Podchaser Logo
Home
How to learn from regret, with Robert Leahy, PhD

How to learn from regret, with Robert Leahy, PhD

Released Wednesday, 24th January 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
How to learn from regret, with Robert Leahy, PhD

How to learn from regret, with Robert Leahy, PhD

How to learn from regret, with Robert Leahy, PhD

How to learn from regret, with Robert Leahy, PhD

Wednesday, 24th January 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:02

All of us have regrets. A

0:04

business that fails, a degree we

0:07

never finished, or a friendship we let

0:09

fade away. Sometimes we

0:11

make choices we regret, while

0:13

other times we regret the choices

0:16

we didn't make or the paths we didn't

0:18

pursue. Regret can

0:20

be painful. Who doesn't

0:22

know the sinking feeling that comes with

0:24

saying, if only I hadn't done

0:26

that. But some

0:28

psychologists believe that regret can also

0:30

be productive. By learning

0:32

from our regrets rather than dwelling on them,

0:35

we can make needed changes in our lives and

0:38

set ourselves up to make better decisions in the

0:40

future. So what's

0:42

the difference between productive and

0:44

unproductive regret? Why do

0:46

some people seem to ruminate on their regrets

0:48

more than others? If

0:50

regret is consuming your thoughts, what can you do

0:53

about it? And in this

0:55

age of social media and fear of missing

0:57

out, do people have more regrets than

0:59

they used to? Welcome

1:01

to Speaking of Psychology, the

1:03

flagship podcast of the American

1:05

Psychological Association that examines the

1:08

links between psychological science and

1:10

everyday life. I'm Kim

1:12

Mills. My

1:16

guest today is Dr. Robert Leahy,

1:18

founder and director of the American Institute

1:21

for Cognitive Therapy in New York City

1:23

and a clinical professor of psychology in

1:26

the Department of Psychiatry at Weill

1:28

Cornell Medical College. He

1:30

is the past president of the Association for

1:32

Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies, the

1:34

International Association of Cognitive Psychotherapy,

1:37

and the Academy of Cognitive Therapy. Dr.

1:40

Leahy is the author or editor of 29 books

1:42

for clinicians and

1:44

the general public. His latest

1:46

book is called, If Only, Finding

1:49

Freedom from Regret, and

1:51

it is all about learning to

1:53

understand regret and make it a

1:55

tool for self-knowledge and change. Dr.

1:58

Leahy, thank you for joining me. If

2:00

you came for having me and poop from

2:03

you don't regret having be on the show.

2:06

I don't think I will. Let's

2:09

talk about what we mean by

2:11

regret is regret and emotion. Away.

2:14

As thinking little boss.

2:17

School so it's a little about

2:19

both. Are you read? Read is

2:22

certainly a way of thinking is

2:24

say us a sense of. Motion

2:27

of disappointments oh or

2:30

remorse Were sadness about

2:32

an action taken are

2:35

not taking. Ah,

2:37

or what we might anticipate. Oh

2:39

we might feel if we. Take.

2:42

Action or do not a connection.

2:44

So since both cognitive and emotional.

2:48

Indices interesting about regret Kim

2:50

is that. In

2:52

Nz and there is no into

2:54

clinical literature, but the really is

2:57

very little. On. Regret that

2:59

I came across the my

3:01

reading the news. A lot

3:03

on regrets and behavioral economics.

3:05

Decision making decisions processes. A

3:07

priest the second most commonly

3:09

mentioned a motion. In

3:12

conversations that college students have.

3:14

Loves being the first so

3:17

on. so it's a pervasive

3:19

emotion. It's. Often

3:21

an emotion. That. People

3:24

ruminate about. Ah, In

3:26

emotion that can linger on. Ah,

3:29

That for decades something I've

3:31

talked to people in their

3:33

nineties who regret. Decisions.

3:35

They made when they were in their

3:37

twenties. so is some. So. Fascinating

3:40

emotion and looking forwards and talking

3:42

about it today. What's

3:44

the difference between productive

3:46

and unproductive regret? For

3:49

a a lot of people, especially

3:51

on social media, were is always

3:53

the positive, the power of positive

3:55

thinking. Roth's regret is. Ah,

3:57

I'm. says. it is

3:59

an emotion, like all emotions evolved

4:02

because they were adaptive. So how can regret

4:05

be adaptive? It certainly can

4:07

be maladaptive. It can lead

4:09

to rumination, depression, self-criticism, resentment.

