Podchaser Logo
Home
S8 E10: Comprehension is an outcome, with Sharon Vaughn

S8 E10: Comprehension is an outcome, with Sharon Vaughn

Released Wednesday, 14th February 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
S8 E10: Comprehension is an outcome, with Sharon Vaughn

S8 E10: Comprehension is an outcome, with Sharon Vaughn

S8 E10: Comprehension is an outcome, with Sharon Vaughn

S8 E10: Comprehension is an outcome, with Sharon Vaughn

Wednesday, 14th February 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

1:10

I think the strength I

1:12

have, from being in this business

1:14

for decades, is that I've

1:17

seen things go awry. Good

1:19

things get interpreted incorrectly.

1:22

I mean, the Science of Reading has that potential.

1:27

This is Susan Lambert. And welcome to Science

1:29

of Reading: The Podcast, from Amplify, where

1:32

the Science of Reading lives. What

1:35

is reading comprehension? How

1:38

can background knowledge be built, rather

1:40

than taught? And how can vocabulary

1:42

be developed most effectively in

1:45

the classroom? These are some of the questions

1:48

that Dr. Sharon Vaughn explores on

1:50

this fascinating episode of the

1:54

podcast. Dr. Sharon Vaughn is the Manuel

1:57

J. Justiz Endowed Chair in

2:00

Education and executive director of The Meadow

2:02

Center for Preventing Educational Risk

2:04

at The University of Texas,

2:07

Austin . She was also the lead author of the

2:10

What Works Clearinghouse Practice

2:12

Guide, "Providing Reading Interventions for

2:14

Students in Grades 4–9." For

2:17

those who may be unfamiliar, that

2:19

guide is full of grade information, which

2:21

we also discuss during this conversation.

2:25

And so without further ado , I'm thrilled

2:28

to share this conversation with

2:30

Dr. Sharon Vaughn. Well,

2:33

Dr. Sharon Vaughn, thank you for joining us on

2:35

today's episode.

2:37

You're welcome! Thank you for having me on

2:39

this episode.

2:40

Before we hop into the first question, I

2:42

do wanna highlight the

2:45

What Works Clearinghouse Practice Guide.

2:47

So the IES Practice Guide.

2:49

I think this is the most recent one that you

2:51

supported, all about providing reading interventions

2:54

for students in grades four through nine. This

2:57

is a really important practice guide. And I

2:59

bet you've gotten great feedback on it.

3:01

I so like that you said that, because

3:04

I think it is a really important practice

3:06

guide too. And so you

3:08

having said it opens the door for me to

3:10

say, in the audience, if you

3:12

have not looked at this practice guide, it's

3:15

on the IES website. And it

3:17

is about teaching students with

3:19

reading difficulties in grades four to nine.

3:21

And here's the cool thing, not only

3:24

does it list the high

3:27

priority practices that have

3:29

been proven, are scientifically

3:32

based , if you will, to improve outcomes.

3:34

It also has really cool

3:37

examples of how to do it in your classroom,

3:39

with real lessons. So dig

3:41

in! And not only learn what they

3:43

are, but how to do it.

3:45

Yeah , and nobody will actually have to go out and look

3:47

for it. We'll link our listeners in the show notes

3:50

to this practice guide. And I

3:52

love that you let me talk about it, because I think

3:55

the practice guides are underutilized. But

3:57

I found this one particularly helpful.

3:59

So, thank you for helping to craft that guide.

4:03

So, let's just talk a little bit about

4:05

reading comprehension, and what reading

4:07

comprehension really is. And

4:11

you have a quote that I absolutely

4:13

love. I have carried it with me since the

4:15

first time I heard you say it, and I can't remember where

4:17

that was. But the quote is,

4:19

"You can't teach reading comprehension. You

4:22

can't teach reading comprehension. It's

4:25

an outcome." Can you tell us about

4:27

that?

4:28

Oh gosh! Thank you so much for

4:30

saying that, because I think that,

4:32

you know, especially around the Science of

4:34

Reading, many people are sort of

4:36

obsessed with the idea that the Science of Reading

4:39

is phonics . And they are like, "Oh

4:41

my gosh!" That gives them sort of

4:44

a justifiable cause against the Science

4:46

of Reading, because it isn't just phonics.

4:49

Well, of course it's

4:51

not <laugh>! However,

4:54

it is

4:58

quite defensively impossible to learn

5:00

to read if you cannot read

5:03

words. So if you wanna get to

5:06

comprehension, the most important thing you have to get

5:10

to is accurate

5:12

and efficient word-reading. And of

5:15

course the mechanism for getting to accurate and

5:17

efficient word-reading are things like phonemic awareness and

5:20

phonics. But, the point is, you have to be able to read these

5:23

words. And, here's the second thing, you have to know what

5:26

they mean. So, vocabulary is important. You

5:28

gotta know how to read 'em. You gotta know what they mean.

5:31

And those two developments work

5:34

together. And

5:36

then with adequate background

5:40

knowledge, comprehension comes for

5:43

free for the vast majority of

5:46

students. So, comprehension is

5:49

an outcome. And it's based on being

5:51

able to read words accurately, know what

5:54

they mean, have adequate background knowledge,

5:56

and also being able to make

5:58

inferences and not

6:01

check yourself when you go to

6:03

a text. For the most part, if we're

6:06

paying attention, if we can read words

6:08

efficiently, and know what they mean, and have background

6:10

knowledge, it yields comprehension.

