09:04

SSRIs and SNRIs have different effects

1:09:06

on people? Amelia H.

1:09:08

wanted to know, is it just the molecular structure?

1:09:10

Heather Gentry is a first time question

1:09:12

asker. So is Gracie Zeca. And

1:09:15

they both kind of asked about increasing

1:09:18

numbers of atypical antidepressants. And

1:09:20

if the serotonin and depression model

1:09:23

is not correct, if it's bigger than

1:09:25

that. It's definitely bigger than that. OK.

1:09:28

It's definitely bigger than that, especially

1:09:31

when you're

1:09:31

talking about the interplay between

1:09:33

depression and anxiety. And

1:09:35

that's what I think of when I think of a combination

1:09:38

of SSRIs and SNRIs.

1:09:42

So

1:09:43

we're still talking about reuptake inhibitors.

1:09:45

We're still talking about the little

1:09:47

molecules that go around collecting the

1:09:49

neurotransmitters and shoving them back into

1:09:51

the cell that they originated from and waiting for the

1:09:53

next opportunity to release them and

1:09:56

inhibiting this process. So keeping those

1:09:59

neurotransmitters.

1:09:59

in the synapse longer so that you get a more

1:10:02

prolonged signal. Now we're talking

1:10:04

about changing the amount

1:10:07

of serotonin and norepinephrine

1:10:10

and titrating those differences. That's

1:10:12

why a lot of people have to try multiple different

1:10:14

combinations of drugs until they find

1:10:16

the one that works for them because their

1:10:19

problem might be more serotonin or

1:10:21

less. Serotonin might be more about norepinephrine

1:10:23

and if it's anxiety related, it probably is

1:10:26

or less. Yeah. I

1:10:28

wonder if that's why SSRIs

1:10:29

didn't do much for me but as a person with

1:10:32

generalized anxiety disorder, thanks very much.

1:10:35

And SNRI was helpful.

1:10:38

What is happening with the norepinephrine

1:10:41

when it comes to anxiety? Is

1:10:43

it going off? Is it? Oh yeah.

1:10:46

I mean like I was saying, norepinephrine is

1:10:48

keeping you awake and it's telling you what to focus

1:10:50

on. So with generalized anxiety disorder, not

1:10:52

only are you awake but you're constantly having to focus

1:10:55

on all the things that are chasing you. Your

1:10:59

attention is on all the things that

1:11:01

could potentially kill you because your brain's trying to

1:11:03

keep you alive but it thinks that everything is

1:11:05

trying to kill you so you have to pay attention to everything

1:11:08

and then there's all the things and it gets really overwhelming because

1:11:11

everything is trying to kill you and it's like living in

1:11:12

Australia but yet it's you know, this

1:11:16

is like a terrible cycle for brains to

1:11:18

get into. Australia. The

1:11:21

land of sharks and snakes and spiders

1:11:24

and angry kangaroos. I

1:11:26

guess an angry kangaroo too

1:11:28

who gives you just one star. Oh look kangaroo

1:11:31

too, I said your name again and you loved it.

1:11:33

I feel like perhaps you're

1:11:35

very empathetic to this particular

1:11:38

problem. It might be that I have experienced

1:11:40

that before. And so

1:11:43

does an SNRI, does it, what

1:11:45

exactly is it doing to norepinephrine if it's a

1:11:48

selective norepinephrine reuptake

1:11:50

inhibitor? Is it good to have

1:11:52

more norepinephrine between the cells? Yes,

1:11:55

it can be but it depends on the comparison

1:11:58

levels to the other neurotransmitters.

1:11:59

Okay. Right. So

1:12:02

you're trying to balance dopamine, serotonin, norepinephrine

1:12:05

and get that right cocktail so

1:12:07

that you get a harmony instead of a discordant

1:12:10

dysfunction, if that makes sense. That does

1:12:12

make sense. I didn't realize that SNRIs

1:12:14

and anxiety, that

1:12:16

could be a good link. I always thought,

1:12:19

if I've got anxiety, why do I want more goddamn

1:12:22

norepinephrine

1:12:22

and my synapses? I didn't. That

1:12:25

was me screaming in my own brain. Yeah.

1:12:29

So yes, SNRIs affect both the

1:12:31

norepinephrine and the serotonin and

1:12:33

it's the balance that can be helpful.

