Podchaser Logo
Home
How biodiversity contributes to human health and more…

How biodiversity contributes to human health and more…

Released Friday, 24th November 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
How biodiversity contributes to human health and more…

How biodiversity contributes to human health and more…

How biodiversity contributes to human health and more…

How biodiversity contributes to human health and more…

Friday, 24th November 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

Sick Boy podcast is a health and comedy

0:02

show about what it's like to be sick. Wait, is that

0:04

right? How can illness be funny? You'd be

0:06

surprised Okay, Sick Boy is hosted

0:09

by me Brian Stever and me Taylor

0:11

McGilvary and myself Jeremy Saunders every week

0:13

We're bringing you three new podcasts that

0:15

amazingly tie together illness vulnerability Cutting-edge

0:18

medical science and downright silly best friend

0:20

humor Come on in and join us every Monday Wednesday

0:23

and Friday to melt your heart learn something fascinating

0:25

and bust a belly laugh Trust us. You'll

0:27

be glad you did you can find Sick Boy on the CBC.

0:30

Listen

0:30

app or wherever you get your pods

0:32

This is a CBC

0:35

podcast

0:41

Hi, I'm Bob McDonald welcome

0:43

to quirks and quarks on this week's

0:45

show don't eat red

0:47

snow either Biologists

0:49

investigate the climate implications of

0:51

what they call watermelon snow.

0:53

Well, it's a growth of Microorganisms

0:57

primarily algae and in this

0:59

instance It's happening on the surface of the snow

1:01

and this bat has an intimidating

1:04

organ, but it's not for penetration

1:07

To observe this penis for the first time is really

1:10

impressive You you will have this

1:12

very long and also very wide

1:15

Penis, especially at

1:17

the terminal part plus

1:20

stripes turn out to be slimming because

1:22

spiders won't eat them What

1:24

it's like to have a third arm in your hands

1:27

and how biodiversity drives

1:29

nature's Pharmaceutical innovations

1:31

all this today on quirks

1:34

and quarks Let's

1:36

say you saw an animal with long powerful

1:39

legs You might reasonably

1:41

think it was adapted for running fast Or

1:44

if it had broad pectoral flippers

1:46

or web paws You'd assume it was

1:48

a proficient swimmer or

1:49

if it had sharp stabbing teeth, probably

1:52

a ferocious hunter So

1:55

what if you saw an animal with an impressively

1:57

long and agile penis?

2:00

Well, I'm sure you might assume certain

2:03

things, but it turns out

2:05

your assumptions might be wrong. Well,

2:08

mostly wrong. The male

2:10

of this species does use its penis

2:12

for sex, but not in the way you

2:14

might expect. This story starts

2:16

when Dr. Nicola Fazl, a bat

2:19

researcher working at the University of Lausanne

2:21

in Switzerland, got an unusual email

2:23

about a very large penis. Now,

2:26

normally you wouldn't open an email like

2:29

that, but then he realized it

2:31

was from a science-minded bat enthusiast

2:33

in the Netherlands. We'll let him

2:36

take it from here.

2:37

Dr. Fazl, hello and welcome to our program.

2:40

Okay, hello. First

2:42

of all, tell me about this email that

2:44

you clearly clicked on, otherwise we wouldn't

2:46

be talking here today. What about it caught your attention?

2:49

I received this email. First I

2:51

was thinking, okay, it may be a spam,

2:55

but then I thought, okay, it's

2:57

about Eptesicus, which is the

2:59

Latin name for the species we were studying.

3:03

I thought, okay, that will be too targeted

3:05

for a spam. So I decided still to take

3:07

the risk to open the link. Then

3:09

I found those amazing videos of

3:12

a compilation of these bats. We

3:14

could see that the bats

3:17

have a very large penis

3:19

when it's erect. Okay,

3:22

so we're talking about a species of bat here.

3:25

What species is it?

3:26

It's called a common

3:29

serotonin. It's a kind

3:31

of big bat, one of the biggest in Europe, in

3:33

Switzerland. It loves to

3:36

live in buildings, so it's

3:38

very, very fond of churches. And

3:42

it loves also to eat

3:44

some big flying beetles. Now,

3:47

I'm going to read a sentence from your scientific paper.

3:49

It reads, quote, with the erect

3:52

organs seven times longer and

3:54

wider than the vagina, its possible

3:56

function becomes a perplexing question.

3:59

Yes, indeed. So tell

4:02

me about this bat penis. To

4:04

observe this penis for the first time is really

4:06

impressive. You will have this

4:09

very long and also very wide

4:12

penis, especially at the terminal

4:15

part. And the terminal part

4:17

is really interesting because it has the

4:20

shape of a heart, but turn,

4:23

so the two lobes in front are

4:26

quite difficult to imagine to penetrate.

4:30

How long is it? So

4:32

the penis, when

4:35

directed, it's 1.6 centimeters,

4:38

so almost 2 centimeters,

4:40

and this is quite big. This will represent

4:43

one-fifth or one-quarter of

4:45

the bat size.

4:46

So besides its size, that heart shape doesn't

4:49

sound like it's designed for penetration. Exactly,

4:51

exactly. That was the big question

4:54

we had, is that how can you penetrate

4:56

anything with this shape? Well

4:59

before you embarked on this study, how much

5:01

did we actually know about how bats copulate?

5:04

Very little. So

5:06

bats normally copulate during

5:09

very secretive moments, so they can be in

5:11

caves or in the darkest

5:13

place of a house. So it's

5:16

very difficult to observe any copulation.

5:19

We were super lucky to have Jan

5:22

putting and fitting so many videos

5:24

inside the church and also having our Ukraine

5:27

colleagues who

5:29

worked with bats in cages and

5:31

who could witness those copulations.

5:35

Okay, so once you looked at all of these

5:37

videos, what did you see? Give

5:39

me a play-by-play of how these

5:41

bats copulate. So you

5:44

will see a female first, and

5:46

then you have this male that will just arrive

5:48

and try to kind of jump

5:51

on her, so both are crawling,

5:54

and the male will start to bite

5:57

the nap of the female. Once

6:00

a female is fixed, he will actually

6:02

go with a very mobile penis

6:06

around the tail membrane. Once

6:09

it has found the place,

6:11

he will push against

6:13

his vulva and

6:16

stay there for quite a long time. How

6:18

long? So, that was quite

6:21

a surprise. We had a median

6:23

time, not the average,

6:26

but the middle one, which

6:28

was the most one hour but then

6:30

we had big champions staying in the

6:33

corporation for more than 12 hours.

6:36

12 hours? Well, what's

6:38

the male penis doing during all

6:40

of this time? So the male penis

6:42

stays in front of the vulva,

6:45

sometimes he's pressing more or less

6:47

and then he's just staying

6:50

in contact with the vulva. In

6:53

your paper, you describe the bad penis like

6:55

an arm reaching under the female. So

6:57

you have to imagine that the female,

7:00

like the male, they have a tail membrane. So

7:03

between their legs, they have a membrane that they

7:05

can use to cover their lower

7:07

part and then you have the male

7:09

that has to bypass this membrane

7:12

and use actually this huge penis

7:15

as an arm. On the video that we

7:17

could get, you can clearly see

7:20

the male moving his penis and moving away

7:22

the

7:22

tail membrane in order to reach the vulva.