6:13

And comprehension, when you try to teach

6:17

it independent of word-reading,

6:21

efficient word-reading, which some people think of as fluency

6:23

, vocabulary, and background

6:25

knowledge, you can't get there.

6:28

So if students can't read words, don't know what they mean, emphasizing

6:31

comprehension is just really

6:34

the wrong priority.

6:37

I think that's what I meant by that, Susan.

6:39

Well, it stuck with me a really long time.

6:41

And I think it's because I put myself back

6:43

in the classroom. And I'm sure other

6:46

classroom teachers are going to relate to this.

6:49

Do you know how often what we thought we

6:51

were doing was teaching comprehension? I'm

6:55

sure teachers can relate to that right now. We thought

6:57

we were teaching comprehension. And to hear

6:59

that, "Yes, there's other factors that can

7:01

influence it. And if these other factors

7:03

are in place, reading comprehension comes

7:05

for free." That's another great quote! I

7:08

think that's another Dr. Sharon Vaughn great

7:10

quote <laugh> that we're gonna put out there is

7:12

that if you do it right, reading comprehension

7:14

is free <laugh>.

7:16

Yeah! And all

7:18

of these strategies, like if you don't know how to

7:20

read words, there are not

7:23

enough comprehension strategies to get

7:25

you to comprehension. If

7:27

you don't know what the words mean,

7:29

you can teach me 27

7:32

comprehension strategies and my comprehension

7:34

will not improve. All I will do is be

7:36

confused by all these strategies you've taught me.

7:40

But if I have efficient word-reading, and

7:42

know what the words mean, and have background knowledge, then

7:45

things like main idea, and

7:48

summarization, and inferencing become

7:51

very meaningful tasks that

7:53

help me dig deeper into what

7:55

I'm reading. And then you don't

7:58

need a bucket full of

8:00

comprehension strategies. You need

8:03

a handful. Just a small handful

8:06

of comprehension strategy.

8:08

I love how you said that, because, as

8:10

you know, right now there's just this big

8:13

conversation about, well,

8:15

is it background knowledge and vocabulary that we should

8:17

be teaching? Or is it comprehension strategies

8:20

that we should be teaching? And

8:22

really good readers have both

8:25

things. They use both things.

8:27

So it's not an either/or proposition.

8:30

However, background knowledge is pretty important;

8:33

and it really does influence comprehension.

8:35

Well, it does Susan. And like all

8:38

things in education, we

8:40

have every single good idea that we

8:42

can mess up <laugh> . And I

8:47

kind of worry that we might

8:49

do that with background knowledge. Because

8:53

is it important? Yes. Does

8:55

it facilitate comprehension? Yes.

8:58

Has it been overlooked? Yes.

9:01

But listen to this, how could

9:03

we mess it up? The

9:06

way we could mess it up is to

9:08

think that you TEACH background

9:10

knowledge rather

9:13

than BUILD background

9:15

knowledge.

9:16

Oh, tell me what you mean.

9:20

Did you like that idea?

9:22

I did, yes! It's another Sharon

9:25

Vaughn excellent quote there .

9:27

<laugh> I'm hanging out with you more often <laugh>!

9:29

But, what I mean by that is,

9:32

if we start seeing background

9:34

knowledge is having, like, these components.

9:37

And we have to stand in front of students, and

9:39

start teaching it where the teacher's

9:42

doing all the work. And the

9:44

students are sitting there half aware , half

9:47

asleep, and mostly hoping it ends

9:49

soon, then we

9:51

are not going to enhance background knowledge

9:53

. But if we say to ourselves, "Hmm. I

9:57

need students to have broader,

10:00

deeper, more thorough

10:02

background knowledge, how do

10:04

I build that?" The

10:07

way I build that is

10:10

this duo-focus, from the

10:12

very early stages of teaching,

10:15

in which I have listening

10:18

through higher level texts,

10:20

because students can comprehend through listening

10:23

at a higher level, and I'm

10:26

using information texts

10:28

that build on what students

10:31

are going to be learning in the future in

10:33

social studies, and science, and various areas.

10:36

So I'm literally building this background

10:39

knowledge from the beginning through

10:41

listening comprehension. And

10:44

of course I'm focusing more

10:46

on information texts, because

10:48

those are excellent sources for doing

10:50

that. And so by building,

10:53

I mean that you systematically say

10:55

to yourself, "What are the topics — these

10:59

students are vulnerable, meaning they

11:01

have inadequate background knowledge — and our

11:04

priorities — meaning they're g onna be taught

11:06

in the near future, in the next year or two — that I

11:09

can ramp up?" So that

11:12

the key ideas, concepts, and

11:14

vocabulary words are familiar

11:16

to students when they encounter them. Does

11:19

that kind of fit, with what I'm thinking

11:21

about with building rather than teaching?

11:23

It makes a lot of sense. It actually does, yeah.

11:30

There's an essence of coherency here too

11:32

that I hear coming through in what you're describing. That,

11:35

probably, when we build background knowledge, it's

11:37

not, "Oh, we're going to look at this

11:40

topic today. And tomorrow we're

11:42

gonna think about another one. And the next day

11:44

another one." Right? There must be some

11:46

kind of time on topic that

11:49

probably makes a difference. Does that make sense?