1:12:35

Although the first few weeks on an SNRI

1:12:38

can be rocky as hell and more stressy

1:12:40

as your brain adjusts and then becomes

1:12:43

more chill. So my brain asked

1:12:45

me to tell your brain that as

1:12:47

a heads up. Nicky, first time question asker,

1:12:49

asked, is the dopamine pathway activated when

1:12:52

you eat an

1:12:52

Oreo while studying like

1:12:54

it would be when you smoke a cigarette? What

1:12:58

is that Oreo question? Okay.

1:13:00

So I'm going to assume that the reason we're talking about Oreos

1:13:03

is because there is a paper that showed that mice

1:13:05

prefer Oreos to cocaine. And

1:13:07

then it was used in mainstream media

1:13:10

to promote many popular

1:13:12

but scientifically irrelevant headlines

1:13:15

like sugar is more addictive

1:13:17

than cocaine and a bunch

1:13:20

of

1:13:20

other things. So I'm just going

1:13:22

to substitute Oreo for

1:13:25

Parmesan goldfish, which is what I

1:13:27

eat when I'm studying. And

1:13:31

yeah, very, very different things. So we're

1:13:34

talking about sugar and carbohydrates and

1:13:36

feeding your brain in a certain way. There's

1:13:38

definitely dopamine release when you're

1:13:41

eating food because that's one of the things that are going to keep

1:13:43

you alive and that's what dopamine is there for.

1:13:46

Have we gotten this point enough? I'm not sure.

1:13:49

Let me reiterate it. Nicotine is released

1:13:51

when you encounter things that keep you alive. The

1:13:54

nicotine completely different. So

1:13:56

nicotine is a cognitive enhancer. So it's probably

1:13:58

helping your prefrontal.

1:13:59

cortex function and it's shown

1:14:02

to help decrease anxiety. So

1:14:05

it's probably interacting in your amygdala

1:14:07

to reduce stress and facilitating

1:14:10

better studying.

1:14:12

Okay.

1:14:13

Side note, just Google Oreo

1:14:15

plus cocaine, that study is

1:14:17

everywhere. It was cited by pretty much every news

1:14:20

outlet in the known universe. And a professor

1:14:22

who worked on the study stated in a 2013 press

1:14:25

release that he, quote, hadn't touched

1:14:27

an Oreo since the experiment. But

1:14:29

it's unclear if that's because

1:14:32

of their addictive implications or

1:14:34

just because watching rats pick

1:14:36

apart any food with their tiny

1:14:38

clawed feet for years on end tends to

1:14:40

kind of tarnish its appeal. Now,

1:14:42

speaking of

1:14:43

full little bellies. So this is

1:14:45

a good segue to

1:14:47

the gut biome and Libby

1:14:49

Miller, Brigid, Emma Hoch Schneider, Kaby

1:14:52

Mabey, Isabelle Christine Hottinger,

1:14:55

Kira Gowan, Mackenzie Campbell, generic Nicky,

1:14:57

Michelle Lees, Eileen, Mackenzie Campbell, Stefan

1:15:00

Williams, Jen Anathas, and Michelle Lee all

1:15:03

asked about

1:15:05

how many of our neurotransmitters are made

1:15:07

in our guts. And do we have any leads

1:15:09

yet on good foods for good neurotransmitters?

1:15:12

Christine Hottinger, that is, how do I eat

1:15:15

myself happy? Well,

1:15:18

actually, there's been a lot of recent studies

1:15:20

on the microbiome and the influence of

1:15:22

food on mood. And we've always known,

1:15:25

even before we identified that the microbiome

1:15:27

was a thing, that diet had a huge

1:15:29

impact on mood. And

1:15:32

of course, we have always talked about sort

1:15:34

of blood sugar

1:15:35

activity and how crashing

1:15:38

after a lot of sugar can influence

1:15:40

our mood and make us depressed. But

1:15:43

what I think that we're really asking about here

1:15:46

is the chemicals that are released by the

1:15:48

gut biome. One of those chemicals

1:15:50

has been shown to be serotonin,

1:15:52

which is like one of the really really big findings

1:15:55

in that field and like in the neuroscience field

1:15:57

too because we thought, oh,

1:15:59

neurotransmitters.

