Podchaser Logo
Home
Why babies forget, and how fear lingers in the brain

Why babies forget, and how fear lingers in the brain

Released Thursday, 14th March 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Why babies forget, and how fear lingers in the brain

Why babies forget, and how fear lingers in the brain

Why babies forget, and how fear lingers in the brain

Why babies forget, and how fear lingers in the brain

Thursday, 14th March 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

This podcast is supported by the Icahn School

0:02

of Medicine at Mount Sinai, the academic arm

0:04

of the Mount Sinai Health System in New

0:06

York City and one of

0:08

America's leading research medical schools. How

0:11

will advances in artificial intelligence

0:13

transform medical research and medical

0:15

care? And what will this

0:17

mean for patients? To find out,

0:20

we invite you to read a special

0:22

supplement to Science Magazine prepared by Icahn

0:24

Mount Sinai in partnership with

0:26

Science. Visit our

0:28

website at www.science.org and

0:30

search for the frontiers

0:32

of medical research-artificial intelligence,

0:35

the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai.

0:38

We find a way. Introducing

0:40

the Giants Ladder, written

0:42

by leading science marketing expert Elizabeth

0:45

Schaub. Crafted for professionals at

0:47

the intersection of science and commerce, the

0:50

Giants Ladder guides you through a

0:52

structured approach to marketing scientific discoveries,

0:54

enabling them to resonate in places that matter

0:57

most, from laboratories to

0:59

boardrooms to policy chambers. To

1:01

get the bestselling book, Kirkus describes as, a

1:04

helpfully practical and authoritative introduction

1:06

to the marketing of scientific

1:08

products at Amazon and

1:10

other book retailers today. This

1:18

is a science podcast for March 15, 2024. I'm

1:22

Sarah Crespi. We have two

1:24

neuroscience stories this week. First up,

1:26

freelancer Sarah Reardon asks, why do

1:28

infants' memories fade? We

1:30

discuss ongoing experiments that are looking

1:33

to pin down the hows and

1:35

whys of infantile amnesia. Next

1:37

on the show, more brain stuff. Researcher

1:40

Hue Kwan Lee is here to talk

1:42

about how the brain encodes generalized fear.

1:45

This is a symptom of some anxiety

1:47

disorders like social anxiety and

1:49

PTSD. I

1:56

think my earliest memory is from when

1:58

I was just about two, two years

2:00

old. I'm sitting or maybe walking on

2:02

this like mustardy brown carpet. Turns

2:05

out though that this is probably not a

2:07

real memory but something that I picked up

2:09

from photos or descriptions of the place I

2:12

was living right when my younger sister was

2:14

born. You know and actually if

2:16

I really think about it I just don't remember

2:18

anything in school at six.

2:21

So maybe like one or two back

2:23

there. This Week in Science freelance science

2:26

journalist Sarah Reardon wrote about this puzzle

2:28

of infantile amnesia. Why we

2:30

don't remember our babyhood. A time

2:33

when we are learning so much about

2:35

the world but we still don't

2:37

remember learning it. Hi Sarah welcome

2:39

back to the Science Podcast. Hi thanks for having

2:41

me. So are you gonna tell us

2:43

your earliest memory that you believe is actually

2:45

a memory? Yeah I think I remember our

2:48

family dog who died when I was

2:51

three but it could just be from

2:53

photos. I definitely remember the Gulf War.

2:56

Oh wow. So that's actually dateable

2:58

but I wasn't very old then I guess I

3:01

was five. Yeah it's

3:03

funny how little I remember

3:05

and I think some of it's kind of gone

3:07

away as I've gotten older but there's definitely a

3:10

huge chunk that's always been missing. And

3:12

so I guess the big question the story and

3:14

it definitely makes me think you know when our

3:17

brains are so busy devouring everything we're

3:19

learning to eat to move to see

3:21

to talk all this stuff we need

3:23

to remember to get on with our life. We

3:26

don't remember learning these things. What

3:28

are some of the explanations out there? What are

3:30

some of the ideas about why we do forget

3:32

this this key point in our lives? One

3:35

thing I learned from reporting that I thought

3:37

was really fun was that the first person

3:39

to really coin the term infantile amnesia and

3:42

study it in any sort of depth was

3:44

Sigmund Freud. And he of course thought that

3:46

it was because we had

3:48

this horrible psychosexual experience of being born

3:50

we needed to wipe that from our memories

3:52

or else we would be traumatized forever. And

3:55

the talk about in the story he wasn't

3:57

entirely wrong it's that to forget being born.