11:52

Whoa! So I am going to borrow

11:54

that word, coherence. I'm

11:57

going to borrow it, and use it in the future,

11:59

because it is really one

12:02

of the fundamental ideas about

12:04

making building of background knowledge

12:07

successful. Because, as

12:09

you said, it's not sort of

12:11

this hunting and pecking around different

12:13

ideas, like, " Oh, today we're gonna talk about legs

12:15

on a spider. Tomorrow we're

12:18

gonna talk about icebergs." We

12:22

can go through those things in iterative

12:24

ways. And we can return to these topics.

12:27

It doesn't mean you spend all your

12:30

time on one topic and never leave. But

12:33

there is a structure, an organization,

12:36

a coherence, as Susan says, that

12:39

really allows students to

12:42

sort of, in a spiral and iterative

12:44

way, encounter these topics. Enhancing

12:48

their background knowledge and concept understanding

12:50

over time. And you know what

12:52

else Susan, they could be done in

12:55

ways in which they're sort of thematically

12:57

organized. So, it could be something

12:59

like places in the world.

13:02

And so for a while you build

13:04

on different places in the world that

13:07

students need to know about. Or it

13:09

could be something like things that

13:12

move fast. And

13:14

you can go from animals to F1,

13:17

to all kinds of really interesting

13:20

things that move fast. And you

13:22

really learn about them. You learn vocabulary,

13:24

and concepts. Then you learn things

13:26

like velocity. And you could

13:28

just imagine how helpful that could be

13:31

as students broaden what they read

13:33

and learn later on.

13:35

There's motivation

13:38

in that for students. I'm in the world

13:40

of elementary, that's where I taught. But

13:43

I loved seeing students get

13:46

excited about topics. And get

13:48

excited to use words that they felt

13:50

like were big people words. There

13:53

is a motivation to that for kids in schooling.

13:56

And since you

13:58

said it, let's talk a little bit about it. There is this relationship

14:00

between vocabulary and

14:03

knowledge in the process of building

14:05

knowledge. You're building and

14:07

acquiring more breadth, and depth,

14:10

and vocabulary as well. And so,

14:12

the two things are so reciprocal in

14:14

nature that when you have

14:17

vocabulary, you likely have the knowledge

14:19

about where that vocabulary sits,

14:21

in terms of themes and topics.

14:23

I really think that's right. And

14:26

I think the emphasis on vocabulary,

14:29

which we've seen, Susan don't you agree, grew

14:31

in the last 10 years.

14:32

Yes.

14:33

And it's pretty well accepted

14:35

in elementary, upper elementary,

14:37

even secondary that vocabulary

14:40

is sort of the , if you will, high

14:43

nutrition. We need to really build

14:46

the DNA of learning. And

14:49

I think that's all right.

14:52

And like I said, there is no good

14:54

idea in education that we can't

14:56

mess up. So let me tell you my worry about vocabulary.

14:59

You ready for this?

14:59

Please! Yep, yep I am.

15:02

<laugh> So here's my worry.

15:04

What I've seen a lot of is that,

15:06

because vocabulary

15:09

is a proxy for background knowledge,

15:11

meaning if we really wanna understand

15:15

students' background knowledge on a particular topic, a

15:18

pretty good way to do that is to

15:20

ask them whether they

15:22

know the meaning of some of the words that

15:25

are fundamental to that

15:27

background knowledge. And so, it

15:29

becomes this sort of proxy. And we,

15:31

therefore, think that by

15:34

teaching vocabulary, we're teaching background knowledge.

15:36

Because they are associated. And

15:39

so that's worry number one.

15:41

I get that. Yes.

15:42

Right? And that's not the same.

15:44

They're related, but they're not the same. And

15:46

the second thing, it's like telling

15:49

me my cousin is related and we're

15:51

the same. I was like, no, no, no, no, no. I'm

15:54

not. The

15:57

second part of vocabulary that worries

15:59

me is that there's

16:01

just a lot of words.

16:03

There are <laugh>.

16:04

Right? There's, like, 5,000

16:08

words that students need to learn every

16:10

year. And so, if

16:12

we as teachers think, "Oh my gosh, I

16:15

have to directly teach all of

16:17

these words. It's my

16:19

job to be the word teacher,"

16:21

rather than, or

16:24

in addition to, "It's

16:26

my job to teach students

16:29

to be word-conscious." Because

16:32

if they can accumulate words and

16:35

become word collectors,

16:38

then a lot of the

16:41

learning needed to acquire that

16:44

multi-thousand number of words they

16:46

need really comes as

16:49

they go through life. Listening,

16:51

and learning, asking questions, reading,

16:54

and thinking about words. Because

16:57

we can teach a couple of words a

17:00

day. We can teach handfuls

17:02

of words a week. And even

17:05

if we're on it as a

17:07

teacher, we will inadequately

17:10

meet the word learning

17:12

needs, meaning the needs,

17:16

of the students we teach. So

17:18

that's kind of a worry, that we sort of think it's

17:21

on us to teach them the

17:23

words and their meaning. 'Cause

17:25

we just can't do it all. That doesn't mean we shouldn't do some

17:27

of it, right? Absolutely. But we

17:29

have to do both.

17:30

You know, I love that you're framing both

17:32

of these things so far, vocabulary and

17:35

background knowledge, in what Sharon

17:38

Vaughn worries about.

17:39

<Laugh>.

17:39

How these things can go go awry. How

17:41

we in education might, what

17:43

did you say? Ruin them, or destroy them, or something

17:45

like that? Because it's always good.