1:15:59

are synthesized in the neurons,

1:16:02

but I guess not always. I guess there

1:16:04

can be serotonin and potentially other neurotransmitters

1:16:07

just kind of floating around in your bloodstream.

1:16:09

Fancy meeting you here. Does it influence

1:16:12

mood? Yes, probably. Are

1:16:14

there particular super foods that

1:16:16

you can eat to raise your serotonin? Probably

1:16:20

not. But what we eat definitely

1:16:23

does influence the different types

1:16:25

of microorganisms and

1:16:27

the ratios of those microorganisms

1:16:29

in our gut. So

1:16:33

I can't tell her what to eat to make

1:16:35

herself happy, but if she finds

1:16:37

a particular type of diet that does

1:16:40

make her happy, she's not just – it's

1:16:42

not all placebo. Right. It's

1:16:44

not just in her head. It's in her gut. Yes, exactly.

1:16:48

I think that's so bananas that so much serotonin

1:16:51

is made in our simmering poo

1:16:54

tubes. Who knew? Who knew?

1:16:56

That's crazy. And I think about that too

1:16:58

sometimes when

1:16:59

I'm on a particular

1:17:02

binge of very, very unhealthy food

1:17:04

and I'm like, how long am I going to have to eat healthy

1:17:07

to readjust the ratios

1:17:09

of gut bacteria because I know that I'm

1:17:11

feeding it a certain type of sugar or just a

1:17:14

lot of sugar that there's

1:17:16

going to be overgrowth of one population

1:17:18

in my microbiome. Right.

1:17:21

And I'm apologizing to the potentially

1:17:24

more valuable and rarer bacteriums

1:17:27

in my gut. Like I'm sorry. I know I'm overfeeding

1:17:29

that.

1:17:29

You know. I feel like if you like

1:17:32

SimCity, you'll love the

1:17:34

gut biome. Oh, yes. That's so

1:17:36

true. It's like real world consequences.

1:17:39

For more on this topic, you can see last

1:17:42

November's microbiology episode with

1:17:44

Dr. Elaine Shao, who herself says

1:17:46

she tries to eat a varied diet. So

1:17:49

give your microbes natural foods that

1:17:51

would help them thrive, i.e. not

1:17:53

Oreos or cocaine, which was

1:17:56

in soft drinks until the early

1:17:58

1900s, which is just

1:18:00

bananas. Okay, speaking of guzzling

1:18:03

up, a few people asked about alcohol. Lindsay

1:18:05

DeFalco, Amelia H. Anna Thompson and Emmanuel

1:18:07

Sanchez asked what's going on in the brain with different

1:18:09

drugs and controlled substances like

1:18:12

alcohol. And Amelia H. wants to know why

1:18:14

is alcoholism an inheritable trait? Oh,

1:18:18

alcohol is so interesting because there's

1:18:20

no like alcohol receptor. Oh,

1:18:23

like it doesn't act on a particular

1:18:25

receptor the way that

1:18:28

I was describing acetylcholine and

1:18:30

nicotine. It like sort of cozies

1:18:33

up to the receptor and it's like soft

1:18:36

influence. Like we would call it allosteric

1:18:38

modulation. Okay. So it doesn't like

1:18:40

bind to the receptor and cause the

1:18:43

receptor to do anything, but it affects

1:18:45

the way that the receptor responds to

1:18:47

the molecules that it's really supposed to

1:18:49

be talking to. So it can

1:18:52

make it open like easier.

1:18:53

So it needs like less

1:18:56

drug or less neurotransmitter before it responds.

1:19:00

So it's very like sneaky and insidious

1:19:02

in terms of the activity in the brain. Alcohol

1:19:05

has, as anyone that has been

1:19:07

drunk, like, you know, it affects your motor

1:19:10

control in your muscles as well. So has more

1:19:12

than just brain effects,

1:19:14

but in the brain it acts in

1:19:17

that allosteric sort of soft power

1:19:20

kind of a way. And does it kind of mess

1:19:23

with frontal

1:19:23

cortex activity? Like does it

1:19:25

in terms of like loss of inhibition

1:19:28

and maybe less control over emotion?

1:19:31

Yeah, it

1:19:33

disinhibits the

1:19:35

inhibitory neurons. Okay.