4:14

But it can be adaptive if you use

4:16

it in the right way.

4:18

So for example, productive regret

4:20

would be an ability

4:22

to learn from my mistakes

4:25

or to anticipate learning from

4:27

my mistakes. So for

4:29

example, if

4:31

you ask young people to

4:34

think about what they're going to live on

4:36

when they're in their 60s toward

4:38

the end of their working career, how much

4:40

money they're going to have, that

4:43

increases plan

4:45

savings. Half the

4:47

people, for example, who are prescribed

4:51

medication for hypertension a year

4:53

later don't take the medication. But

4:55

if you ask them to think about what their

4:58

life would be like if they have a

5:00

stroke and they're paralyzed or unable

5:02

to speak, whatever, that

5:04

significantly increases the compliance

5:07

with medication. So

5:10

regret can be used productively if you look

5:12

back and you think, gee, what did I

5:14

do that in the

5:16

future I might do differently? That's

5:19

like a self-correction type thing. Unproductive

5:23

regret or maladaptive regret is

5:26

characterized not by self-correction but

5:29

by self-criticism. And

5:31

in fact, in some cases,

5:34

self-loathing, self-hatred. It's

5:37

unproductive regrets characterized by dwelling on

5:40

it over and over and over

5:43

and not viewing it as an opportunity

5:45

to learn from experience. I mean, if you

5:47

think about regret

5:49

as you

5:52

carried out an experiment, you did

5:54

something or you chose not to do something and

5:57

you're not satisfied with the consequences. Well,

6:00

we as psychologists are always carrying

6:02

out experiments, but we

6:05

in our ordinary lives carry out

6:07

experiments. You carry out

6:10

the experiments of bringing up the

6:13

most provocative political

6:15

statement on Thanksgiving dinner

6:17

with your family, and you

6:19

find out that that experiment didn't work very

6:22

well. What did you learn from that

6:24

experiment? What's

6:26

interesting to me is

6:28

there are some people who don't

6:30

seem to learn from their mistakes

6:34

or don't anticipate their mistakes.

6:37

So, for example, people who

6:39

abuse drugs, who overeat, who don't

6:41

take their medication, who

6:43

engage in unsafe sex, who

6:45

say inappropriate things, they

6:48

don't seem to anticipate the

6:50

regret. They're not using

6:52

what I would call prospective

6:55

regret or anticipatory regret in

6:57

a useful way. People

7:00

who are manic, who think they can do

7:02

anything, that they have all the

7:04

powers in the world and they're

7:06

too sexy for their own clothes, they

7:09

don't anticipate regret.

7:13

They're a little bit too over

7:15

the top with their confidence. So

7:19

it's an important thing to use

7:23

regret productively. I think the

7:26

most economical way, Kim, to

7:29

use regret is to

7:31

use the regress of other people. I

7:33

know when I was in my early 20s, a

7:37

lot of my age peers were

7:39

misusing drugs or misusing alcohol.

7:45

The only thing I really have of any

7:48

value, because I had no money, I

7:50

was on a fellowship, the only thing I have of any value

7:52

is my brain. I'm

7:55

not going to endanger my brain

7:58

or my health. some

8:00

of these people have. So it's using

8:02

the regrets of other people in

8:05

a productive way by

8:07

learning from their mistakes. So

8:10

there is a kind of inoculation then

8:12

for certain people if you're capable of

8:14

visualizing what your future might be like

8:16

if you did something or didn't do

8:19

something. Exactly. I like the word

8:21

inoculation Kim. It's kind of like there's a

8:23

lot of talk about living

8:26

in the present moment, which

8:29

sounds so comfortable.

8:32

But the only creature that lives entirely

8:34

in the present moment is

8:36

a mosquito. So I

8:39

like to think about living in all

8:42

the moments of my life, thinking about

8:44

what I did

8:47

when I was younger, the mistakes I made, the

8:49

things I did right, but

8:51

also thinking about my future self. So

8:54

for example, one of the techniques that we

8:57

use in cognitive behavior therapy is

9:00

ask yourself what would

9:02

your future self say? I often

9:04

do role plays with patients. So

9:07

what would your future self say

9:10

about over drinking or trying to

9:12

malign your boss or spending

9:19

all this money or whatever it is?