7:25

So it's sort of like opening curtains to appear on the other

7:27

side. Exactly, you got it. I

7:30

guess if it can go on for 12 hours,

7:32

there must be something in it for her.

7:35

Yes, and probably actually

7:37

it is the way to test the

7:39

capacity of the male. If the male can stay

7:42

as long as 12 hours or more,

7:45

it will be a way for her

7:47

to say, okay, this one is really into

7:49

it and it's not just coming and going

7:52

away. But if there's no penetration

7:54

involved here, what's your

7:56

best guess on how the semen is actually getting

7:59

into the vagina? We quite often

8:01

believe that the male part

8:03

is just important and that

8:05

the female genital tract is just

8:08

a vase with a passive egg that

8:10

is just floating around and waiting for the spermatozoa.

8:13

But I guess we forget about all

8:16

the mechanisms from the female side to

8:18

actually move also physically the

8:20

spermatozoa towards the fertilization

8:23

side. For example, in birds,

8:25

you don't have except in the xanostrins

8:28

maybe, but you don't have a penis

8:30

and birds are just touching their cloaca

8:33

together and the sperm is just transferred from

8:35

one cloaca to the other and

8:38

apparently it's working perfectly fine. So

8:41

perhaps it's like sort of an internal

8:43

pump within the female that can draw the

8:46

semen in? I think more than possibly.

8:50

The female genital tract is known

8:52

to move the sperms

8:54

towards the fertilization sides.

8:58

So putting all of this together, what

9:01

insight does this give you about where

9:03

bats fit in when it comes to the evolution of copulation?

9:06

We know already that bats are very deep,

9:08

very unique. They live super long

9:11

in terms of immunity. They have

9:13

lots of disease and viruses,

9:15

but they do perfectly fine. So

9:17

there are many aspects that are quite unique and

9:19

I guess with reproduction, we

9:22

have also something that is quite amazing.

9:24

So for example, it's known that bats

9:27

have the possibility to restore sperm for

9:29

several months. They can delay

9:31

ovulation, they can delay the implantation

9:34

or the embryonic development.

9:37

So yeah, they are really unique in

9:39

many aspects and apparently in copulation,

9:41

they are also quite strange.

9:44

Dr. Fazl, thank you so much for your time. Thank

9:46

you very much. Dr. Nikola Fazl

9:48

is a bat researcher working at the University

9:51

of Lausanne in Switzerland. If

9:53

you want to see the video Dr. Fazl

9:55

mentioned, shame on you. I

9:57

mean, you can find it on our website at...

9:59

at cbc.ca slash parks.

10:11

If you are wondering what the fashionably

10:14

cautious insect will be wearing this

10:16

season, it seems that black

10:18

and white stripes may be de rigueur.

10:21

It turns out dressing up a little and

10:23

especially adding some contrast can

10:26

turn predators' heads. And

10:28

surprisingly, that's not a bad thing.

10:31

In fact, it may explain why the striped

10:33

pattern is so common in nature. At

10:36

least that's what a new study looking

10:38

at hungry jumping spiders and

10:41

stylish termites seems to show. Dr.

10:44

Lisa Taylor, a behavioral ecologist

10:46

at the University of Florida in Gainesville was

10:48

part of the team. Dr. Taylor, welcome

10:50

to Quirks and Quarks. Thanks for having me. Let's

10:53

begin with the jumping spider. Tell me a little

10:55

bit about them.

10:57

So jumping spiders are everywhere. They're found

10:59

worldwide. They are voracious little predators.

11:02

So they're in everybody's backyards and gardens.

11:04

Sometimes they even wander into your house. They're

11:07

in agricultural fields. They're pretty much everywhere you

11:09

can imagine.

11:10

And do they really jump?

11:11

They do. Yep, they jump. So

11:13

they can walk a little bit to get around, but when they're moving fast,

11:15

they jump. So you often see them jumping through the leaflet

11:18

or jumping across the ground.

11:19

And what do they prey on? Pretty

11:22

much everything. So everything that's smaller than

11:24

them. And then also sometimes things that are a little bit

11:26

bigger than them.

11:28

Wow. Well, what is it about the jumping

11:30

spider that got your attention? Do

11:32

you want to study the relationship between what

11:35

it sees and what it eats?

11:37

So just really interesting. They have eight

11:39

eyes. Two of those eight eyes are really

11:41

big and positioned in the front of their face. And

11:43

those are the ones that in some species have

11:45

color vision, but they also have these eyes on the side

11:47

of their head that help them detect motion. So

11:50

they're just really interesting. And so I've just always

11:53

been interested in understanding what they see,

11:55

how they see, and how they use

11:57

the information that they see to make decisions. So they've

11:59

got information. coming in through all those eight

12:01

eyes and then they have to figure

12:02

out what to do with it somewhere in their brain.

12:04

Now what about this issue of

12:07

contrasting black and white?

12:09

We're particularly interested in the black and

12:11

white stripe patterns because it's something that you see all

12:13

over nature. So a lot of animals will pick

12:15

up toxins from their food or produce

12:18

toxins to protect them from predators. And

12:20

a lot of animals that do this also advertise their toxicity

12:22

using bright colors or sometimes patterns.

12:25

And so black and white stripes are really common

12:27

way that animals that are toxic advertise their toxicity.

12:30

And so we're really interested in finding

12:32

out how jumping spiders respond to those

12:34

straight

12:34

patterns. Kind of as a way to understand the

12:36

way that jumping spiders see and interpret

12:39

prey, whether that has contributed to the evolution

12:41

of those straight patterns. I think those straight patterns work really

12:43

well so that it can protect a lot of insects

12:46

and other small invertebrates from

12:48

getting eaten by jumping spiders in particular.

12:50

Well tell me about your setup. How did you test

12:53

this?

12:54

Yeah, I've been testing this idea. We actually took

12:56

small termites and we put these little paper

12:58

capes on them.

12:59

And so the benefits of doing things this way is that

13:01

we can basically manipulate the color patterns as

13:03

different termites to understand how

13:05

those color patterns influence the jumping spiders

13:08

decisions to attack or not.

13:09

Paper capes?

13:11

Paper capes, yeah. So it's

13:13

a pretty low tech setup. We basically just

13:15

pinched out these little oval pieces of paper. They

13:18

either were white, solid white, solid black,

13:20

or had black and white stripes on them. And then

13:23

we took a little bit of Elmer's glue and we just adhered them

13:25

to the back of the termites. Because termites

13:27

during soon stages of their development actually have

13:29

wings, like really big wings, the

13:31

termites were able to move around just fine with

13:33

these little capes on. And so we put them in

13:36

a dish. We let them wander around. And

13:38

then we introduced a spider. We gave each

13:40

spider

13:40

the choice of two black, two

13:42

white,

13:42

or two striped termites all moving around them. And

13:45

then we looked at the spider and saw which one got their attention

13:47

the fastest. And then we let the spiders

13:49

actually attack the termites and see which ones

13:52

they chose to attack.