4:00

But there is a growing amount of evidence that

4:02

these memories are suppressed for some reason rather than

4:04

just not being formed in the first place So

4:07

now the question is how and why

4:09

and why does it matter? So it

4:12

could be possible that some of our

4:14

earliest experiences are housed somewhere in the

4:16

brain just archives But it's just not

4:19

accessible. Yes, that's the thinking What are

4:21

some other reasons that people besides Freud think

4:23

that we don't remember this part of our

4:25

lives? For a long time people

4:27

have just kind of dismissed this as well.

4:29

It's an immature brain babies just been born

4:32

It's not important that we remember

4:34

things and so these neurons

4:36

just aren't able to form these memories in the first

4:38

place or they form them for a little while then

4:40

they just get deleted to make room for other things

4:42

and That could be

4:44

true. And then the other hypothesis is that

4:46

they are formed and they're there forever But

4:49

are somehow suppressed to make room for

4:51

new memories I was really

4:54

surprised to learn from your story that

4:56

other mammals have infantile amnesia I guess

4:58

I just never really thought about whether a

5:00

kitten with me would remember or not remember

5:02

its earliest days So what does

5:04

that suggest about what's happening with us

5:07

if we're seeing these other mammals go through the

5:09

same thing? Yeah, yeah, and I've been one of

5:11

the things that people I thought for a long

5:13

time was that it was linked to language development

5:16

But until we could describe our experiences and

5:19

words, we weren't really remembering them But

5:21

so finding that mice and rats and

5:23

other sorts of mammals have this as

5:25

well kind of negated that theory But

5:27

it's really interesting one researcher found that

5:29

a few what they call precocial mammals

5:31

Which are mammals that are born with

5:33

their eyes open able to fend for

5:35

themselves right away guinea pigs are like

5:37

that They're not dependent on their mothers.

5:39

Really they can just fend for themselves.

5:41

They don't seem to have infantile amnesia

5:43

So maybe everything may learn they

5:45

don't have parents around that They

5:48

can just offload their care and

5:50

the need to remember things. So it's much

5:52

more important that they remember, you know where they

5:54

live or What is

5:56

a scary thing? But this brings up

5:58

an important point we're talking about a specific

6:00

type of memory. You know, we remember

6:02

how to talk, but we don't remember

6:04

what we're calling episodic memory, which

6:07

is what events are happening. So what

6:09

defines that? Episodic memory

6:11

is, it's been described to me as sort of the

6:13

what, where, when. I went to Disney

6:15

world when I was three years old and I met

6:17

Mickey Mouse. That would be an

6:19

episode that occurred in your life. And

6:21

it's a little hard to study that in animals

6:24

because they can't tell you where

6:26

and when. So instead researchers have been

6:28

looking at what they're calling contextual memories,

6:30

which are like, when I'm in this

6:32

box with a patterned floor, I

6:34

might get a shock if I step over here.

6:36

And that's sort of a proxy for

6:38

episodic memories that's being studied in mice.