9:21

What would your future self say? Not

9:23

your immediate self, your future self, because

9:25

your future self may

9:28

be the wise self. And

9:30

that's something that I think

9:34

that's something that

9:36

people often

9:38

don't recognize that

9:41

consequences will follow from

9:43

actions that you take or do not take. Is

9:47

there a difference in the degree

9:49

of regret between something that you've

9:51

done and regretting something that you didn't do,

9:53

you know, the road not taken? Is one

9:56

type of regret more difficult to learn from

9:58

than the other? Well, it's interesting. because

10:00

in the short term, we

10:02

tend to have more hot

10:06

regret or emotional intensity

10:08

of regret for actions

10:10

taken. So,

10:14

we may

10:17

be correct that if we

10:19

take this action, we

10:21

may immediately after have some

10:23

regrets. For example, buyer's remorse,

10:26

you buy a car, you buy an

10:28

apartment, or a house, a very common

10:30

thing is right after you think, what

10:32

was I thinking? I'm putting all this

10:36

money down, whatever. So, this

10:39

is called the action effect. We

10:41

have more regrets for action taken

10:43

in the short term. But

10:46

in the long term, as people look back

10:48

on their lives, they

10:51

tend to regret what they

10:53

did not do. And this

10:56

is true cross-culturally, in other cultures

10:59

that have been studied. We

11:02

tend to regret things we did not do. We

11:04

do not pursue that course

11:07

of action, or that education,

11:09

or that relationship, or that

11:12

investment. So,

11:14

that tends to be less of

11:17

an intense passionate regret. It

11:19

tends to be more of

11:21

a lingering, unpleasant

11:24

feeling. But people can ruminate

11:27

about that for months,

11:29

years, decades. Well, let's

11:31

talk about ruminating for a minute because some

11:33

people seem to get really stuck dwelling

11:35

on their regrets and living and

11:38

reliving what they did or didn't

11:40

do. Are there some personality characteristics

11:42

that lead some people to experience

11:45

more regret or more painful regret?

11:47

Sure. Yeah, so we

11:49

know that rumination is a fundamental part

11:51

of depression. And

11:53

in fact, the research by the

11:56

late Nolan Hoxema at Yale shows

11:58

that people who ruminate or more likely

12:01

to get depressed and stay depressed. Having

12:05

said that, in terms of tying

12:07

in regret with rumination, there are certain

12:10

ways of looking at the world that

12:13

lead to more rumination. One

12:16

is to have

12:18

inflexible expectations that

12:22

this is what I expected, it wasn't

12:24

what turned out and

12:27

so I'm going to just dwell on

12:29

it. The other is the

12:32

tendency to not

12:34

accept trade-offs. In

12:37

my view, I live in New York City and

12:40

I know Kim that you live in New York City at

12:42

one time. When

12:44

you live in New York City, you've got

12:46

to accept trade-offs. Again,

12:49

standing for an elevator is a

12:52

major trade-off negotiation. Where's

12:54

my closet? Where's

12:57

my backyard? Exactly.

13:05

When people get married, they end

13:07

up getting divorced over closet space,

13:09

not over arguments. Some

13:16

people have a hard time accepting

13:19

trade-offs. It's

13:22

like there

13:24

are two ways of thinking that I think contribute

13:27

to rumination and

13:30

regret. One is that I call pure

13:32

mind, that my mind should be

13:34

pure. For example, I should

13:36

never be ambivalent. For example, I

13:38

was talking with a young

13:41

man who had been dating

13:44

and actually living with this woman for

13:46

several years. He said, I

13:48

don't know, how can I get married if

13:51

I have mixed feelings? He

13:53

equated ambivalence with,

13:55

it's not a good decision. I

13:58

said, first of all, the word decision. in

14:00

Latin means to cut away from. And

14:03

it means that you have mixed feelings. That's

14:05

what a decision is. Second,

14:08

one reason you have mixed feelings is that

14:10

you know each other. I

14:13

mean, Romeo and Juliet is a

14:15

wonderful fantasy story, but it

14:17

would not be a great way of basing your

14:19

future commitment to

14:22

your life partner. Within five

14:24

days, there are several people dead, including

14:26

Romeo and Juliet, based on a glance

14:29

at a party without

14:31

any conversation at the

14:33

time. So ambivalence may

14:35

be a sign

14:38

of reality testing, rather than the

14:40

idea that my mind should be absolutely

14:43

pure, my emotions should always

14:45

be positive. We have to

14:47

recognize that life is filled

14:49

with complexity and nuance, and

14:52

that ambivalence is simply recognition,

14:54

an honest recognition of that.