13:53

Using Elmer's glue to stick paper

13:56

capes on the back of the termite. It sounds like

13:58

a science-based technique. experiment,

14:00

an inquisitive 10-year-old would

14:03

do. Yeah, there's a lot of things you

14:05

can learn about the natural world using pretty low-tech

14:07

methods.

14:08

So what was the spider's reaction to these three

14:10

different choices?

14:11

Yeah, so what I also didn't mention

14:13

is that termites are wandering around on a white background. And

14:16

so not surprisingly, the white

14:19

ones kind of blended into the white background. And so

14:21

both the black ones and the striped ones seem to get the

14:24

spider's attention the quickest. And that's not

14:26

super surprising because those are the ones that were the most

14:28

highly contrasting with the background. What was

14:30

interesting was that the ones that had the stripes were

14:32

attacked at the lowest rates compared to either the black or the

14:34

white. So that suggests that the

14:37

contrast with the background is what's needed to get

14:39

the attention. And then once they oriented to the

14:41

termites and looked at them kind of face

14:43

on with those big eyes in the front, they used the pattern

14:46

to decide what to eat. And so anything with stripes,

14:48

they didn't want to eat. So

14:50

it suggests they're using the stripes to

14:52

make a decision not to attack.

14:54

Why would it not attack the one that had the stripes

14:56

more?

14:57

Yeah, I mean, I guess one of

14:59

the kind of prevailing hypotheses

15:02

is just that the spiders have an innate aversion

15:04

to stripes, even innate aversion to stripes

15:07

or

15:07

a learned aversion to stripes, basically because a

15:09

lot of things out in the environment that have stripes

15:11

are using those stripes to advertise their toxicity.

15:13

So if they have an innate avoidance to stripes,

15:16

then that would kind of protect them from wasting

15:18

energy or even getting poisoned from a lot

15:20

of the things that are out there that are toxic and

15:23

using stripes to stay away. But

15:25

there's also other reasons that they might be particularly

15:27

attentive to stripes and why stripes might work

15:29

as a warning better than other cleaners or

15:32

than other patterns. And one is that the

15:34

stripes are just, you know, regardless

15:36

of what background you're on, black and white stripes

15:39

are really

15:39

conspicuous. So they might be they might

15:41

be good at both getting attention and signaling

15:43

to a predator not to attack.

15:45

So the spiders take a good long look at

15:47

things to make sure that they're this

15:50

is the one I'm not going to eat. I'm going to eat that one. They do. Yeah.

15:53

The way jumping spiders hunt is really interesting. So

15:55

they just they typically sit and wait predators. And

15:58

so when things catch their attention.

15:59

and then those lateral eyes, then the spider

16:02

will actually swivel and look really closely

16:04

at the prey with those front eyes. And in some species,

16:06

those big eyes in the front also have

16:08

color vision, and they have really good resolution.

16:11

So they can see things like pattern, they can

16:13

see things like color. Those side eyes don't

16:15

seem to have very good vision,

16:18

but they do allow the spider to detect things moving

16:20

basically almost 360 degrees around it.

16:23

Wow. So this is happening with

16:25

little tiny spiders, and yet we see

16:28

black and white stripes all the way up to zebra,

16:30

so to large animals. Does this explain why

16:33

stripes are so common in nature?

16:35

Yeah, I think

16:35

it helps to explain why stripes are so common in

16:37

nature. So people

16:38

have been thinking about these ideas for a long

16:40

time, thinking about the idea of warning colors and

16:42

why certain warning colors and

16:44

patterns work better than others, and why there seem

16:46

to be these kind of universal colors

16:49

and patterns that work across all animals, regardless

16:51

of whether you're a bird or you're

16:53

a fish or you're a lizard or

16:55

whatever. But most of that work has been done

16:58

on really big animals, but there's been

17:00

very little work or comparatively less

17:02

work done on small animals like jumping

17:04

spiders. And so we have a lot

17:06

of insight already from birds. I think what's really

17:09

interesting is a lot of things that we're finding from jumping spiders

17:11

are really similar to what we're finding from birds. So

17:13

that might explain why there are kind of these universal

17:15

patterns out there that just seem to be really good

17:18

at signaling

17:19

danger.

17:21

And I guess as your science progresses,

17:23

you're just going to have to make sure you have lots of paper

17:25

and Elmer's glue to continue.

17:27

Yeah, yeah, there's a lot

17:29

of things you can do on a pretty low budget. We also

17:31

need a lot of spiders and we need a lot of termites and

17:34

a lot of time. Dr. Taylor,

17:36

thank you so much for your time.

17:38

Thanks so much for having me. Dr. Lisa

17:40

Taylor is a behavioral ecologist from

17:42

the Department of Entomology and Nematology

17:44

at the University of Florida in Gainesville.

17:54

Imagine you're coming home with a heavy bag

17:57

of groceries on each arm and see

17:59

a package at your

17:59

door.

18:00

You shift the bags to pick up the package

18:03

and then you somehow get the house keys out and

18:06

then oh no your phone

18:08

rings and it's a call you absolutely

18:10

have to take. In a panic

18:13

you wish you had just one extra arm

18:15

or maybe two or six Dr.

18:17

Octopus style to handle all your

18:19

needs at once. While

18:22

this may be the stuff of science fiction for

18:24

now researchers have been exploring

18:26

the very real question of what it would

18:28

be like for us to have even one extra

18:31

limb at our disposal. Well

18:33

now in a simulation experiment they've

18:35

shown just how handy an extra

18:37

limb can be. Katya

18:40

Ivanova is one of the authors of the study. She's

18:42

an assistant professor at Queen Mary University

18:45

of London and a researcher at Imperial

18:47

College London. Dr. Ivanova

18:49

welcome to Quirks and Quirks. Thank you for

18:51

having me. Now we can all imagine situations

18:54

where we could use an extra arm or two. Was

18:56

there a special setting

18:59

for you that inspired this study?

19:01

Absolutely so we are especially

19:03

interested in robotic assisted

19:06

surgery. Normally we

19:08

have this task where we need

19:10

more than two arms or

19:13

two hands. However in

19:15

order to do it right now the only way is

19:17

to have

19:17

an assistant. So a second person

19:19

to work in a team. But in some tasks

19:22

right like surgery

19:24

it's not really coming handy because a surgeon

19:26

may be preferred to have a full control of

19:29

all tools. If we imagine situation

19:31

when

19:32

a surgeon had to have an assistant

19:35

and this assistant had to be first trained

19:38

to almost treat the mind of the

19:40

surgeon. How I would like to have my light, how

19:43

I would like to have my suction. It also

19:46

takes additional time

19:47

during the surgery to communicate

19:49

verbally.