1:19:38

It's like the act of alcohol is

1:19:40

a double negative. Okay. So it works

1:19:42

on your inhibitory neurons.

1:19:44

Oh, okay. That's

1:19:47

why I won't do anything. So they're normally like on

1:19:49

like I'm inhibiting and I'm doing my job

1:19:51

and then alcohol is like

1:19:53

take a break. Oh no. Yeah,

1:19:55

yeah. Crystal says the alcohol

1:19:57

affects dopamine, serotonin.

1:19:59

GABA, which is a neurotransmitter

1:20:02

that helps maintain calm, and glutamate

1:20:04

pathways, which affect memory. But

1:20:07

just as your college roommate may have just

1:20:09

lived for Friday Jager shots and

1:20:11

you have never finished a beer, different

1:20:13

people have different genetics that

1:20:16

influence how receptors respond to alcohol.

1:20:18

But the main point is it doesn't have to just

1:20:20

affect dopamine to become addictive. And

1:20:23

scientists, they're still figuring out how it all works.

1:20:26

Neuroscience,

1:20:26

it's complicated. Who knew? All

1:20:29

of us, literally all of us. Okay, now

1:20:31

on the topic of substances, a

1:20:33

bunch of people did ask about recreational

1:20:36

drugs.

1:20:38

Jes Boiseta Garcia, Rebecca Landry,

1:20:40

Joe Portofino, Jimin Alonzo,

1:20:42

Kevin List, James Bullio, Cassie,

1:20:45

Kerry Bringham, all kind of asked,

1:20:48

hey, what's going on with recreational or

1:20:50

ritualistic drugs? Like Ayahuasca,

1:20:54

Kevin List asked, what are your thoughts on microdosing

1:20:56

for mental health issues like depression? And

1:20:59

Jes wanted to know, flim flam or not?

1:21:01

Is psilocybin an effective treatment

1:21:03

for medication resistant psychiatric

1:21:06

conditions? So what's going on with

1:21:08

magical things? Magical

1:21:10

things are

1:21:11

like, it's if one aspect

1:21:14

of the orchestra went completely,

1:21:16

like we came on steroids. If

1:21:19

you showed up and there was like 37 cellos.

1:21:26

And like four of all

1:21:29

of the other instruments, whatever that

1:21:31

would sound like, that's kind of what recreational

1:21:33

drugs do. They put things completely

1:21:36

out of balance and

1:21:39

we experience a new

1:21:42

reality through that lens. Like

1:21:44

brains are basically making a guess

1:21:47

at our realities anyway. And so we

1:21:49

experienced a brain's best guess

1:21:51

at what is actually happening right

1:21:54

now. Your whole reality is

1:21:56

just a picture

1:21:56

that your brain has painted based

1:21:59

on what it's

1:21:59

sensing. How weird is that? What

1:22:02

is even real? And so when the

1:22:05

predictions of the brain or the way

1:22:07

that the system that is the brain tries to anticipate

1:22:10

or interpret these completely

1:22:13

out of whack situation,

1:22:15

that's when we get the fun that is

1:22:17

recreational drugs. What happens

1:22:20

with psychedelics? Is it a particular neurotransmitter

1:22:22

that is just going off? A lot

1:22:24

of them are

1:22:26

acting on the serotonin system. Okay.

1:22:29

So serotonin is like

1:22:31

when it's sort of a mod, it's a modulation.

1:22:34

It's more global than a lot of

1:22:36

the other neurotransmitters, I would

1:22:38

say. And so when you've got like a bunch of serotonin

1:22:40

like dumped into the system, you have a lot

1:22:43

of different brain regions that are all like trying

1:22:45

to cope with life. Is that why people will take supplements

1:22:48

like five HTP after they will

1:22:51

do like Molly or something

1:22:53

like that? Yeah, you can deplete the amount because

1:22:55

your brain is synthesizing those molecules.

1:22:57

There's a limited number of them. If you think about

1:22:59

a factory production line, it only goes

1:23:01

so fast so you can only produce so

1:23:03

many toys or so many cars, or you can only

1:23:05

produce so many molecules of

1:23:08

your particular neurotransmitter. And so

1:23:10

if you have taken Molly

1:23:13

or one of these recreational drugs that has dumped

1:23:16

a whole bunch of neurotransmitter into your brain

1:23:18

and you've been like backstroking through those happy

1:23:20

molecules for a while. Oh, the water

1:23:22

is great. It's

1:23:25

time for your brain to go back to normal because it's

1:23:27

no longer getting those signals. There's

1:23:29

like it has nothing left to give literally.