14:58

It's like Salieri, a

15:01

famous composer around the time of Mozart,

15:03

said too many notes.

15:06

But you know, Mozart had just

15:08

the right of notes, the right number of

15:10

notes, whatever. We know

15:12

that now. Yeah, he's

15:15

pretty good, that Mozart

15:17

guy. Maybe not

15:19

as great as Taylor Swift, but you

15:21

know, we can't have everything in life.

15:24

But so ambivalence, the intolerance of ambivalence

15:27

contributes to

15:30

regret and rumination. In

15:35

any decision, you know, people think about

15:37

what is the risk of

15:39

staying, what is the risk of changing? All

15:43

decisions are risk versus risk. So

15:46

some people look at life as,

15:48

I'm going to try to find something

15:50

where there's no risk, you

15:53

know. Every option has

15:55

a risk. So for example, if

15:57

you put your Money. Into.

16:01

A savings account and get close

16:03

to zero percent. Interest.

16:06

but you the risk you take

16:08

as you lose against inflation and

16:10

you lose against see opportunity to

16:12

make money if you invested or

16:15

put it in bonds not invested

16:17

in the Us equities. Mom. For.

16:20

You put it in the neck. We

16:22

equities. He run the risk of the

16:24

stock market crash to lose risk versus

16:26

risk. If you

16:28

get married, there's a risk it

16:30

won't work out. If you don't

16:32

get read married, there's a rescue.

16:35

Be lonely or whatever is comparing

16:37

the wrists assault? Looking for a

16:39

risky free alternative on. The.

16:42

On the other part of what

16:44

leads people seem to. Have

16:46

difficulty making decisions on

16:49

is the degree to

16:51

which we. Didn't. Have

16:53

this especially true with people who are

16:55

depressed the degree to which we. Tend

16:57

to have a bias to predict negatives. So.

17:01

I was talking with somebody

17:03

who very, very smart, very

17:05

successful insect, very wealthy, who.

17:08

Keeps almost all his money in. Your

17:12

Cds and in money markets

17:14

are rather than. Investing

17:16

in the stock market now

17:18

on. That. Might be good. Or

17:21

new season, but in the long

17:23

term plans probably not a good

17:25

strategy. But like other things in

17:28

this person's life, he was risk

17:30

averse in Urdu. Should I leave,

17:32

my partner, should I, you know,

17:34

make decisions, whatever. it is. So

17:36

accepting. Except the reasonable

17:39

risk. For. part

17:42

of making decisions and is also

17:44

part of living with the consequences

17:46

so if the consequences one of

17:48

the things that makes it hard

17:50

for people and least some to

17:52

regress to having mom more regret

17:54

and rumination is he said to

17:57

see they have to idealize the

17:59

alternative and to discount

18:01

what they have. So,

18:04

for example, people who regret

18:07

my also, if I had married this other person

18:09

or pursued this other career, I would be

18:11

so much happier. I

18:15

remember a few years ago thinking, I

18:18

know when I was in high school, everybody thought I was going to be a

18:20

lawyer. I thought I

18:22

was going to be a lawyer, but I decided

18:24

to be a psychologist and I'm pretty happy with

18:26

that. But I thought, maybe I should

18:28

have been a lawyer and all that. And then

18:30

I began thinking, gee, I know a lot of

18:32

lawyers and they have one of the

18:34

highest rates of depression. It's

18:38

such an adversarial. I'm not

18:40

maligning lawyers, but I think

18:42

for me, being a

18:45

psychologist has been a good choice.