19:50

If I have control over all tools myself

19:53

I don't have to do it.

19:54

So basically it's more efficient.

19:57

Also if I have a new member of the

19:59

team

19:59

This time the team also needs to be

20:02

trained every time.

20:04

So basically we need to learn

20:06

to work as a team every time

20:09

when we have a new member.

20:11

Well take me through your experiment. What did the extra

20:13

arm in your study look like?

20:14

Right now it's just

20:17

a virtual arm on the screen.

20:20

So basically we did an experiment where we controlled virtual

20:22

arms on the screen, presented on the screen.

20:25

And we controlled the three arms, two

20:29

hands by robotic

20:30

interfaces. And we

20:32

also used foot to

20:34

control a short arm.

20:35

Okay, so a person sitting at a computer

20:38

screen, they have a controller in each hand

20:40

and their foot on another controller, is that

20:42

right? Exactly. So

20:45

what kind of tasks did they have to do

20:47

with the three arms?

20:48

So they have presented the

20:50

three arms on the

20:51

screen, presented, replaced

20:53

a screen.

20:54

And their arms

20:57

in the screen were connected with a

20:59

virtual elastic band. So they had

21:02

to move the center of this

21:04

triangle in the direction of the target.

21:06

They had to reach the target

21:09

with the three arms by

21:12

also containing the

21:12

distance between the arms.

21:15

Now normally feet are

21:17

not used for delicate operations

21:19

the way the hands are. So how well

21:22

did the people do?

21:23

Actually pretty well. But I need

21:25

to say the interfaces are also quite good.

21:27

Okay, but again feet are not

21:30

as dexterous as hands. So was that

21:32

a limitation? Did it take a lot of practice

21:35

for people to develop the control?

21:37

This is exactly what

21:39

we explored in the study as well. So

21:41

basically it took

21:43

four sessions one hour over three days.

21:45

They were really quite good with it. We had

21:47

also another study before had where

21:50

we asked people to control with the foot and with

21:52

the arms different types of motion

21:54

and within five days. Also

21:57

very short training times, 15-20 minutes. they

22:01

won't be able to control it. But of course we're

22:03

saying here, we're speaking

22:05

about a very simple task, right?

22:07

So it's not really a surgery right now.

22:10

So

22:11

in this case, yes, it was quite

22:13

simple and natural to learn it.

22:15

However, the other task could

22:17

be more challenging.

22:19

Now why were you doing

22:21

this on a computer screen and not using

22:23

an actual prosthetic limb?

22:26

It's a good question, but we have to start

22:28

somewhere, right? So the first

22:31

step to understand whether we're able

22:33

to learn pre-manipulation would be

22:35

on an experiment like that

22:37

before we actually develop all of

22:39

these expensive prototypes and

22:42

use actual robotics. Because also using

22:44

actual robotics in

22:46

direct contact with the human body had

22:48

also their limitations. First,

22:51

there are not so many

22:53

interfaces that we could use right now ethically

22:56

or from the safety perspective as well.

22:58

And this is a fast and

23:00

efficient way to check

23:02

the hypothesis whether humans can learn

23:05

pre-manipulation. Oh, I see. So

23:07

now that you've shown that the people could do it quite

23:09

quickly, they could learn it easily on

23:12

the computer screen, what's your next step? So

23:15

we did some studies also in

23:17

3D in virtual reality. So it's a little

23:19

bit more closer to what

23:22

it could be. And the next step

23:24

was to use real interfaces. Of

23:26

course, the challenge is to understand what interfaces

23:28

should we use from robotics

23:31

point of view. Should we use conventional interfaces

23:33

such as conventional robotic arms,

23:36

very rigid, then from metal plastic,

23:39

or should we go in the direction of interfaces

23:41

of robotics which is how compatible

23:44

with the human body done

23:46

from textiles and silicones, but

23:49

which have limitations

23:50

on their own. For example, they are not

23:52

that controllable or they cannot

23:54

lift

23:55

that much weight.

23:56

Did the participants

23:59

in your experiment complain at all

24:02

that they were I guess overloaded by

24:04

operating so many limbs three at once?

24:07

So

24:07

in this particular experiment we actually compared

24:09

the manipulation with working

24:12

in a team

24:12

of two people. No one

24:15

complained.

24:17

We also evaluated

24:19

a mental load of participants in

24:22

first session, so in a pre-test and also

24:24

after the training in a post-test. And

24:27

we saw that actually the mental

24:29

load

24:30

is getting lower

24:32

because people are learning how to control

24:34

three limbs and they're starting to prefer almost

24:37

to control all three limbs

24:38

by their own instead of having partner

24:41

with them who is controlling either

24:43

foot or hands. Oh I see.

24:45

It's better if you do it all yourself. It's more

24:48

efficient and if you learn how to do

24:50

it, I suppose people like to have some

24:52

agency. Now in

24:54

your experiment when they were moving

24:56

around on the screen you said it was very simple just follow

24:58

a dot. How much

25:00

more difficult would it be to work in three dimensions?

25:04

Much more difficult. So

25:07

we also collaborated with

25:09

Sorbonne University where

25:11

they have such interface

25:14

and also at Imperial College London our

25:16

mentor, Professor Tien-Bortat is

25:19

now developing a system which we actually

25:21

call Dr. Octopus which

25:23

will have four additional

25:26

artificial limbs. So I suppose

25:28

we will have many

25:31

new experiments to go.

25:32

Now surgery is a pretty delicate

25:34

procedure. Would you be comfortable having

25:36

surgery, maybe your appendix removed or

25:38

something by a surgeon using

25:41

multiple arms and their feet to control

25:43

things? And after some training

25:45

why not because

25:46

the surgeon will use their

25:49

natural hands as usual

25:51

but the foot for

25:53

example will control something like lighting

25:55

or

25:57

suction. So why not?

25:59

Well, I guess when you

26:02

drive a vehicle with a standard

26:04

transmission, you're using all four limbs to control

26:06

the car. So I don't know.

26:09

Exactly.

26:10

Dr. Ivanova, thank you so much for your

26:12

time.

26:13

Thank you very much.

26:14

Katya Ivanova is an assistant

26:16

professor at Queen Mary University of London

26:19

and a researcher at Imperial College London.

26:23

For decades, there's been a lot of talk about transitioning

26:26

to renewable energy around the world,

26:28

but in Uruguay, they actually did

26:31

it. On Planet Money, as part of

26:33

NPR's Climate Week, how that

26:35

happened, the story of Uruguay's

26:37

green energy initiative and

26:40

what we can learn from it. Listen to Planet

26:42

Money wherever you get your podcasts.

26:48

I'm Bob McDonald and you're listening to Quarks

26:50

and Quarks on CBC Radio One. Coming

26:53

up later in the program, why the natural

26:55

world turns out to be a fantastic

26:58

pharmaceutical innovator.