17:47

It's like when we teach vocabulary. What it is and what

17:50

it's not. It puts some guardrails

17:52

on it too. It's like, wait a minute, these

17:54

things are important, but they could also go

17:56

wrong if we're not careful about how we're

17:58

viewing these in instruction.

18:00

I like that you said that,

18:02

because I think

18:04

the strength I have from

18:07

being in this business for decades

18:10

is that I've seen things go

18:13

awry. Good things get

18:15

interpreted incorrectly. I mean, the Science

18:17

of Reading has that potential, don't you think Susan?

18:19

Where people could take that

18:22

and sort of start creating their own

18:24

meaning about what it means. And

18:26

start downloading that on

18:29

districts and schools in ways

18:31

that are counterproductive.

18:33

Yeah, it's certainly true. And

18:35

I think we started this

18:37

episode with just that. It's that even

18:40

now some folks think the Science

18:42

of Reading is just, and only about,

18:45

phonics instruction. And, although that's

18:48

critical, and an important part, it's a

18:50

much bigger body of evidence

18:52

and body of research than just that.

18:56

We'll be right back. Last

18:59

time around, we told you we were sharing our

19:01

final message on knowledge building from

19:03

last year's cohort of Science of

19:05

Reading Star Awards finalists. But

19:08

I've got good news! We actually have

19:10

one more message to share with you this

19:13

time around. We're going to hear from Heather

19:15

Campbell, a learning coach in southern Utah.

19:18

Heather was a finalist for The Changemaker

19:21

Award. Here's Heather.

19:24

When it comes to talking about

19:26

knowledge and knowledge building with

19:28

the Science of Reading, it really is

19:31

important to make sure that the

19:33

teachers understand how to do it in

19:35

bite-sized pieces. Take what

19:37

they're already doing and apply

19:40

it in different ways. It is

19:42

making sure that we're more explicit with vocabulary.

19:45

And making sure that we're taking the

19:47

knowledge strand and providing the

19:49

students the prior knowledge they need

19:51

in the content areas. So they will

19:54

be successful with their comprehension

19:56

later on. I am lucky

19:58

to live in Utah where we have Senate

20:01

Bill 127, which

20:03

states that our current kindergartners,

20:06

by the year 2027, when they are

20:08

third graders, 70%

20:10

of them will be reading at grade level. And

20:13

for us, it's been really diving

20:16

into what is grade level like?

20:18

What does that look like? And it's

20:20

helping the teachers understand the importance

20:23

of increasing the rigor, and

20:25

providing an equitable education for every

20:27

single student in the classroom. So

20:30

that wherever they go,

20:33

and what future careers they choose,

20:35

they will have the knowledge that they need

20:38

in order to be successful and

20:40

to have future jobs and careers.

20:42

That was Heather Campbell, learning coach from

20:45

Sunset Elementary in Washington County School

20:48

District in Utah. Heather was a

20:50

finalist for The Changemaker Award. That

20:53

was our final message

20:55

from our 2023 cohort

20:58

of the Science of

21:00

Reading Star Award finalists. But

21:03

applications are open for

21:05

the 2024 Science of Reading Star Awards.

21:09

Find

21:15

out more information, and submit a

21:17

nomination, at amplify.com/sor-star-awards. And now,

21:20

back to our conversation with Dr. Sharon Vaughn. I

21:23

wanna go back a

21:25

little bit, not to plug your

21:28

practice guide again, from the What

21:31

Works Clearinghouse, but ... for grades four through nine, I do a lot

21:32

of work with

21:35

elementary teachers and educators. And more and more

21:38

people are understanding the importance of read-alouds,

21:40

and getting this background knowledge and vocabulary

21:42

to kids, and texts that are well above

21:44

their grade level, because they're not even reading

21:46

yet. But even in this What

21:49

Works Clearinghouse Practice Guide,

21:52

you remind fourth through

21:54

ninth grade

21:56

educators, in recommendation number three, about building a

21:59

set of comprehension-building practices,

22:02

or using a set

22:04

of practices. And I know you don't have this memorized,

22:06

but I have it in front of me. And

22:08

it says two things. There's four parts to this. The

22:11

first two parts are build students'

22:14

world and word knowledge, and

22:16

then consistently provide students with opportunities

22:19

to ask and answer questions to

22:22

better understand the text. And I think those two things

22:25

are really interesting. Because one, I

22:27

think upper-elementary and middle-grade teachers are not quite

22:29

sure what this comprehension and

22:31

knowledge building should look like. But

22:34

we're still focusing on those things, right? In

22:36

the upper elementary and middle grades. Word

22:39

knowledge. World knowledge. Ask and

22:41

answer questions. And get kids involved

22:43

in this building of background knowledge.

22:46

Did I get that right? Did I make a connection

22:48

correctly?

22:50

I like that bridge.

22:52

And it bridges beautifully to what

22:54

we were talking about with

22:56

background knowledge. World

22:59

knowledge is just another way to say background

23:02

knowledge. And word-reading

23:04

is just another way to talk about what

23:06

we were saying, where you have to be an efficient and accurate

23:10

reader of words. And I

23:12

keep using the word, "efficient." And

23:14

the reason I do is because

23:17

when you are slow and laborious,

23:20

even if you are accurate, that

23:24

slow, laborious

23:29

reading keeps

23:32

you from freeing up your cognitive

23:34

resources in such a way that you can think

23:36

about what you're reading. And you can

23:38

hold multiple ideas in your head.