1:23:31

And so giving it some precursors

1:23:34

for the molecules that it needs to replenish is

1:23:36

sort of a way of helping it get back

1:23:39

to normal because you're skipping a few steps

1:23:41

in the assembly line. Got

1:23:43

it. So you're not left like high and

1:23:45

dry, like literally high and

1:23:48

actually dry of the good brain

1:23:50

juices. Now this next topic was

1:23:52

on the minds of patrons. John Arokvik,

1:23:55

Graham Tattersall, Maria, generic Nicky,

1:23:57

Sydney Manzill, Dawn Ewald, first.

1:23:59

Time mindfulness question asker,

1:24:02

Jennifer Tran, and first time question asker, Ashley

1:24:04

Beatty, who wondered about the impact of meditation

1:24:06

on anxiety and depression specifically. And

1:24:09

now what about meditation, yoga,

1:24:11

things like that? Do

1:24:14

you ever use any of it? Do

1:24:16

you feel like you should be using it? I think

1:24:18

that it's definitely a good place to start. I'm

1:24:21

one of those people that

1:24:22

is like, why would you pay for gym membership

1:24:24

if you could just go outside and run? And then

1:24:26

I just stay inside and watch Netflix the entire

1:24:28

time. And that's kind of how I feel

1:24:30

about mindfulness.

1:24:33

Like it's something that you can do quite easily. You

1:24:36

know that there's positive effects. Like there's been

1:24:38

scientific papers that have shown that there are positive

1:24:40

effects of meditation practice,

1:24:43

of mindfulness practice that really does

1:24:45

help quiet some of the overactivity

1:24:48

and the amygdala that we see in like Western

1:24:50

society, for instance. So

1:24:52

why not do it? I

1:24:55

don't do it. I

1:24:58

probably should. It would definitely

1:25:00

help me a lot. So do

1:25:02

as I say, not as I do. Okay, doctor. Okay,

1:25:06

what is your least favorite thing about neuroscience,

1:25:09

about brains

1:25:12

or about your life as a doctor

1:25:15

brain, essentially on TV

1:25:17

and all over the world? Neuroscience

1:25:20

is really hard to do without actually

1:25:23

touching the tissue that you're trying to study.

1:25:25

And so we

1:25:28

use a lot of model brains in order

1:25:30

to learn the things that we learn,

1:25:33

which is really challenging because

1:25:35

a lot of the, even the information that

1:25:37

I was sharing with you today, like we know this

1:25:39

to be true for mice and

1:25:41

rats. And we assume that it is

1:25:44

also true for humans

1:25:46

to the best of our possible ability.

1:25:49

But

1:25:50

as far as I know, we aren't able to like

1:25:53

do the same types of experiments on

1:25:55

humans. So a lot of we

1:26:00

know is inferred. Right.

1:26:03

Would you ever donate your brain to science?

1:26:06

What do you think? I

1:26:08

think I would be a terrible

1:26:10

test subject. I'm

1:26:13

always the outlier. I never

1:26:16

feel that I am a good representation

1:26:19

of the mathematical average of

1:26:21

a human anything. I

1:26:23

feel like my brain would give like

1:26:26

wrong data or like not accurate

1:26:30

data. And I think that actually speaks

1:26:32

to there was an earlier question about like,

1:26:34

why don't we know how these things work?

1:26:36

And it's like, well, we can know things pretty

1:26:39

accurately for a particular breed

1:26:42

of mouse or particular breed of rat because they're

1:26:44

all exactly the same. They're all clones of each other.

1:26:46

So it's really easy for us to know what's going on there.

1:26:48

We can't clone humans. We can't

1:26:51

do research on humans. So all

1:26:53

of the genetic background, all of the environmental

1:26:56

differences,

1:26:56

all of those things mean that

1:26:58

we're really just kind of guessing at what's going to work

1:27:01

for the average population. Isn't

1:27:03

it kind of crazy that we just have clones, like animal

1:27:05

clones running around? Is that kind of weird?