18:48

But we tend to idealize the alternatives. And

18:51

people who regret often idealize

18:54

the alternatives. They have the fear of missing

18:56

out. Somebody

18:58

recently published an article on the joy of missing

19:00

out. So it's kind of like... It's

19:04

another way to look at it. Thank

19:07

God I'm not at 42nd Street at 12 midnight New Year's

19:09

Eve. Thank

19:14

God I missed out on that. So

19:19

there's this sort of tendency to

19:21

idealize the alternatives and

19:23

then to discount what you have. One

19:26

way of countering

19:28

the idea of... There's

19:30

several ways of countering the idea of

19:32

living with what you have is

19:35

to imagine if you had nothing. And

19:41

this is, I think, something that in

19:44

an affluent and very ambitious

19:46

society, we often don't spend

19:50

enough time with gratitude.

19:53

So for example, I walk to

19:56

my office. I live on the

19:58

Upper East Side. My office... This is

20:00

on 58th Street. And

20:04

even if I walk down 3rd Avenue, I'll

20:07

see people who are homeless. I saw an

20:10

elderly woman who was homeless pushing a

20:12

cart and stuff. And

20:15

two emotions that can

20:17

counter regret are

20:20

one is compassion for

20:23

somebody who has so little

20:25

and the other is gratitude for

20:28

the fact that I'm actually able to walk

20:31

down 3rd Avenue and go

20:33

to a nice office and

20:35

talk to you today. So

20:39

it's kind of like

20:41

we don't really have the kind of

20:43

culture of gratitude. One

20:50

thing that's interesting, Kim, is there's

20:54

a search tool called Google Ngram

20:57

that you can look, you can type

20:59

in Google Ngram and

21:02

you'll come up with a search engine. And

21:04

on the search engine, you can type in a

21:06

word and see how often

21:10

that word is used in the printed

21:12

language over the last 150 years.

21:16

So if we look at regret, regret

21:19

had an incredible increase in

21:22

the frequency in English language printed

21:26

books and documents between 1980 and 2019.

21:31

And what happened during that time was

21:34

a dramatic increase in

21:37

economic inequality, a dramatic

21:40

increase in perfectionism and surveys

21:42

that were taken from

21:44

1989 to 2016. And

21:51

a dramatic increase, I think,

21:53

in the idea that I should be

21:55

able to do everything. So

21:58

regret is the opportunity. emotion. And

22:01

we often think that opportunity

22:03

is always a wonderful thing, but

22:05

opportunity is also leads

22:07

you to think, well, this

22:09

person is so successful. Why

22:12

am I not successful? Why am

22:14

I not extremely wealthy

22:16

or famous or whatever? So

22:19

we have this, I mean, it's a dramatic

22:21

increase in 1980 on, you find the same

22:23

thing, by the way, in

22:25

Spanish literature in

22:27

Russia. So you have this dramatic

22:30

increase in economic inequality,

22:33

globalization, alienation,

22:35

perfectionism, social

22:38

media kicking in 20 years ago,

22:40

where it gives you the social

22:42

comparison. How many people are

22:44

comparing themselves to the homeless,

22:47

elderly woman on Third Avenue?

22:50

They're comparing themselves

22:53

to Elon Musk,

22:56

who by the way, doesn't seem to be a very

22:58

happy person with his $240 billion. But

23:03

I mean, they're even comparing themselves to

23:06

their peers, but because people use social media

23:08

in such a way as to always present

23:10

something beautiful. This is the lovely dinner I

23:12

had last night. This is the vacation I

23:14

took, whereas their life might be miserable, but

23:17

you don't know that. Exactly.

23:19

Yeah, like the humble brag, I'm so

23:21

humbled, I'm so outstanding and having a

23:23

wonderful time. No,

23:26

you're not humble. But

23:32

let me actually bring up humility. One of

23:34

the concepts I describe in my book,

23:37

if only, Finding Freedom from Regret, is

23:40

what I call adaptive

23:43

humility. This is

23:45

a concept in the literature on

23:47

humility that I find very appealing.