23:41

And you can compose meaning so

23:44

that you can read for understanding. And

23:46

that's why that efficiency is so

23:49

important. Especially when

23:51

you get to fourth through ninth grade. Because

23:53

without it, that really

23:57

extensively slow reading bogs

24:00

you down. So that by the

24:02

time you reach the end of the paragraph,

24:05

you can't possibly remember

24:07

what the beginning of the paragraph was about.

24:11

So, we really want to think about ways to

24:13

improve efficiency. And that's why fluency

24:15

ends up being important, even in those upper grades.

24:18

And kicking back to

24:21

your question about the

24:23

strategies of asking and

24:25

answering questions. Can we return to

24:28

that as well, Susan ?

24:29

Yeah , please.

24:29

OK. So, here's why I wanna

24:32

return to that. Asking questions is, as you

24:37

know, extremely difficult. Asking

24:39

the RIGHT questions. Asking

24:41

questions that get students to think about,

24:44

and wonder about, what they've read. And

24:46

integrate it with what they know and think from

24:49

other things . That's where

24:51

the real learning juice is.

24:55

Asking questions that

24:57

require me to go back into

25:00

the text and find one-word answers.

25:03

You know, what color was the boy's hat when

25:05

he was riding the pony? Well,

25:08

I mean, I can find that, right? And

25:11

I don't even necessarily have to know a lot about

25:13

comprehension in order to do it. So,

25:16

I don't think those questions are

25:19

useless, but

25:21

they're pretty close to useless. We

25:24

really have to figure out how to

25:26

ask questions. Like, what about

25:30

how? Why?

25:33

When did this happen? And

25:36

why do you think the author wanted

25:38

that? So, really,

25:40

the quality of the question we

25:43

ask has everything to

25:45

do with the quality of the comprehension

25:47

we can expect. And

25:51

we think of these questions, Susan,

25:53

as coming after students read.

25:56

Not bad, you know, we should do some

25:58

of that. But, it's also not a bad idea

26:00

to say, "So far,

26:03

here's what we've read about Napoleon.

26:06

In the next page, something

26:09

is going to happen that

26:12

changes the way Napoleon

26:15

thinks about how to lead

26:17

men. As you read this, see

26:21

if you can figure out what that

26:23

is? And how that affected him?"

26:26

That's very

26:29

thought-provoking, in terms of the

26:32

quality of our comprehension. I think this is

26:34

what you said. I'm gonna say it back to you again. The

26:36

quality of the comprehension, or

26:38

how we're assessing the quality of students'

26:41

comprehension, is directly related

26:43

to the types of questions we're

26:46

asking, or the quality of the questions

26:48

we're asking. So there's a relationship

26:50

between those two things.

26:52

Thank you for putting a

26:55

headline on those comments, because that's a really nice

26:57

headline. Another headline

27:01

along with yours is

27:03

text matters. I

27:06

think we have this idea for

27:08

so long, Susan, that

27:11

every child should know what

27:14

their level is in their leveled

27:16

reading. And every

27:19

child should read in their leveled

27:21

reading box. And

27:23

golly, forbid if you pick

27:26

up a harder or easier box.

27:29

Well, I don't know about you, but how

27:31

would you like to have a box that you could

27:33

read in and not lower or higher?

27:36

<laugh> Yeah , no , thank you . That's interesting,

27:40

because you also talk a lot

27:42

about, I think this is

27:44

right, nutrition as

27:46

it relates to instruction,

27:49

and making sure we have a good balance of

27:51

that. Is that right?

27:53

I think so. In the

27:55

guidance document that you referred to, one

27:58

of the things they suggest, or we suggest,

28:01

I can't blame them since I'm the first author, we

28:03

suggest.

28:05

<Laugh>.

28:05

If you guys don't like it out there, it's not my fault; if

28:08

you like it, I did the whole thing by myself <laugh>. But

28:12

the recommendation is

28:14

to integrate stretch-text into

28:17

students reading. And the

28:20

thinking behind that is that

28:22

as students are behind, because

28:25

this is focusing on students who are struggling readers,

28:28

we need to understand that for many

28:30

of these students, accessing grade

28:33

level text has never occurred. So

28:36

they have had a steady diet,

28:40

if you will, of text that has limited

28:42

background knowledge, marginalized

28:46

vocabulary, and some

28:48

text that's under-complicated. And

28:51

because of the steady diet of that

28:53

... now, I don't think they should have none of that.

28:55

People need to practice reading what they can't

28:58

read. But they can't ONLY

29:01

practice reading what they can't read. With

29:05

strong teacher support, reading

29:08

texts that are challenging can

29:10

be interesting, fun, engaging, and

29:13

profitable.

29:14

I like

29:16

how you said that. Interesting,

29:19

engaging, and profitable. Can

29:22

you talk a little bit more about what you mean by profitable?

29:24

Well, what I mean is this. So,

29:27

imagine, Susan, that I'm

29:29

one of the students you're teaching. And

29:31

imagine me as a fifth grader, which

29:34

is not actually that hard. I act that way

29:36

quite a bit <laugh> . And

29:42

imagine that I read on the second-grade

29:45

level. So you can

29:47

imagine that over the last six

29:49

years of my schooling, the

29:52

kind of low-level, mind-numbing,

29:56

simplified texts I've

29:58

been exposed to, right? Because I have

30:00

to read my level books. And they're

30:03

all just exactly what

30:05

you think. I think people do

30:07

a really good job trying to make those books

30:10

interesting. I am sometimes amazed at how

30:12

interesting they make them. But nevertheless, I'm

30:15

in fifth grade and these books are

30:17

quite a bit simpler than what

30:19

I am as a developed fifth grader.