6:41

One of the researchers who talked to us, I think

6:43

they were based in Berlin. They were working

6:45

with us on this question with

6:47

a big group of toddlers. What

6:50

are some of the questions they are trying to find out from

6:52

working with? I think it's like 300 toddlers. What

6:55

are the experiments like and what are they trying to

6:57

learn from this group? They're trying to study this

6:59

contextual memory again, rather than having them

7:02

recall episodes, which there's been a lot

7:04

of work on that, having people recall

7:06

early episodes in their life. And like you were

7:08

talking about earlier, you might be picking this up

7:11

from photos. So it's really hard to, uh, to

7:13

trust some of that data. So this

7:16

is the first prospective study where they

7:18

are giving toddlers these experiences and then

7:20

seeing what they remember months later. And

7:22

so they're having these kids learn

7:25

when they're in a certain room that's

7:27

got spaceships on the walls, for instance,

7:29

one of the boxes has a toy. If they're

7:31

in another room, it's a different box that has

7:34

a toy. And so the kids can learn that,

7:36

but they're not going to remember it very long.

7:38

And so they're repeatedly doing these tasks over the

7:40

course of months to see when did the kids

7:42

start actually being able to remember those cues and

7:45

that information. And the neat thing about this

7:47

experiment is it can be replicated exactly in mice.

7:49

So they're having mice do the same thing

7:51

with bits of food, and they're going to

7:53

be trying to figure out whether some of

7:55

the brain activity, some of the patterns, the

7:58

characteristics that they're observing in the kids are observable. When

8:00

miles and then they can say to a

8:02

more invasive things with the mice to try

8:04

and understand what's happening at the brain level

8:06

the when he can't with humans are we

8:08

able to then pinpoint when these toddlers. Can

8:11

suddenly start remembering when they go

8:13

into the space room. Those. Toys.

8:15

Gonna be in the specific box. There's still

8:17

early in the experiments for the think. right?

8:19

around twenty months is land. Sort of

8:22

switcher cursed from being able to. Not

8:24

remember anything to being able to remember things.

8:26

So I should add that they call infantile

8:28

amnesia up to about eighty three where you

8:31

really can't remember much of anything at all.

8:33

That between ages three and ten, they have

8:35

what is called childhood amnesia for their memories

8:37

are pretty spotty. Yeah, they're not very good

8:39

at separating memories. Lot of things tend to

8:42

run together and that is gradually improves. Over

8:44

the course of those years a super interesting.

8:47

Let's. Talk a little bit about that mouse experiments,

8:49

you know, As you say, To go a bit

8:51

further, for example, you talk to some. Folks that

8:53

use opto genetic such as. This

8:55

Saudis. Have very finely. Control Act Certain

8:57

are certain regions of the brain. What are

9:00

we learning from those? Types of experiments. Yes,

9:02

A. With the epigenetic experiments, what they're

9:04

able to do is figure out exactly

9:06

which neurons in the hippocampus to start.

9:09

A brain that processes memories. Which.

9:11

Neurons exact airing coating that memory

9:13

serves like a constellation. Of neurons is

9:15

a set of them that will connect the other

9:17

and fire together, and that's what forms memory. And

9:19

they call that an engram. Is the

9:21

weather able to do is to figure out when

9:23

the baby mouses first learning the task? Let's say

9:26

that the food is in. The. Box over here.

9:28

Which. Sells are firing at that moment

9:30

and they can label those cells with

9:33

a protein that banks the later activated.

9:35

And. So that city mouse ago about his. Business. it'll

9:37

forget how to do that as an adult.

9:40

It doesn't remember that either. But if the

9:42

researchers shine a light in the brain. These.

9:44

Light sensitive engram cells will fire and

9:46

that causes a mouse to suddenly recall

9:48

the memory. And know where the food as again. For.

9:52

That suggests that there is a way to

9:54

access it via I memory that was lost.

9:56

From infants amnesia are approximation

9:58

of that. Exact. It out.

10:00

It's a very artificial method. I

10:02

don't know that everyone's entirely convinced this

10:05

is. Evidence that this is what happens

10:07

in nature and that we are do all have

10:09

of these hidden and grams bad as is going

10:11

to. Take. Further research to sort out.