26:59

Evolution is a massive, massive

27:02

research and development project

27:04

in a sense, R&D on massive

27:06

days of the series.

27:09

By now, many parts of Canada

27:11

have already had the first snowfall of the year.

27:14

And when we think of snow, we think winter,

27:17

cold, white. What

27:19

we don't associate with snow is the color

27:21

red. But that's precisely the

27:23

kind of snow that Canadian ecologist,

27:26

Lynne Quarmby, has been studying. It's

27:28

something you may have seen in photographs. Entire

27:31

fields of snow turned eerily different

27:34

shades of red, looking almost

27:36

like the inside of a watermelon. In

27:38

a recent study, Dr. Quarmby and her colleagues

27:41

tracked how much watermelon snow showed

27:43

up on glaciers from 2019 to 2022. They

27:47

wanted to find out whether this unusual

27:49

color had an effect on how fast the glaciers

27:52

around the world were melting. Dr.

27:55

Quarmby is a professor of molecular biology

27:57

and biochemistry at Simon Fraser

27:59

University.

27:59

Hello and welcome back to

28:02

Quarks and Quarks. Hi, Bob. Thanks

28:04

very much for having me back.

28:05

So first of all, what causes snow to turn

28:07

red?

28:07

Well, it's a growth of microorganisms,

28:11

primarily algae, and analogy

28:13

would be the blooms that one

28:15

sees in lakes and the ocean in

28:17

the summertime, blooms of microalgae,

28:19

red tides, and in this instance

28:22

it's happening on the surface of the snow.

28:23

Where does it come from and how does it survive

28:26

on snow?

28:26

There are two leading hypotheses about

28:29

where the algae come from. One is

28:31

that they're blowing around the globe, that they're

28:34

maybe in reservoirs, the cysts

28:36

are tucked away in rock crevices,

28:39

but the species that we've been studying

28:42

in other projects in my lab, we

28:44

have some evidence. It's not very

28:46

strong yet, but my favorite

28:48

model is that these guys

28:51

overwinter as cysts underneath

28:54

the snow on the substrate. And

28:56

then in the spring, whether it's a trickle

28:59

of

28:59

water or a bit more

29:00

light, whatever the signal

29:03

is, that they hatch and

29:05

they grow their cilia, they're very

29:08

powerful swimmers. They could easily cover

29:10

several meters of swimming upstream

29:13

to the surface of the snow. And

29:15

then once exposed to the high

29:18

light at the surface, my hypothesis

29:20

is they would then shed their cilia

29:22

and start producing this astaxanth

29:24

and the red pigment to provide

29:27

the antioxidant in the shade. The

29:29

blooms don't happen until the snow

29:31

starts to melt in the spring and

29:34

the algae grow in the interstitial water

29:36

between the snow crystals. Under

29:38

the right conditions, that means warm

29:40

enough to have some water, cold enough to still

29:43

be snow, and for

29:45

a long enough period they can form

29:47

blooms and snow fields and on top

29:49

of glaciers.

29:50

What gives the algae the red color?

29:52

The algae that grow in the

29:54

snow are actually evolutionarily related

29:56

to the green algae that

29:58

we see form the snow.

29:59

pond scum if you

30:02

will in lakes and ponds. But

30:04

the snow is a very challenging

30:06

place for these algae to grow. The light is

30:08

very bright so the light reactions

30:11

of photosynthesis where a photon

30:13

comes in and excites electrons

30:16

and starts the process, that's very

30:19

rapid and intense but

30:22

the follow-up reactions fixing carbon

30:24

dioxide and making sugars of it, that

30:27

part of the reaction is very slow because

30:29

of the cold temperature.

30:29

So we

30:32

think that the red pigment is

30:34

an adaptation to those conditions.

30:36

Okay so let me see if I got this right. You're saying

30:38

that red algae grows in the snow because

30:41

the red color essentially works better to absorb

30:43

light and protect the algae in the bright

30:46

cold environment of the snow? The

30:47

red actually absorbs heat

30:51

and so it helps melt the snow and it

30:54

shadows, it's like a bit of an umbrella

30:56

so it will shade the chloroplast

30:59

so that it doesn't get too much light.

31:00

Okay and chloroplast

31:02

those are the the devices

31:05

that absorb light

31:07

that contribute to photosynthesis.

31:09

Exactly.

31:10

Well how are you able to identify

31:12

areas with red snow in your new study?

31:15

So my graduate student Casey

31:17

Engstrom took a deep dive

31:19

into available satellite data

31:21

and the satellite can take images across

31:24

not just visible light but very

31:27

detailed sampling of different

31:29

bandwidths for the wavelengths of light

31:32

and so Casey developed a model for

31:35

interpreting where was red snow

31:37

and where wasn't it and he trained

31:41

a model for machine learning

31:44

to recognize which

31:46

regions were watermelon snow

31:48

and which were

31:49

something else and and

31:51

that's how we were able to look at the extent in

31:54

the duration and the intensity

31:56

over these four years. So

31:58

what areas get the red snow?

32:00

Well, it turns out that northwestern

32:03

North America is

32:05

one of the hotspots on the globe. So it sounds like

32:07

British Columbia is a hotspot.

32:09

It is, but red snow

32:11

happens everywhere. So we

32:14

can find it in Antarctica, we can find

32:16

it in the Arctic. It's pretty

32:18

cosmopolitan.

32:19

Now

32:20

if this algae is covering

32:23

the snow and turning it red, how

32:25

is that affecting the melting of the glacier

32:27

when spring comes around?

32:28

A number of factors influence how

32:30

much it

32:31

increases the melt, how intense

32:33

the bloom gets, that is how

32:35

red is it, what extent

32:38

does it cover, how big is the bloom,

32:40

and how long does it last over

32:42

the course of the season. And so we were

32:44

able, or Casey was able, to look

32:47

at those measurements and to

32:49

use modeling and estimate

32:51

that for this region, for northwestern

32:54

North America, it's contributing

32:56

to about 5% of the

32:59

melt over the season.

33:00

Okay, so if they're contributing to about 5%

33:02

of the melting of the glacier, has there been

33:04

a change in that effect

33:07

over time? I guess

33:08

the short answer is it depends. Some

33:11

glaciers are melting faster, others

33:13

have not been impacted. What's

33:16

very clear in our data is

33:18

the

33:19

striking absence

33:21

of large blooms in 2021. And

33:24

that is the year that we had the big

33:27

heat dome here. And

33:29

that heat dome caused

33:31

the snow to melt out

33:34

before the big blooms

33:35

could form. So as the temperature

33:37

warms, as we proceed into the future,

33:39

does that mean we'll have more or less red

33:41

snow?

33:42

Well, we might have more in

33:44

the short term, but it's possible

33:47

that this beautiful ecosystem,

33:51

the red snow, I'll remind you, is not just

33:53

the algae, but it's many species of

33:55

algae, it's fungi, bacteria,

33:58

beautiful rotifers and tardic... grades,

34:01

it's a whole ecosystem that

34:04

it may be an ecosystem that we

34:06

lose quite early. As

34:08

we saw in 2021, the blooms

34:11

just didn't form when it was too warm.