30:22

Now, imagine that

30:25

there's a topic I'm interested in.

30:27

And imagine you find a book about

30:30

trains on the fourth-grade level. And

30:33

we are able to work through it,

30:35

because as a seasoned, knowledgeable

30:38

teacher, you know what words

30:40

to show me ahead of time. You know

30:43

how to tell me how to re-read. You

30:45

know how to read the paragraph first,

30:48

before me, and say, "I'm gonna read it first and then you're

30:50

gonna gonna read it." You know how to support me, so

30:53

that accessing this text

30:56

is profitable for me.

30:58

That's great. It's

31:02

really helpful. A helpful reminder

31:05

that education is about

31:08

stretching, and growing, and helping

31:10

our students access those things that they

31:12

might not have been able to access

31:15

without us. And so,

31:17

that idea of helping kids get to

31:21

what you're calling stretch-text is really great. I

31:23

think you

31:28

know something too about

31:31

literacy instruction, that

31:34

actively and effectively supports

31:37

students' literacy development. And I'd love for you to talk about a

31:39

couple of things. The first

31:42

one is called Collaborative

31:45

Strategic Reading, also known as

31:47

CSR, because we like to shorten things <laugh> . Can

31:52

you talk a little bit about CSR?

31:55

And what it is? And why it's so effective?

31:58

Thank you for that invitation, because my

32:01

colleagues Janette Klingner

32:03

and I, and unfortunately

32:06

Janette Klingner has passed away, which is very,

32:08

very sad for me. That was

32:12

probably about eight years ago or so. And

32:15

she and I, all the way back when

32:17

we were living in Miami, which was over 25

32:20

years ago, wondered

32:22

how it was we could make classrooms,

32:26

that is, what we think of as general education

32:28

classrooms, more

32:32

supportive of students who were challenged

32:35

by reading. And how could we set something

32:38

up? So that information text, again,

32:40

thinking about background knowledge and vocabulary,

32:43

was at the center of that. And

32:45

we also knew too many strategies

32:48

means you waste too much of

32:50

students' cognitive processing on learning strategies,

32:52

and not enough on learning reading. So

32:55

with that in mind, we pulled together

32:58

a set of practices

33:01

that would be conducive to

33:03

small-group reading. And

33:05

in which students in

33:07

the group would take on various roles

33:10

to lead the group. So we

33:12

would have a leader. We would have

33:14

a gist expert, which is basically the main

33:16

idea. We would have a

33:19

summarization expert, that would put the gist together.

33:22

And then we would have a clunk expert. And the clunk

33:25

expert was somebody who would help

33:27

the group when words were

33:32

either too difficult because they were multi-syllable or

33:34

because students couldn't

33:37

figure out the meaning.

33:38

Did you say clunk expert? C-L-U-N-K?

33:42

Clunk, not cluck . This is not about chickens

33:45

<laugh>.

33:45

OK, got it <laugh> .

33:48

Yeah . Clunk expert. Because

33:50

we thought of a word

33:52

when you're reading as being a clunk,

33:54

it stopped you dead in your tracks and you

33:56

couldn't go any further . So we

33:59

had a set of strategies that we

34:01

taught related to each of those practices.

34:03

Clunk expert, gist expert,

34:06

summarization expert. And

34:08

we used information text. And students working

34:11

in groups would help each other, because they

34:13

would become experts at these. And

34:15

then we would rotate expertise

34:17

over the course of a year, so

34:19

that, you know, in a six-month period, every student

34:22

in the group would become an expert in

34:24

each one of these things. And the

34:26

idea was that once they practiced

34:28

and became an expert in the

34:30

group, it would generalize to other reading they

34:32

would do throughout the day. And

34:35

the teacher then would model

34:37

these strategies, and support the groups, as students

34:39

were reading it. But the

34:41

initial work that we did was in

34:44

whole-class, general-education settings.

34:47

And we've done many studies.

34:49

Students with disabilities, students who are

34:51

English learners, students who are poor

34:53

readers. And we've done studies in multiple

34:57

states. CSR is consistently associated with

35:00

improved outcomes, to the point where I kind of feel

35:02

like, "Well, maybe I should do another

35:04

CSR study." And actually, I am.

35:06

I just started doing a CSR study

35:08

with my colleague, Elizabeth Swanson and

35:11

Phil Capin. And we're using

35:14

CSR through computer-assisted instruction,

35:16

in which we have texts of

35:19

various levels, easier and harder,

35:21

and in English and in Spanish,

35:24

so that we are using CSR

35:27

through computers. So we're just getting

35:29

started, but we're really excited about the idea.

35:32

Wow! That's really interesting! And I

35:34

have a question for you about the way that the

35:37

strategy is then employed during instruction.

35:39

So, each of these individual experts,

35:42

if you will, are actually there

35:44

though in service of increasing

35:48

the knowledge that the kids are gaining

35:50

from the text, right? So we're putting the content first,

35:53

but these strategies are coming up to support the

35:55

learning of that content. Is that right?

35:57

Yeah.