10:14

So. He tackled at about the role of

10:16

neurogenesis. Growing. Our brains, growing neurons

10:18

and our brain that it might have in

10:20

forgetting. So you know how would that work

10:23

at how that being tested if that's one

10:25

of several hypotheses for what's happening. And yet

10:27

is that these engram? So they're being formed

10:29

so like they're talking about earlier. It's not

10:31

that the brain can't form these memories that

10:34

can do it just fine, We just can't

10:36

access them. And so the idea here is

10:38

that forgetting. Whether. That's for getting

10:40

your childhood memories Are forgetting where you

10:42

left your car keys could just be

10:45

a function of. It Engram being

10:47

written over with the new engram with

10:49

neurons that the brain decides are more

10:51

important first one still there is just

10:53

not the one that's preferred. At

10:56

that point. Where. That comes in with

10:58

and somehow amnesia is that. What you're

11:00

saying earlier, the brain is growing. Really fast

11:02

learning all kinds of things. Lots and lots

11:04

of neuron growth that happening as well. And.

11:06

So once that starts to slow

11:08

down, maybe the brain is better

11:10

at keeping things rather than constantly

11:13

fighting over them. So. What

11:15

happens if you interfere with neurogenesis that

11:17

affects early memories in this particular experiment?

11:19

Ah yes, if they block neurogenesis than

11:21

they also seem to block and fantail

11:24

amnesia has a tried any other ways

11:26

to block the loss of memories from

11:28

baby mice. There's a few ways that

11:30

can be done. In one paper that

11:32

without and science advances last year found

11:35

that if the infects the mother mouth

11:37

when she's pregnant with a protein that

11:39

sort of mimics like a viral infection

11:41

d baby mice the males only for

11:43

some reason or. Less likely to have

11:46

infantile amnesia they seem be able.

11:48

To. For memory as much earlier, it's

11:50

not entirely clear why that is to

11:52

be that some immune cells in the

11:54

brain seem to be implicated. They might

11:57

be affecting how the brain is developing

11:59

Overall, Or how certain connection to be

12:01

informed tweener on. But. Lie that's

12:03

very early days still. The. Data suggests

12:05

that maybe there are some people that had

12:08

adverse effects on who were hurt real with

12:10

little or where they are in utero that

12:12

might actually have the super early memories. Yeah,

12:14

yet there is some sort parallel evidence that

12:17

as well that. In animals if you

12:19

separate them for their mothers friends since. Their

12:21

brains seem to mature earlier. And. Whether

12:23

or not that's a good thing, For the rest of

12:25

their lives we don't really know. Those animals seem

12:27

to have higher levels of anxiety and

12:30

we know in humans and so many

12:32

ways, those first. Years of life for

12:34

so crucial for forming your personality,

12:36

forming your. Mental abilities so kind

12:38

of makes sense that if that maturation

12:40

process is sped up or interfered with

12:43

in some ways that could be some

12:45

really lasting effects that are. Difficult.

12:47

If not impossible to reverse, Looking.

12:50

Across all these different experiments that you report

12:52

on, do you feel like they're getting closer

12:54

to the answer to the Why question? You

12:57

know, why might we not remember this stuff?

12:59

Here there's several varying ideas. The I think

13:01

the lot of them come down to. What

13:04

The brain Science important? It's

13:06

more important. At that stage in your life. And

13:08

unless you're a guinea. Pig on because you can

13:10

depend on your parents is more important at

13:12

that stage and your life to be learning

13:14

about the world in general. To be learning

13:16

what a cat is versus. Remembering I

13:19

live next door to this particular orange cat,

13:21

so you can forget about the orange cat.

13:23

but you remember the word for cat. You

13:25

remember what of things associated with it and

13:27

you could kind of. Focus. More

13:29

on finding patterns than having individual memory

13:31

is. I. Really like this idea that

13:34

you talk about here. sorry where. There.

13:36

Is it's kind of suits capacity in the

13:38

infant brain that we can't even conceive of

13:40

as adults exists as big differences in it.