23:49

Adaptive humility is not I'm a

23:52

doormat and I deserve nothing. That's

23:54

not what adaptive humility is. Adaptive

23:56

humility is I'm just another human

23:58

being. I'm not

24:01

special. I don't deserve any special

24:03

treatment. I can make mistakes. I

24:06

can be wrong. I can apologize. And

24:10

what we know is that from the

24:12

research on humility, that

24:17

people who are viewed

24:19

as having humility, people

24:21

who are viewed as sincerely

24:24

apologizing, are trusted

24:26

more, have better relationships, have

24:28

better friendships. And people

24:30

want to work with them. And I'm

24:33

sure many people listening to this can

24:35

think of people they

24:37

know who never apologize. Adaptive

24:40

humility is, I made a mistake. I

24:44

said something that wasn't fair. I

24:48

really felt bad about that. I want to

24:50

apologize. I hope you accept my

24:52

apology. But

24:54

in a sense, really,

24:57

you can't say, you

24:59

owe me an apology. It's

25:02

kind of like simply asking for an apology

25:06

is not the currency to

25:08

buy an apology. But

25:11

adaptive humility allows you to

25:13

live with mistakes, allows

25:15

you to anticipate mistakes, allows

25:19

you to use

25:21

regret productively, allows

25:23

you to have

25:26

gratitude for what you have. I

25:28

mean, I grew up very poor in

25:31

a housing project. I mean, it

25:33

was a tough neighborhood.

25:36

But I'm

25:38

not glamorizing poverty.

25:41

But I think it led

25:44

me to, one, to have

25:46

compassion for people who have

25:48

less. Not

25:51

to be an elitist with

25:53

people who don't

25:56

have high status positions

25:58

to treat everybody. as an

26:01

equal to

26:05

be able to live as

26:07

a human being, not as some

26:09

character above the crowd. But

26:13

in your work, you talk about

26:15

the sunk cost effect when it

26:17

comes to regret. What

26:19

do you mean by that? And how can we

26:22

overcome that cost to make better choices so that

26:24

we might have fewer regrets in the future? Yeah,

26:27

so the sunk cost effect, I think we

26:29

all can identify with this. You

26:33

buy something, you spend a lot of money, you buy

26:36

a dress or a jacket or

26:38

whatever, and you

26:40

wear it once, you put it in your closet,

26:42

and it's there for 5 or 10 years. And

26:47

you or your partner say, why don't you throw

26:49

that out? You haven't worn that

26:52

jacket, Bob, in 10 years. Oh,

26:54

I paid good money for it. So

26:57

it's what's called a sunk cost. In

26:59

other words, I've already put the

27:01

cost in, I've already paid the good

27:03

money, but now it's not

27:06

useful to me because I'm not using it.

27:09

And I'm only kidding myself. And plus, I'm taking

27:11

up room in the closet. Plus, I

27:13

could give it away to somebody else who could use it. But

27:17

we have humans are the only

27:20

living creature that gets

27:22

trapped by sunk costs. I mean, cats

27:24

and dogs and insects and birds, monkeys,

27:28

they're not trying to justify their past decisions.

27:32

They're not saying, well, I'm going to hold on

27:34

to that banana for the next five months because

27:37

I worked hard to get that

27:40

banana. When

27:42

it's no longer useful, they move on to something else.

27:45

But humans are self-reflective. We want to

27:47

make sense of our decisions. We

27:49

think that, oh, if I throw

27:51

that piece of clothing out, I'm

27:54

going to regret it. I'll miss it.

27:57

I'll be wasting. One

28:00

exercise I use when I

28:02

give workshops is, let's imagine I hold up a $100 bill

28:05

and I said,

28:07

you know, I'm not going to spend

28:09

this on myself. I'm not going to give this to anybody.

28:12

But what I'd like to do is burn it in front of people.

28:16

Most people watching me burn it would be

28:18

really angry with me because they

28:20

don't want to see me wasting a $100 bill.

28:24

And we don't, you know, it's like when your mother says

28:26

to you and you're 10 years old,

28:28

eat everything on your plate, you know, there

28:30

are people somewhere else in the world starving.

28:33

You know, it's this human thing

28:36

about wasting and regret. So

28:40

what can you do with the sunk costs? Like

28:43

for example, you can have sunk costs about a

28:45

relationship or a career. You

28:48

know, some people start out

28:50

a career and they hate it after a few

28:52

years. And they say, oh, I can't give

28:54

it up because I put several years of training. I

28:57

can't walk away from all that. That's the

28:59

sunk cost. The question is, is the future

29:01

going to be useful and is it going

29:03

to be pleasurable and meaningful? When

29:08

I left academics, because I wanted

29:10

to pursue a clinical career, the

29:12

husband of one of my colleagues

29:15

said, Bob is leaving academics

29:17

and research. What a waste of a

29:19

good mind. And

29:22

I think maybe he had a point. I should have listened to

29:24

him. But you

29:26

know, it's sort of

29:28

like the idea that we

29:31

have to justify our past

29:33

investment. For example, people will hold

29:35

onto a stock and say, well, I paid $100 for

29:37

the stock. It

29:40

went down to $60. It will

29:42

come back. No,

29:44

your money died and went to

29:46

heaven. You know, it's not coming back. It's

29:49

not going to be resurrected. They

29:52

go to the cemetery, rest in peace. That's

29:55

your investment. But

29:58

it's something that humans do. So what

30:00

we should think about, one is what

30:03

is the future utility of this? What

30:06

is the cost of my keeping it?