35:57

So it's not the strategy for strategy's

36:00

sake, right?

36:00

Oh , that's right. Thank you. You always

36:03

do such a nice job summarizing what I've said. I

36:06

appreciate it <laugh>.

36:08

That's really very interesting.

36:11

And so, in your study using computers

36:14

or technology, what makes

36:16

it different than the in-classroom kind

36:18

of stuff that you did previously?

36:21

Well, we're just getting started. This is a brand

36:24

new study that we just had funded. And

36:26

right now we're building the text base, so

36:29

we're trying to get background knowledge. Let's

36:31

say we're talking about the

36:34

history of Egypt, which is

36:36

one of the content areas students need to learn.

36:38

We are building a cadre of

36:41

easier and harder text on every

36:43

topic, so that students can

36:45

use the easier text to

36:47

then access the harder text. The

36:49

idea is that you

36:51

can start with the easier text to kind of

36:54

get some of the background knowledge so that the harder

36:56

text is easier to read. And

36:58

we're also then using the

37:01

strategies of CSR to

37:04

help with comprehension around those texts.

37:08

That makes sense. So, in other words, the

37:11

technology being employed here is in

37:13

relationship to the text?

37:15

Yeah.

37:15

And the increased difficulty of the text. OK.

37:18

Thank you for that clarification. And

37:20

I think this is what we would call, somewhat of, a

37:22

replication study. Is that what

37:25

this might be? Is that we're seeingiIf it can work?

37:27

Or maybe is it in a different context?

37:29

Yeah, I think replication studies can

37:31

be either direct replications, in

37:34

which they literally replicate everything

37:36

that was done in previous studies, or they

37:38

are extended replications. So

37:40

I think of this more as an extended replication.

37:43

Yeah, that makes sense. When was the first time

37:46

that you actually studied CSR?

37:49

Whoa, I'm gonna say it was like in

37:51

'95.

37:53

And the

37:55

reason I say that is, because I think we forget,

37:58

or maybe we don't even realize, maybe some

38:01

newer educators don't even realize how

38:03

often we sort of build on this research.

38:06

Things that we see work, and we replicate,

38:09

like you said, and extend on that replication.

38:11

And continue to work out. That's

38:14

part of the scientific process.

38:16

You know, in fact, back to the Science of

38:18

Reading, which we can't leave behind

38:21

<laugh> , and that is that if you

38:23

look at the early studies from

38:25

the late eighties and early nineties,

38:28

they really were the building blocks for

38:30

phonemic awareness and phonics. And

38:33

the way in which we have identified

38:35

the foundation skills as being essential. So,

38:38

we act like the Science of Reading is something new,

38:40

and we've been building this for decades.

38:43

Yeah, that is a great point. That's a great reminder

38:45

of how long

38:48

this actually takes to get these things right. So,

38:54

does it bother you or surprise

38:56

you that we're still talking about the

38:59

reading wars, or trying to get this instruction

39:01

right in the classroom?

39:05

You know, what bothers me is

39:08

that it has challenged us to

39:11

get this practice up and

39:13

running. I think

39:16

teaching is hard, but I

39:18

hardly think that teaching

39:20

reading is something that we

39:22

have not understood. We've understood

39:25

this for a long time. And

39:29

the principles and practices that

39:31

are so fundamental to what makes

39:33

a difference for students having access

39:35

to print is not

39:37

novel. And any

39:40

arguments and discussions that

39:43

are unreasonable around

39:46

it frustrate me. Yes.

39:48

Well that's coming from

39:50

somebody that's dedicated a

39:52

really long time to this whole

39:55

process. So, thank you for that. I'd

39:57

love to ask you about one more project

40:01

that you worked on called Project PACT.

40:05

Can you tell us a little bit about that?

40:07

You know, Susan, I was just actually working on

40:09

that this morning. Because I was putting a speech

40:12

together about the four

40:14

randomized controlled trials that we've

40:16

done on PACT, which is promoting and

40:19

accelerating comprehension for adolescents.

40:22

And it's through text, it's a text-based

40:25

approach. Again, primarily

40:28

information text. A little bit of hybrid text, and by

40:30

hybrid text I mean narrative and information.

40:32

Kind of like biography is a hybrid text.

40:35

But we like to keep our text nutritious,

40:38

so that students learn things, and build background

40:40

knowledge, and increase their vocabulary. And

40:44

PACT is also a class-wide

40:46

practice, meaning that it's done in

40:48

classrooms with a range of learners. And

40:51

it has been associated with improved outcomes

40:53

for that entire range of learners. So

40:55

we have outcomes for English learners, students

40:57

with reading difficulties , etc. And

41:00

the set of practices really

41:02

starts with what we think of

41:04

as a comprehension canopy. So,

41:07

we sort of think about a unit,

41:09

maybe a week-long or two-week unit, and

41:12

what the big overarching idea

41:16

is to learn about in that unit.