13:42

And that might be one of the reasons

13:44

that we're not. We're not able to access

13:46

those memories. Infant. Brains go through a

13:48

number of what are called critical period. Example

13:51

I always think about is a learning a language

13:53

friends as we know the kids can learn a

13:55

language so much more easily than adults do as

13:57

because their brain is just very plastic can pick

13:59

up. all sorts of things and

14:01

that critical period, they say it closes

14:04

at a certain age and after that point

14:06

you really can't reopen it and

14:08

your brain will never be that plastic

14:10

and malleable again and memory formation seems

14:12

to be a similar thing where your

14:14

brain is going along picking up all sorts

14:17

of things, forming these memories in

14:19

a very different way than it will later

14:21

on in life and so there's a

14:23

lot of research into that as well

14:25

like what's happening during these critical periods

14:28

that's changing how the brain matures

14:30

and you were talking about like the experiences

14:32

too, there's some research finding

14:35

that depending on the experiences you have

14:37

during that critical period the brain

14:39

will mature faster or in different ways

14:41

than if you had a different set

14:44

of experiences so it really all comes

14:46

down to just how crucial those first

14:48

few years are. Infantile amnesia is an

14:50

interesting puzzle, definitely something that I could just

14:52

keep talking about for hours but can

14:55

we talk a little bit about how learning

14:57

the why of it, the how of it,

15:00

you know what can we do with that information to either

15:02

look at how adults forget or remember or

15:04

you know other things that we also care

15:06

about? There could be some

15:09

implications for early childhood learning

15:11

like preschool, daycare even, what's it worth

15:13

teaching children at this age? Should we

15:15

be teaching them music? Do you have

15:17

to be fighting to get them into

15:19

the best preschool? One researcher I talked to

15:21

said that she'd written a column for the

15:23

New York Times where for grandparents, so grandparents

15:25

were concerned that their grandchildren might not remember

15:27

them if they died too young so how

15:29

could they, how could you get your kids

15:31

to be able to remember their grandparents? But

15:34

there's this broader question too which is

15:36

very controversial over whether infantile amnesia this

15:39

kind of forgetting is similar to the

15:41

forgetting we do as adults and we talked

15:43

about the neurogenesis for instance and that whole

15:45

idea is very controversial but if it

15:47

is the same kind of forgetting we

15:49

might be able to understand that a

15:51

little bit better by studying this sort

15:53

of as a model. There's some evidence

15:55

for instance that during Alzheimer's of course

15:57

your brain gets all of these plaques.

16:00

that just completely tear up its integrity

16:02

and make it impossible to remember

16:04

anything. But even before those start

16:06

forming, people start forgetting things and

16:09

we're not entirely sure what's happening at that

16:11

point. If we do understand this better, we

16:13

might be able to start getting some clues

16:15

into whether those memories are still there and

16:17

just not accessible before the brain starts accumulating

16:19

all of this damage. Do you want

16:21

to say that there's a lot of different

16:23

hypotheses floating around and they're not

16:26

necessarily mutually exclusive. There could be

16:28

some neurogenesis going on at certain points. There could

16:30

be these critical periods opening and closing

16:32

in ways that we don't fully understand

16:35

just yet. But the fact

16:37

that we have these optogenetic techniques, we're

16:39

getting better at human brain imaging as

16:41

well with younger and younger children. We

16:43

might really start to be learning a lot more

16:46

about this and answering some of these questions over

16:48

the next few years. This really reminds me of

16:50

sleep research. You know, it's something that is omnipresent

16:52

in our lives and we just never think about

16:55

how like technical it is and what it's

16:57

doing and you know how important it is. We

17:00

just don't know. You know, there's just so much

17:02

there's so much to learn about. That's such

17:04

a basic piece of human biology. It's really interesting.

17:06

One of the researchers I barely quoted in here

17:08

is doing stuff on naps and whether kids who

17:11

give up naps at certain ages might have more

17:13

or less mature brains and might like it might

17:15

be more useful to teach them friendship if they

17:17

stop napping on their own. But only those kids

17:20

who've already stopped napping. That's so interesting. Yeah, it's

17:22

all very new but I thought it was really

17:24

interesting. Wow, that's really cool. I just remember being

17:26

forced to take naps and just reading under my

17:29

covers. That's my earliest memory. All

17:34

right, thank you so much Sarah for coming

17:36

on the show. Yeah, thanks for having me.