30:08

What is the benefit? Second, like

30:10

with a relationship, if I stay in

30:12

this bad relationship for another year, what

30:16

opportunities will I miss? If

30:19

I stay in a bad marriage

30:21

or whatever, I'll miss the opportunity of

30:23

finding a new relationship or the opportunity

30:25

of being happy on my own. I'll

30:29

miss the opportunity of not having

30:31

arguments with my partner. So

30:34

we think about opportunity costs. We

30:36

think about the error of trying to

30:38

justify the past rather than trying

30:40

to make the future better. So

30:43

your decision should always be about

30:45

the future self and future

30:47

utility. But we often think,

30:49

well, people look at me and think, oh, what an

30:51

idiot, you know, he wasted

30:53

or she wasted all that

30:56

time. I

30:58

think when people see you getting out

31:01

of a bad situation, they

31:03

often think, well, it's about time, you

31:06

know, good for you. But if they don't, what

31:09

kind of friend is that? If they

31:11

want you to stay in a bad

31:13

situation? The other thing is, what advice

31:15

would you give to somebody else? So

31:17

we're really, really good at telling

31:20

other people, get the

31:22

hell out of that relationship, or

31:26

change your career or change your

31:28

job or whatever. We're

31:31

really good at telling people to

31:33

make a change because we don't have

31:35

to justify their decision.

31:38

It's not, this is like cognitive

31:40

dissonance. We don't have to justify

31:42

for them why

31:44

they made that decision. So

31:47

yeah, the sunk cost is very powerful. And your

31:49

cat or your dog or whatever

31:51

pets you have or birds

31:54

or whatever, they're not sitting

31:57

there ruminating about what

31:59

will other birds do. or cats think of me and

32:02

I'll just cruise, I'm a bad

32:04

decision maker. As far as we

32:06

know, they're not doing that. I

32:09

won't be able to get into the college of

32:11

my choice. Yeah. Cats

32:17

only have four cognitions. This

32:19

feels good, this doesn't feel good. I want

32:21

that and what's next? So

32:29

to shift gears a little bit here,

32:31

let me ask you this. How early

32:33

in life does regret manifest? What

32:36

age do children begin to feel

32:38

regret and does that change as

32:40

they get older? Yeah,

32:42

so it's interesting. The kids

32:45

begin showing evidence of regret between

32:48

the ages of four and seven. And

32:51

the kids who express regret turn

32:53

out to be better at making

32:55

decisions because they learn from

32:58

their mistakes or they anticipate mistakes.

33:01

Second, they're better at regulating their

33:04

emotions because they think, well,

33:06

if I act out in this way, maybe

33:08

I'll regret it. Or if I

33:10

do this, my emotions will become dysregulated.

33:15

As people get toward the end of life, when

33:18

you look at the regrets of people in hospice,

33:20

people who may be dying in the next month

33:23

or few months or whatever, they're

33:25

not having regrets. I wish I spent

33:28

more time on social media or I

33:31

wish I got the bargains at

33:33

Bloomingdale's at the end of the Christmas

33:35

season. They're not thinking about that. The

33:38

regrets that they express, I wish it had

33:40

been more true to my emotions. I

33:43

wish I had told people I love them. I

33:45

wish I had pursued things I really wanted to do.

33:48

And basically living according to

33:51

my values and according to

33:53

the human relationships. And I think

33:55

that's an important thing to listen to

33:57

people as they get toward the end

33:59

of life. life. What they've learned,

34:02

they have a lot of wisdom about

34:05

what matters. And so, if

34:08

you look at the regrets of people in

34:10

hospice, that's what we see.