1:27:07

Is that ever creepy out? It doesn't really

1:27:10

creep me out, but I guess because I mostly

1:27:12

work with bacteria and with mice

1:27:15

and they're not,

1:27:16

it's easy to not see

1:27:19

them as necessarily having personalities.

1:27:22

I guess, but I never, I never raised

1:27:25

mice. I was a beneficiary of

1:27:27

people that did mouse experiments, but

1:27:29

I never actually had a colony of mice

1:27:31

that I was raising. And I know that neuroscientists

1:27:34

that do work directly with live

1:27:36

behaving animals would absolutely tell

1:27:38

me that I'm crazy, but they have, they

1:27:41

have personalities and differences, even

1:27:43

though genetically they're the same. Wow.

1:27:46

Do you hear that

1:27:47

Barbara Streisand cloned her dog and she thought

1:27:49

she was getting one and they're like, well, you're four. And she's

1:27:51

like, fuck. She's just like, oh no.

1:27:54

She'd like give it away to like her assistant's daughter

1:27:56

or something. She's like, I didn't, I didn't think I'd get

1:27:58

four of them. It's true.

1:27:59

Barbra Streisand missed her dog, Samantha, so

1:28:02

much that she had four more made

1:28:04

from a swab of her cheek. Now the

1:28:06

runt of that litter sadly died, but

1:28:09

she kept two of the other ones and the third

1:28:11

she says, the 13-year-old daughter of my

1:28:13

A&R man bonded with one of the clones, so

1:28:15

I gave them that puppy. So there you go, clones, they're

1:28:18

all over the place. It is just like not

1:28:20

a biggie. Shrug. What's your favorite

1:28:22

thing about your job or neuroscience

1:28:25

or the brain?

1:28:26

I mean,

1:28:27

I think that we are inherently selfish

1:28:30

and that we really like to know things about ourselves.

1:28:34

And neurosciences is kind of like my

1:28:36

way of trying to understand this like

1:28:38

human condition. So,

1:28:40

you know, brains are where they're really

1:28:43

intense and they want everything to have meaning

1:28:46

and they will ascribe meaning to

1:28:48

things that really there is no

1:28:51

purpose to. And so I think that's

1:28:53

probably just like what I'm trying to do with my

1:28:56

meaningless life is to

1:28:57

figure out, you know, why

1:29:00

humans? And why

1:29:02

me? That's the best

1:29:04

title for a biography. Why

1:29:07

humans? Why us? Oh,

1:29:09

as for neuroscience movies, Crystal says

1:29:12

pretty much none of them get

1:29:14

it right. Like none. And they

1:29:16

all try to make things way too spiritual

1:29:19

and that using only 10% of your brain is

1:29:21

a big hairy smelly myth. And

1:29:23

that the Scarlett Johansson vehicle Lucy was

1:29:26

wall to wall flim flam

1:29:27

and a griches. So she thinks writers

1:29:30

and directors should just focus on the real neuroscience

1:29:32

because it's bananas and it's mysterious

1:29:34

enough.

1:29:35

Reality is stranger than fiction.

1:29:38

And so let's figure out what's actually going on

1:29:40

and how can we tell that story in

1:29:43

an epic but accurate way because

1:29:45

it really is enough to blow your mind. Right.

1:29:48

Your actual mind. Thank you

1:29:50

so much, Dr. Delworth. Thank you for having

1:29:53

me. Okay, so now that you are fully enchanted

1:29:55

by the knowledge of Dr. Brain Crystal

1:29:57

Delworth, you could head to Crystal Delworth.

1:29:59

for links to her social

1:30:02

media and her LinkedIn page. There is

1:30:04

a link to that in the show notes of this episode. And special

1:30:06

thank you to Casey Hanmer for making sure that

1:30:08

she got that domain name. That's crystaldillworth.com.

1:30:11

So go there and follow her on Twitter

1:30:14

and on LinkedIn and on Instagram.

1:30:17

You can also check out Mission Unstoppable on

1:30:19

CBS every Saturday. Now links

1:30:21

will be up at allyward.com slash ologies

1:30:24

slash molecular neurobiology,

1:30:26

including to the charity supported and to the sponsors

1:30:29

making that possible.

1:30:29

We are at ologies on Instagram

1:30:32

and Twitter. I'm at Allie Ward with one

1:30:34

L on both. If you have a picture of yourself in

1:30:37

merch on Merch Mondays, we repost it.