17:38

Sarah Reardon is a freelance science journalist. You

17:40

can find a link to the story we

17:42

discuss at science.org/podcast. Stay

17:45

tuned for a chat with researcher Hui

17:47

Kuan Li about how fear after acute

17:49

stress lingers in the brain. Researchers

17:59

at Clean University Belfast translate

18:01

research into action and make sense

18:03

of a rapidly changing world. They

18:06

keep up with technological, societal,

18:08

and economic advances and

18:10

drive change through collaboration and

18:13

real-world partnerships. Their

18:15

research leads to critical breakthroughs in areas

18:18

such as green technology, food

18:20

and agricultural sustainability, peace building,

18:22

and health care. Queen's

18:25

University Belfast network of international

18:27

researchers has a reputation for

18:29

global excellence. Over

18:31

99% of their research was

18:34

assessed as world-leading or internationally

18:36

excellent in RAS 2021. The

18:39

impact of this research is felt around the

18:41

world. Visit qub.ac.uk to

18:43

find out how Queen's

18:45

University Belfast is bringing

18:47

research to reality. This

18:56

week's episode is brought to you in part

18:58

by Science Careers. Change your

19:01

job and you might just change the world.

19:03

For anyone who's looking to get

19:05

ahead in or just plain get

19:08

into science, there is no better,

19:10

more trusted resource than Science Careers

19:12

and it's free. On

19:14

our site, you can search career opportunities

19:16

across all disciplines and levels, research

19:19

potential employers, sign up

19:21

to get job alerts via email, upload

19:24

your resume or CV to the

19:26

searchable database or read career advice

19:28

articles. There's no shortage

19:31

of global problems today that science

19:33

can't solve. Be part

19:35

of the solution. Visit sciencecareers.org today.

19:46

Generalized fear is a symptom in

19:48

some anxiety disorders, in panic disorder

19:51

or social anxiety or PTSD. Generalized

19:54

fear is basically reacting in a

19:56

fearful way to events or environments

19:58

that don't relate specifically. to

20:00

the original stressors from the past.

20:03

There are plenty of cues around suggesting that

20:05

there's no danger, but you still

20:07

feel afraid. This generalized fear can

20:10

cause stress and reduce quality of life. And

20:12

we don't know much about how to turn

20:14

it off, but we are learning

20:16

about how it might get turned on. This

20:19

week in science, Hui Quan Li and

20:21

colleagues wrote about changes in the brain

20:23

linked with the onset of generalized fear.

20:26

Hi, Hui Quan, welcome to the Science

20:28

Podcast. Hi, Sarah, thanks for having me

20:30

here. Sure, let's talk a little bit

20:33

about the work you did here. It

20:35

was in mice and also in some

20:37

post-mortem brains. So let's start with what's

20:40

happening with the mice. Going

20:42

into this work, you had an idea of

20:44

where in the brain the switch might

20:47

be happening, where generalized fear might

20:49

be starting, but not the specifics

20:51

of how this change occurs. In

20:54

order to get at those details, you looked

20:56

at mouse brains across a variety of conditions.

20:58

What were the different setups for the mice?

21:00

How were they set up to

21:02

experience generalized fear? We

21:05

used a mouse model and use

21:07

a foot shock as the stressor

21:09

to trigger fear in mice.

21:12

And we observed conditioned

21:15

fear and generalized fear

21:17

were produced after this foot

21:20

shock. In some cases, the

21:22

mouse would go into a space and get shocked,

21:25

and then it would never want to go back to that

21:27

specific spot. It would freeze. It would be afraid of going

21:29

back there. That's kind of the basic fear response, right? But

21:32

then generalized fear is something a little bit different in

21:34

a mouse? Exactly. In

21:37

the same environment, this type of

21:39

fear is called conditioned fear. But

21:41

generalized fear is that the mice

21:44

were put into a novel environment.