41:18

So, again, building on background knowledge. And

41:21

then within that, what some of the

41:23

key constructs or vocabulary words

41:25

are. So we know we can't teach

41:27

all of 'em, but we wanna teach some of the nuggets

41:30

each week. And then we

41:32

also work very hard to be sure

41:34

that students work in

41:37

teams . So it's team-based learning. It's

41:39

not cooperative learning, which is what CSR

41:42

is. But team-based learning means

41:44

that students demonstrate what

41:46

they learn from the text independently,

41:49

and then they work in their team to give feedback,

41:52

and go back and revisit it. So

41:55

team-based learning, Susan, actually came out

41:58

of universities, in which they were using

42:00

it in things like pharmacy, and nursing,

42:02

and medicine. To make sure that

42:05

individuals as well as groups learned

42:07

all they needed to learn. And we

42:09

applied it to middle school and

42:11

high school, quite successfully. And

42:14

then, of course, there's

42:16

a set of instructional practices very

42:19

similar to the ones in CSR. So

42:21

there's a lot of overlap, because these

42:23

are effective practices, but there's a

42:25

lot of independence as well. This one is much more

42:27

text-based, and much more content,

42:30

background-knowledge-based.

42:32

And when

42:34

you say some of the strategies, can you just paint

42:36

a picture of what it would look like in a

42:38

classroom during a lesson when

42:41

PACT was being utilized?

42:43

Yeah, thank you for that. Well, it's

42:45

kind of hard to do that, because every day focuses

42:48

on something a little different. It is a set of

42:50

practices, but what I can

42:52

say, and we encourage people

42:55

to do this, is go to our website,

42:57

which is really easy to remember. So it's www.meadowscenter.org.

43:03

So the beautiful meadow, only with an "s" on

43:05

it. And we

43:08

have sample lessons there. I

43:11

find it very hard to do without

43:13

pictures, and without instruction, to

43:15

really portray this multiple

43:18

set of practices.

43:19

Well, we will also link the listeners in our show

43:21

notes to that. And just

43:23

to reiterate, this is middle and high

43:25

school. So this is sort of an upper-grades strategy or

43:29

project.

43:30

Yeah. We're working on an elementary

43:32

version of it right now. So we're

43:35

just getting ready to do some of our initial efficacy

43:37

trials . So we'll have more information on that

43:40

in the next year or two.

43:40

Great. Well, we'll follow that on the website.

43:43

Thank you for pointing us in that direction. I

43:45

appreciate it. There's

43:48

been a lot of Dr.

43:50

Sharon Vaughn nuggets <laugh> in the last

43:53

45 minutes, so thank you

43:55

for including all those. I just

43:57

wonder if you have any final thoughts for

43:59

our listeners? Anything that

44:01

you would like to share?

44:03

Well, the one thing I wanna share is

44:05

I wanna thank the listeners, because I feel like I

44:07

preached to the already converted <laugh> . So

44:09

anyone listening to your show already

44:12

probably knows many of the things firsthand

44:15

that we talked about. And sometimes they just

44:17

look for affirmation. And so

44:20

I hope some of the things that we have said today

44:22

provide the kind of affirmation your

44:24

very capable listening group already

44:27

knows.

44:27

Well, thank you for that. And I would

44:29

add to that, for those that are listening,

44:33

share this episode with those folks that

44:35

maybe are looking to

44:37

learn more about the Science of Reading. 'Cause

44:39

Dr. Sharon Vaughn is actually

44:42

one of those pillars of the Science of

44:45

Reading. Someone who's been around. And

44:47

who knows this work. And has done this work. So

44:50

I really appreciate you joining us today. It

44:52

was a real honor, and pleasure, to be able to chat

44:54

with you. And good

44:56

luck on the research that you're

44:59

doing right now. We will definitely keep up on

45:01

that.

45:01

Thank you. And thanks for being such a great interviewer.

45:04

I was lucky to work with you.

45:08

Thanks so much for listening to my conversation

45:10

with Dr. Sharon Vaughn. Dr.

45:13

Vaughn is the Manuel J. Justiz endowed

45:16

chair in education and executive

45:19

director of The Meadow Center for Preventing

45:21

Educational Risk at The University

45:23

of Texas, Austin. Check

45:26

out the show notes for links to some of the resources

45:28

we discussed, including the

45:30

What Works Clearinghouse Practice Guide

45:33

providing reading intervention for students

45:35

in grades four through nine. We'd

45:38

be thrilled to hear your takeaways from this conversation.

45:41

Please add to the conversation in

45:44

our Facebook discussion group, Science

45:46

of Reading: The Community, Science

45:48

of Reading: The Podcast is brought to you by

45:50

Amplify. For more information

45:53

on how Amplify leverages the Science of

45:55

Reading, go to amplify.com/ckla.

46:01

Next time on the show, I'll be joined by

46:03

Dr. Greg Ashman for a fascinating

46:06

conversation about Cognitive Load Theory

46:09

and how it can be applied in an education

46:11

setting.

46:12

People think of memory in quite a reduced

46:15

and limited way. They think about memorizing

46:18

facts or you know, the date of

46:20

a battle and they think that's what memory is. But

46:22

to us, in Cognitive Load Theory, memory is

46:24

a much more expansive thing. So the ability to

46:27

hit a ball in a certain way is actually

46:29

something that is stored in long-term memory.

46:31

That's coming up next time. Don't

46:34

miss that. Or any other upcoming

46:36

episodes by subscribing to Science

46:38

of Reading: The Podcast, wherever you

46:40

find your podcasts. While

46:43

you're there, please consider leaving us a rating

46:45

and review. Thank you again

46:47

for listening.

Rate

Join Podchaser to...

  • Rate podcasts and episodes
  • Follow podcasts and creators
  • Create podcast and episode lists
  • & much more

Episode Tags

Do you host or manage this podcast?
Claim and edit this page to your liking.
,

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features