34:13

So

34:13

how does this affect

34:15

that the algae is having on the melting glaciers?

34:17

You say about 5%. How does that compare to

34:19

some of the other factors that are affecting glacial

34:22

melt?

34:22

So the contribution

34:24

of algae we do in this paper

34:26

provide parameters that will help

34:29

glaciologists maybe tweak their models,

34:32

but it is nothing that's going to cause

34:34

big changes in their predictions. What is

34:38

really clear is that it's

34:41

the fossil fuel emissions, it's the carbon dioxide

34:43

in the atmosphere that is really warming

34:47

up the planet.

34:47

So the red snow isn't exactly a feedback

34:50

loop as we've seen with other climate

34:52

phenomena.

34:52

Well it is because

34:54

the warmer things are,

34:57

the more red it gets, the more red it gets, the more

34:59

it warms and melts, and the more the algae

35:01

grow. So it is a positive feedback loop.

35:03

It's just not a

35:05

particularly impactful

35:07

one. Dr. Quarmby, thank you

35:09

so much for your time. Thank you so much for having me

35:11

on Bob. Lynn Quarmby is a professor

35:13

of molecular biology and biochemistry

35:16

at Simon Fraser University. Our

35:30

planet is losing species at

35:33

an alarming rate. But

35:35

what are we really losing in this human-driven

35:38

mass extinction? Something

35:40

meaningful, certainly. The richness

35:43

of our natural world has a value that can't

35:45

be counted in money. It's aesthetic,

35:48

even spiritual. But that's

35:50

not to say there aren't powerful practical

35:53

arguments for preserving natural ecosystems

35:55

and biodiversity. An

35:58

important one is that we lose opportunity to

36:00

take advantage of 3 billion

36:02

years of evolutionary innovation, and

36:05

that has important implications for human health.

36:08

You might not know that about half of all

36:10

the drugs in our modern pharmacopeia

36:13

were inspired by nature. A plant

36:16

called the Fox Glove produces

36:18

digitalis used to control irregular

36:21

heartbeats. Aspirin was

36:23

synthesized to provide a pure

36:25

alternative to the salicylic

36:28

acid extract produced from willow bark.

36:31

And then there are antibiotics. 75% of

36:33

currently approved

36:36

antibiotics originate in

36:38

nature. What this means

36:40

is that the remaining mostly unstudied

36:43

diversity of life on Earth represents

36:45

a vast untapped reservoir of

36:48

potentially useful biological molecules

36:50

and compounds that could be medically

36:53

important. Nowhere

36:55

is that more true than in our oceans. Of course

36:58

it's a tricky environment to work in, but

37:01

in a new proof-of-concept study, scientists

37:04

in France have developed and tested a tool

37:06

to sniff the seawater to

37:08

sample the cornucopia of unknown chemicals

37:11

marine organisms release. Dr.

37:14

Charlotte Simler helped to lead a team

37:16

that tested their device called iSmell

37:19

on sea sponges in the Mediterranean.

37:22

She's a natural product chemist at

37:24

the French National Center's Mediterranean

37:27

Institute for Biodiversity and Ecology in

37:30

Marseille.

37:31

Hello Dr. Simler, welcome to our program. Hello

37:33

Bob, thanks for inviting

37:35

me. Why do you need a special

37:37

new device to sample the chemical compounds

37:39

of sea sponge releases?

37:42

There are several objectives

37:44

that

37:44

we pursued by developing

37:46

this device actually. The first

37:49

one is that we know that the marine

37:51

invertebrates that are fixed

37:53

at the bottom of the sea, they are known

37:56

to produce a variety of really original

37:58

molecules that have inspired chemists. and

38:00

biologists alike seeking

38:02

to discover new molecules for their

38:05

seropoietic potentials,

38:06

for example.

38:07

So why C-sponges in particular? C-sponges

38:10

are one of the most

38:12

prolific producers of those really interesting

38:14

compounds. We know that they

38:16

are producing molecules for their defensive

38:19

and

38:19

communication strategies. In

38:22

the ecosystem there are also really

38:24

impressive filter feeders,

38:26

meaning that they will take the food from

38:28

particles that are around

38:30

them in the sea, diluted

38:32

in the sea. And by doing

38:34

so and through their metabolic activity

38:36

they will release compounds into

38:39

the seawater.

38:40

Now why is it a challenge to try to get

38:42

those molecules? I mean couldn't you just sample

38:44

the seawater itself?

38:45

It's a challenge because once

38:48

the compound is diluted, it is produced

38:50

and released in a marine environment, it

38:52

is really diluted in the large volume

38:55

of seawater. And it will be mixed

38:57

with a lot of molecules that are produced

39:00

either by microorganisms or

39:02

other invertebrates or

39:05

species living nearby. And

39:07

the challenge will be actually

39:10

to know where they are coming from, what kind

39:12

of organisms are in fact producing these

39:14

compounds.

39:14

Well walk me through the

39:17

the iSmell device. How does it actually work?

39:20

So the iSmell device is actually

39:23

a system that has an enclosed

39:26

chamber to delimiter zone of interest

39:28

around the species that

39:29

we want to study, for example, or for which

39:32

we want to collect the molecules that are

39:34

released in seawater. And

39:37

just of both this chamber you have

39:40

disks that are placed in

39:42

holders. So these disks

39:45

are like cotton pads and

39:47

they are designed to absorb

39:50

and capture molecules that are passing through

39:52

these disks. And

39:54

then all the features are connected through

39:56

tubing and valves to a

39:58

pump that

39:59

is

39:59

is activated underwater

40:02

by a single push button and

40:05

by the scuba diver. So one of the

40:07

key features is that this device

40:10

can travel with the scuba diver and

40:12

be activated and used by

40:14

a single scuba diver. Oh, I see. And

40:17

what we found out that the chemistry is very

40:19

different when it's collected

40:22

in seawater compared to

40:24

when we collect a little bit

40:27

of marine organism and study its

40:29

chemical composition.

40:30

So I'm just trying to picture that. So a diver

40:33

has this device in their hand of what they

40:35

put it over top of the sea

40:37

sponge?

40:38

Exactly. So they will target the

40:40

species of interest and they will place

40:43

the chamber just above the species.

40:46

And then the diver will activate

40:49

the pump. And by doing so,

40:51

the seawater will go from the marine organism

40:54

to where it's the curtain pads

40:56

or the solid extraction disk

40:58

that we use back outside the

41:01

chamber. So the direction

41:03

is always towards the pads so that

41:05

molecules that are dissolved around,

41:09

produced and released by the marine organism

41:11

goes unidirectionally towards

41:13

the curtain pad and get accumulated

41:16

on this extraction disk.

41:17

So after you did

41:20

the collection of the seawater coming from

41:22

the sponge, how many different

41:24

chemicals did you find?