21:47

We have never been experiencing any

21:49

stress in those environment. But

21:51

these still show fearful response. As

21:54

you said, that is generalized fear. So

21:57

in the setup, you have one

21:59

condition. that gives you fear

22:01

specific to a specific environment. And then

22:04

you have another set up where

22:06

you get generalized fear, and that

22:08

means that the mice are afraid

22:10

in different environments, novel places. So

22:13

when you looked at the neurons in

22:15

this region that you knew was important to

22:18

generalized fear, how

22:20

is it different after generalized fear

22:22

had been induced in these mice? Tell

22:25

us about the brain region and the changes

22:27

that you saw there. We observed

22:30

neurotransmitter changes in

22:32

a brain region called dalsoraphate

22:34

that is in the midbrain.

22:37

And we observed that the

22:40

cell-to-nurgeous neurons in this region

22:43

changed their co-transmitter

22:46

from glutamate into GABA. Let's break

22:48

that down real quick. There

22:50

are basically co-transmitters, so there's two

22:52

kinds of neurotransmitters being released by

22:54

these neurons. Exactly. So

22:57

they have serotonin as their

22:59

major transmitter, and they

23:01

could release glutamate. And

23:04

when they were in the generalized fear condition, you saw

23:06

a switch for that co-transmitter from

23:08

glutamate to GABA, is that right? Yes. And

23:11

when we blocked the switch from

23:14

glutamate to GABA, we observed that

23:16

the generalized fear was blocked.

23:19

Yeah. So this was all in

23:21

mice. You were able to show this relationship

23:23

between the neurotransmitter expression

23:26

and generalized fear. What did you

23:28

see in the human postmortem tissue

23:31

that you were able to look

23:33

at? We observed a

23:35

similar increase in the

23:37

cell-to-numergeous neurons co-expressing GABA

23:39

synthesis in the

23:41

individuals of PTSD compared

23:45

to the control subjects,

23:48

similarly as what we have

23:50

found in mice. The

23:52

correlation and causality between

23:55

the neurotransmitter switch and

23:57

the generalized fear production.

24:00

was demonstrated in the mouse model.

24:02

In human, we have found this

24:04

correlation. The causality is hard to

24:06

be investigated. Yeah, because you have

24:09

to go deep into the midbrain,

24:11

find this very specific region of

24:13

neurons and say, oh, did

24:15

you change what neurotransmitter you're dealing with?

24:17

That's not easy to do in people

24:20

at all. Exactly.

24:22

Yeah. One of the things that you administer

24:25

to the mice to see if you could

24:27

block this progression to the different neurotransmitters, the

24:29

switch in this region of the brain

24:31

was Prozac. So that worked on the

24:33

mice, but there was somewhat of

24:35

a limitation on the timing. True. When

24:38

we provide Prozac immediately

24:41

after the foot shock, two

24:43

weeks later, the generalized fear

24:46

was gone, was not observed

24:48

for these mice. But

24:50

when we provided Prozac two

24:52

weeks after the foot shock,

24:54

when the fear response is

24:57

already produced, there was

24:59

no effect. So it's not reversing

25:01

the switch. It might prevent it from

25:03

happening. So you showed that there

25:05

is a switch in the neurotransmitters in this region of the

25:07

brain. What happens next? So what

25:10

are the downstream effects of these

25:12

neurotransmitters being different? You know,

25:14

how do we know that that might

25:16

translate into generalized fear? What are some

25:18

suspected mechanisms there? We have

25:20

identified two oral regions

25:23

downstream of atosorafate.

25:25

One of them is central

25:27

amygdala. The other one

25:29

is lateral hypothalamus.

25:31

So these two regions that

25:33

you found downstream, you did see changes

25:36

in their neurotransmitter setup, if you will.