1:30:39

So just hashtag it, ologies merch. I'm

1:30:42

also on CBS every Saturday morning

1:30:44

on Innovation Nation with Mooraka. And

1:30:46

I have my own science show on CW

1:30:49

called Did I Mention Invention, which is on Saturday

1:30:51

or Sunday, depending on where you live in the country. And thank

1:30:53

you to Aaron Talbert for adminning the Facebook

1:30:55

Ologies podcast group and for being amazing

1:30:58

people. Also thank you to Bonnie Dutch and Shannon

1:31:00

Feltes of the comedy podcast You Are That for

1:31:02

handling merch at ologiesmerch.com

1:31:05

and also for being wonderful. Transcripts

1:31:07

and bleeped episodes are at allyward.com

1:31:09

slash ologies dash extras. There'll

1:31:12

be a link in the show notes. Thank you to all the Ologies

1:31:14

transcribers and the Ologies transcribers Facebook group

1:31:17

and Emily White for working on those. And assistant

1:31:19

editing was done by Jared Sleeper of MindJam

1:31:21

Media and the mental health podcast

1:31:23

My Good Bad Brain. He talks about ADHD

1:31:25

a lot on that. So check out My Good Bad Brain. And

1:31:28

thanks, as always, to the brain that stitches

1:31:29

all these pieces together each week, Stephen

1:31:32

Ray Morris, who also hosts the percast about

1:31:34

cats and the dino podcast See Jurassic Right.

1:31:36

And just a little fun fact from 2023, if you're like, wait, Dr.

1:31:40

Dilworth, she has the same last name as Noelle

1:31:42

Dilworth, who works on the show. That is

1:31:44

because, yes, after this episode, I

1:31:46

hired Noelle. She works on the show. She's the best. OK.

1:31:49

The theme music was written and performed by Nick Thorburn of

1:31:51

the band Islands. And that cello music you heard

1:31:53

was the cello song by The Piano Guys.

1:31:55

And they're on YouTube. Now, if you stick around until

1:31:57

the end of the episode, you know I tell you a secret.

1:31:59

My secret is that I had a nightmare that

1:32:02

I was getting shot to space. I was like, oh, I'm an

1:32:04

astronaut, I guess. And beforehand they had

1:32:06

to weigh

1:32:07

everything that went into or came

1:32:09

out of my body. Let's just say it was a little too

1:32:11

close for comfort in my dream. And I woke up so

1:32:13

relieved that I didn't have to pee in a bucket in front

1:32:15

of anyone. Also another secret,

1:32:18

I actually do keep candles in

1:32:20

my wallet because honestly it happened so

1:32:22

often. It's someone's birthday and just being

1:32:24

able to shove a candle and like a piece of

1:32:26

toast or a Snickers, it's such a daymaker.

1:32:29

So they don't take up a bunch of room. I just put

1:32:31

like two or three wrapped up in a little

1:32:33

piece of tinfoil and I wedge them in my wallet.

1:32:36

But I'm pretty sure it looks like something illegal,

1:32:38

but I promise keep a few birthday candles

1:32:40

in your bag and you're gonna use them sooner than you

1:32:42

think. They come in handy all the time. Also, does

1:32:45

NASA even make you pee in buckets or did

1:32:47

I just make that up in a dream? Let me know, okay,

1:32:50

bye bye.

1:32:50

Pack a dermatology, audiology, cryptozoology,

1:32:54

lithology, nanotechnology,

1:32:57

meteorology, phytology,

1:33:00

phytology, seriology,

1:33:03

phytology.

1:33:11

Hey, I came here to be drugged, electrocuted,

1:33:14

and probed, not insulted.

1:33:17

When it comes to hummus, delicious

1:33:19

is in the details and Boer's head's

1:33:21

collection of exceptional hummus flavors is

1:33:23

in a class of its own, expertly

1:33:25

crafted to achieve the perfect balance of

1:33:28

creamy texture and refined taste.

1:33:30

From flavorful roasted red pepper to

1:33:33

decadent dark chocolate hummus, made

1:33:35

for you to savor every delicious

1:33:37

scoop. This is hummus so extraordinary

1:33:40

it can only be from Boer's head. Compromise

1:33:43

elsewhere.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features