41:26

So there was a lot of different

41:28

chemicals. We were mainly interested in

41:30

what we could identify as

41:33

being produced by the storm

41:34

species first. We were

41:36

really happy about it because we could actually

41:39

find a subset

41:40

of all the compounds known

41:43

to be produced by the sponge

41:44

in the seawater. We

41:46

could even classify the sponge based

41:48

on the diversity of compounds we could recover.

41:52

And we also found out that

41:55

among the compound that we were able to identify,

41:58

some of them were slightly different.

41:59

different

42:01

from what the sponge is known to produce,

42:03

which for us will be a source

42:06

of new chemicals, new compounds

42:08

that would be worth exploring further.

42:12

Can you give me an example of how a compound

42:14

from a marine organism could

42:16

be useful for biomedical or pharmaceutical

42:19

research?

42:20

Well,

42:21

this is really

42:23

an interesting question. For

42:25

sponges, for example, there

42:27

are compounds that are nowadays

42:29

used as anti-cancer drugs that

42:32

are produced by sponges or have been inspired

42:35

by structure and compound produced

42:37

by

42:37

sponges. So,

42:40

the idea behind it is to say

42:42

that when a marine organism is producing

42:44

a compound, it could be very

42:46

potent from a biological point of view

42:49

because it produces

42:51

it in an environment where

42:53

everything is

42:54

quite rapidly

42:55

diluted. So to be active,

42:57

say to be cytotoxic or to

43:00

act, for example, if a compound is

43:02

released by a marine organism to

43:05

help the marine organism thrive

43:07

and access to food, some of

43:09

this compound might be toxic for other species.

43:12

It's like a defense and competition behavior.

43:15

And the inspiration was to say that maybe

43:18

we could characterize

43:20

this compound and test whether this compound

43:23

would be interesting as a cytotoxic

43:26

potential anti-cancer drug for human

43:28

application.

43:29

So you managed to use

43:32

your eye smell device on a sponge. How

43:34

widely smell device on a sponge, how

43:36

widely could it be used on other marine

43:38

species?

43:39

Well, actually it could be used on

43:42

any kind of other species for which we want to

43:44

know if they are releasing their compounds.

43:47

We tried on sponge because we know they are producing

43:49

a lot of diverse compounds, but maybe we could

43:51

try to test it over

43:54

cohort species or over other

43:56

filter-fitter marine organisms

43:59

such as... C-SCURTs, for example,

44:01

algae,

44:04

and we can actually continue

44:07

to probe and map the

44:09

chemical diversity of marine organisms

44:12

by using this device. And

44:15

also, I think we

44:17

know that a lot of really interesting compounds

44:19

are also produced by microorganisms

44:22

and the ocean is full of different types

44:24

of microorganisms. So for

44:26

that, we would have

44:28

a lot of opportunities for research

44:30

to

44:32

that end. Dr. Simler, thank

44:34

you so much for your time.

44:36

Thank you. Dr. Charlotte

44:38

Simler is a natural product chemist at

44:40

the French National Center's Mediterranean

44:43

Institute for Biodiversity and Ecology

44:45

in Marseille. Now

44:50

it's not just exotic chemicals produced

44:52

by marine organisms that could hold the

44:54

key to new drugs and treatments. With

44:57

more than 8.7 million

44:59

animal species on our planet, nature

45:01

has done a lot of tinkering. There

45:03

could be a lot of answers to questions about

45:05

human health and nature if we

45:08

looked for them more systematically. The

45:11

key, according to Dr. Barbara Natterson

45:13

Horowitz, is to look at what's making

45:15

us sick and look at how

45:17

nature has dealt with those problems

45:20

when inevitably they've come up before.

45:24

Dr. Natterson after all has done many more biology

45:26

experiments than humans ever will, and

45:29

she thinks the biodiversity of the natural

45:31

world can be read for their results.

45:34

Dr. Natterson Horowitz is a cardiologist

45:36

and evolutionary biologist at the University

45:38

of California in Los Angeles. Hello

45:41

and welcome to Quarks and Quarks.

45:42

Oh, I'm delighted to be here.

45:44

How can understanding evolution

45:47

help us better understand why we seem to

45:49

be seeing so many modern health problems like

45:51

cancer, heart disease and diabetes?

45:54

Well, evolution is

45:58

a process that has created the...

45:59

biodiversity on Earth and the

46:02

environment in which our

46:04

Human biology evolved it

46:06

is not the same involvement in which we find ourselves

46:08

now so we evolved in

46:11

an environment where there were Calories

46:14

were hard to come by right and as

46:16

a consequence our metabolism is Adapted

46:19

to an environment where there isn't enough to eat sometimes.

46:22

Well, what do we have today? We have this environment where

46:24

there's Everywhere there's food

46:27

and so a mismatch occurs And so

46:29

we see that obesity and diseases

46:31

related to overfeeding all of those

46:33

things are in a sense a kind of a

46:35

consequence of evolution meeting

46:38

modern day life So as we

46:40

begin to sort of think in an evolutionary

46:42

way we can decode

46:45

and understand why we get sick

46:47

Well, you say the same evolutionary

46:49

principles have also given rise to solutions

46:52

to these problems. So how so?

46:54

Right. This is the really exciting part Biodiversity

46:58

of you know that we're looking at in other animals includes

47:01

the physiology That's not visible

47:03

from the outside but which contains

47:06

countless adaptations which can

47:08

be thought of as you said as solutions

47:11

for the Challenges that these

47:13

animals find in their environments if

47:16

we can find a way to connect

47:18

those adaptations In those other

47:20

species to the challenges

47:23

in biomedicine today

47:24

those very difficult

47:27

Problems that we haven't been able to solve

47:29

yet. We can potentially find new

47:32

avenues to treat and prevent disease

47:34

So I guess nature has done a lot of experimenting

47:37

testing and innovating over the eons

47:39

to solve these problems

47:41

We do precisely evolution is this massive

47:43

massive research and development Project

47:46

in a sense. It's sort of R&D on massive

47:49

doses of steroids

47:50

Okay. Well, let's look at some examples

47:53

of some of the solutions in nature for these

47:55

human diseases that I mentioned Let's

47:57

start with cancer. Where would we find?

47:59

solutions to that problem.