25:39

What do we know about the central

25:41

amygdala and the lateral hypothalamus? You know,

25:43

are these regions associated with fear in

25:45

other settings? Yes, they

25:47

have been studied by other

25:49

researchers that they have a

25:51

role in fear,

25:53

regulation or anxiety

25:56

related behavior. So let's go

25:58

to the other end before we get to

26:00

the switch in neurotransmitters in the

26:02

dorsal reef, in the midbrain. What

26:04

do we know about the causes for that? How do we

26:06

go from fear, a foot shock, to

26:09

a change in neurotransmitters? We

26:12

have identified that stress hormones

26:14

play a role in

26:16

producing generalized fear, particularly

26:19

when we have blocked

26:22

the synthesis of cortical steroid,

26:24

that is a stress hormone,

26:27

when we blocked its

26:29

production, we were able

26:31

to block the transmitter switch, as

26:34

well as generalized fear. So

26:36

how do you see this

26:38

research going forward be integrated

26:41

into therapies or interventions for

26:43

generalized fear? I think

26:45

our study have pointed

26:47

two directions. One

26:49

is with existing

26:51

therapies. Our

26:54

study has suggested that

26:57

earlier intervention is

26:59

likely to produce better effect.

27:02

And for chronic

27:04

PTSD or similar fear-related disorders,

27:09

it may be hard when the

27:12

patients have already a long history

27:15

of the disease. Then our

27:18

study suggest new therapeutic

27:20

target to treat this disease.

27:24

Were you surprised that neurons would

27:26

switch which neurotransmitters they used? I

27:29

don't know, when I took neurophysiology for

27:31

some reason, we

27:34

didn't really talk about neurons switching

27:36

up which neurotransmitters it would use.

27:38

We kind of were like, okay, this

27:40

is the one that you use, this

27:42

is your destiny. Were you surprised that

27:44

these neurons were able to change which

27:46

co-neurotransmitters they were releasing? Yes,

27:49

so for a very

27:52

long history, like in

27:54

classical neurobiology, people

27:56

believe neurons have only

27:59

a single. neurotransmitters throughout their

28:01

life and their neurotransmitter

28:03

is fixed. But

28:05

recently in the last few decades,

28:08

more and more evidence supports

28:10

that neurons do not express

28:12

only one neurotransmitter and surprisingly,

28:15

they can switch their transmitter.

28:18

The Spitzer Lab and

28:20

at UCSD as well as

28:22

other labs who work on

28:24

neurotransmitter switching have not accumulated

28:27

vast evidence in different

28:29

origins in response

28:31

to different stimuli that neurons

28:34

have the ability to switch

28:36

their neurotransmitters. And what

28:38

is more important is that this

28:40

switch contributes to

28:43

behavior changes. So this

28:45

plasticity has an effect all the way at

28:47

what we do or what we think or how

28:49

we feel? Right. Very cool. Thank

28:52

you so much, Hui Quan. Thank you, Sarah.

28:54

Hui Quan Li works as a senior

28:57

scientist at NeuroCrin Biosciences in San Diego.

29:00

When she was doing the work for this paper, she was an

29:02

assistant project scientist in the School of

29:04

Biological Sciences and Center for Neural Circuits

29:06

and Behavior at the University of California,

29:09

San Diego. And

29:12

that concludes this edition of the Science Podcast. If you

29:14

have any comments or suggestions, any of you who are

29:16

interested in learning more about the science podcast,

29:18

please visit us at sciencepodcast.aaaas.org. To

29:21

find us on a post-op thing off, search for

29:23

Science Magazine. Or you can

29:25

listen on our website, science.org.podcast. This

29:28

wall was edited by me, Sarah Krusty,

29:30

and Kevin McClain with production help from

29:32

Megan Todges. On

29:36

behalf of Science and its publisher, AAAS,

29:39

thanks for joining us.

Rate

Join Podchaser to...

  • Rate podcasts and episodes
  • Follow podcasts and creators
  • Create podcast and episode lists
  • & much more

Episode Tags

Do you host or manage this podcast?
Claim and edit this page to your liking.
,

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features