48:01

There was in the 1970s

48:03

a British epidemiologist, a guy named

48:05

Richard Peato, and he observed

48:08

that if every kind of cell

48:10

divides, there's the transfer mutation,

48:13

and that with a mutation there can be

48:15

the possibility of cancer forming,

48:17

and then

48:18

cancer is this sort of unregulated

48:20

division of cells leading to all

48:22

of these health issues. He

48:25

said, well, if every time a cell divides, there's

48:27

a chance of a cancer-causing mutation,

48:30

then animals who are very, very big,

48:32

like an elephant, let's say, that would require

48:35

many more cell divisions to reach

48:37

its mature size than, say, a mouse,

48:40

he said, wouldn't we predict that those very

48:42

big animals, shouldn't they all be just riddled

48:45

with cancer? And that was

48:47

called Peato's paradox. And

48:50

for a long time, people sort of thought

48:52

that was interesting, the implication being that these

48:54

larger animals evolved cancer-citing

48:58

adaptations, right solutions to that

49:00

challenge. But nobody had kind of

49:02

been able to prove that. Then several

49:05

years ago, a number of groups started

49:07

looking at the African elephant. And

49:10

it turns out that the African

49:11

elephant has multiple copies

49:14

of a cancer suppressor gene

49:17

that is very important in our species

49:19

and others. And all of a sudden,

49:22

it really has opened the door to this

49:24

idea that, aha, there is a cancer-suppressive

49:27

mechanism

49:28

in elephants. And of course, since

49:31

then, other cancer suppression mechanisms

49:33

have been found in a number of other animals.

49:35

Wow. Okay, that's

49:38

cancer. What about heart disease?

49:41

Right. So heart disease is the leading cause of death in our

49:43

species. And one form

49:45

of heart disease is heart failure. There's a particular

49:48

type of heart failure that is more common

49:50

in women. And high blood pressure

49:52

exerts a force on the heart's pumping

49:55

muscle, which becomes thicker and thicker.

49:57

And when it becomes thicker, it starts to

49:59

change.

49:59

and stiff and over time, patients

50:02

become short of breath, their exercise

50:04

ability decreases, they're really symptomatic.

50:08

Well, if we turn to the natural world,

50:11

we could ask ourselves, well, are there any other

50:13

animals that deal with high blood

50:15

pressure? And it turns out the modern giraffe

50:17

species with their iconically

50:20

long neck, right, they actually

50:22

have the highest normal blood pressure of any

50:24

animal on Earth up to 300 over 250 or

50:28

something. So it's

50:30

very, very high. And that's just because

50:32

they have to pump blood all the way up that neck

50:34

to get to the head. That's right. It's

50:37

almost like two and a half to three meters from the left ventricle

50:39

that's down in their body between their

50:41

two front legs and the brain. Absolutely

50:44

right. So how does the giraffe get

50:46

around heart disease then?

50:47

Right. So because the heart has to

50:50

push

50:50

vertically of each other, it's the shy

50:52

blood pressure that is basic. The giraffe

50:54

ventricle is thick compared

50:57

to what you might imagine

50:59

if it didn't have the high neck. So that's how the exercise

51:01

is causing the ventricle to thicken. However,

51:04

that ventricle does not appear

51:06

to be stiff. And in our species,

51:09

in the mutations, when a woman comes

51:11

to me and she's had high blood pressure for a long period of

51:13

time, her heart, her ventricle is thick. It

51:15

is stiff. It's got fibrosis. But

51:17

these giraffes have a high blood pressure,

51:20

but they don't have a stiff ventricle. And

51:22

this is a really intriguing

51:25

finding because of course

51:26

the giraffe has to be able

51:28

to flee predators, 40 kilometers

51:30

an hour. So

51:32

it appears that the giraffe have

51:35

evolved adaptations that

51:37

may suppress fibrosis

51:40

to detect its heart from

51:42

the adverse effects of high

51:44

blood pressure.

51:45

Okay. So the giraffe gets around heart

51:48

disease. The elephant isn't

51:50

riddled with cancerous tumors.

51:53

What about diabetes? What animal do we look

51:55

to for that?

51:57

Right. Well, there's actually a Canadian biologist

51:59

since I'm talking too.

51:59

Canadian radio, a guy named Michael

52:02

Asinger and he wrote a book about

52:04

natural animal models and their relevance

52:07

to clinical medicine and he brought up the hummingbird.

52:10

The hummingbird of course is, you know, their

52:12

heart rate is like 1000 or 1200 beats per minute

52:15

and

52:16

they have very, very high

52:18

blood sugars. The level of blood

52:21

sugar that you would see in a severe diabetic

52:23

human, right, but they don't have

52:26

heart disease that happens to human patients

52:28

who

52:28

have high blood sugars, diabetes. They don't

52:30

have kidney disease. These

52:32

birds appear to have evolved adaptations

52:35

to high serum blood sugar

52:38

without the consequences that we see

52:40

in diabetes in humans.

52:41

So how do we take these adaptations

52:44

that nature's come up with to improve our

52:46

own health for things like cancer, heart disease

52:48

and diabetes?

52:49

Well, one example comes from the world of cancer

52:52

medicine and this elephant story to synthesize

52:54

P53, which is this cancer

52:57

suppression gene and find

52:59

ways to deliver it to human patients. In

53:01

the case of the giraffe, there was

53:04

a wonderful paper that was published about two

53:06

years ago

53:06

where they found the genes that

53:08

were unique to the giraffe that seems

53:11

to be involved in protection

53:14

against the effects of high blood pressure and

53:16

they crispered them into mice

53:19

and they exposed the mice to

53:21

high blood pressure. And what they found was

53:23

that these mice who had this giraffe

53:26

gene were resistant

53:29

to heart failure, to the kind of fibrosis

53:32

and the stiffening. And to me, that

53:35

paper is really a sign

53:37

that we are moving forward, that there is now

53:39

a path where modern technology,

53:41

cutting edge science, CRISPR, etc.,

53:44

can meet evolution, right? These

53:47

evolved adaptations that have been

53:50

solving problems for hundreds of millions of

53:52

years and we can bring those two

53:54

things together to find solutions. So

53:56

if we allow biodiversity to, you

53:59

know, the reduction

53:59

in diversity, if we allow

54:02

that to continue, what we're doing is basically

54:05

preventing ourselves from accessing

54:07

solutions that we otherwise would have.

54:10

Dr. Natterson Horowitz,

54:12

thank you so much for your time.

54:14

Oh, a pleasure. Thank you.

54:16

Dr. Barbara Natterson Horowitz

54:18

is a cardiologist and evolutionary biologist

54:20

at the University of California in Los Angeles.

54:25

And that's it for Quirks and Quarks this week. If

54:27

you'd like to get in touch with us, our email

54:29

is quirks at cbc.ca

54:32

or just go to the contact link on our web

54:34

page at cbc.ca slash

54:36

quirks where you can read my latest blog

54:39

or listen to our audio archives. You

54:41

can also follow our podcast or get

54:43

us on the CBC Listen app. It's free

54:46

from the App Store or Google Play. Quirks

54:49

and Quarks was produced by Olsi Sarakina,

54:52

Sonja Biting and Mark Crawley. Our

54:54

senior producer is Jim Levens. I'm

54:56

Bob McDonald. Thanks for listening.

55:02

For more CBC podcasts, go

55:04

to cbc.ca slash

55:06

podcasts.

Rate

Join Podchaser to...

  • Rate podcasts and episodes
  • Follow podcasts and creators
  • Create podcast and episode lists
  • & much more

Episode Tags

Do you host or manage this podcast?
Claim and edit this page to your liking.
,

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features