34:12

We don't see materialism and

34:14

status and, you know,

34:18

proving winning arguments. That's

34:22

not in the regret repertoire of

34:24

elderly people. In fact, elderly people

34:26

have a positivity bias. They

34:29

may have more long-term regrets because

34:31

they've been around for a long time. But

34:35

on a day-to-day basis, they more

34:37

easily distance themselves from regrets.

34:39

They just kind of move on. All

34:41

right, so that didn't work out last week.

34:44

I'm going to move on, you know, every

34:46

day's gift. Do men

34:48

and women experience regret differently?

34:51

So, men and women, and again, I think

34:53

this is going to change as gender roles

34:56

become more integrated,

34:58

let's say. Men

35:01

have more regrets in

35:03

the research about achievement and

35:06

materialism. And

35:09

women have more regrets about relationship

35:12

and having sex too

35:14

early in a relationship. Having

35:16

said that, men

35:19

and women have a lot of the same

35:21

regrets. When we

35:23

look at cultural differences, Americans

35:26

have more regrets about

35:29

romance, about school, about

35:32

education, career. Whereas

35:36

people in Asian culture,

35:40

like in Taiwan or Japan, more

35:43

likely to have regrets about

35:45

family and about relationships and

35:47

about interpersonal things. There's

35:51

a lot of overlap. It's not like either

35:54

war. So,

35:57

it's kind of like thinking about what

35:59

your regrets are all

36:01

about. What do they say

36:03

to you about it? But

36:06

I think going back to what you mentioned earlier, Kim,

36:08

I do think that social media,

36:13

that regret is often tied into

36:15

social comparison. Not

36:17

always, but it's often tied into, well,

36:19

look at that person. They had a

36:21

patient who was

36:27

doing fairly well in his career,

36:31

but had

36:33

not accumulated as much wealth as

36:35

people he went to college with, regretting

36:40

where he was or having

36:42

envy, whatever. But I said,

36:44

well, let's look at what you do have. And he

36:46

had a great relationship with his partner, great

36:49

relationship with his kids, he's healthy, he's

36:51

a good person. And I

36:54

said, do you think

36:57

there'd be people who would be envious of you? So we

37:00

often don't think that when we regret

37:03

things, we don't think, gee, a lot of

37:05

people might be envious of me. So

37:08

it's a paradox, I

37:10

think, that the

37:13

social comparison, I think,

37:15

should be geared

37:18

to gratitude and compassion.

37:20

And I've never

37:22

met somebody, maybe these people

37:24

never go to therapy, but I've never met somebody

37:27

who said, you know, Dr. Leahy,

37:29

you really got to help me because I have

37:32

too much gratitude and compassion. You

37:35

know, I don't know, it's getting the best of me.

37:40

Yeah, we got to change that. Yeah,

37:44

on the other hand, I know someone who

37:47

likes to tell people that she regrets no

37:49

choices that she ever made in her life,

37:51

which I find really hard to believe. Do

37:53

you encounter, are there really such people who

37:55

say, yeah, everything I did, I was right.

37:57

Yeah, right. Yeah, I say nonsense. know,

38:00

it's like you're

38:03

looking in the wrong mirror. I think

38:05

that's part of our contemporary social

38:11

meme. I have no regrets because every

38:14

decision I made was mine. I live

38:16

with the consequences. How are you

38:18

going to learn from your mistakes? Why

38:20

would you ever apologize if

38:22

you don't have regrets? Imagine

38:25

you were looking for a life partner. You're

38:28

looking for a life partner and you meet

38:30

somebody and they say, oh, I really like

38:32

you, I'm an attractive you, you're a wonderful

38:34

person, but I think you need to know

38:36

something about me. I'm incapable

38:39

of regret and apology, right?

38:43

Not unlike some politicians. Well,

38:48

I don't know. For a lot of people, that might be a

38:50

red flag because you

38:52

want people who have

38:54

some regrets and some apologies. That's

38:57

how you heal relationships. Are

39:00

there any big areas in this aspect of

39:02

psychology that you feel are under research that

39:04

we still need to know answers about? Well,

39:07

I think that, I mean,

39:10

I think we really need to look at

39:12

a lot of cultural

39:14

issues and regret, a lot

39:16

of LBGT issues and regret.

39:18

And there's some recent

39:21

research on that, like

39:23

transgender affirming surgery,

39:26

only 1% of

39:28

patients who have that express

39:32

regret. Despite what

39:35

the critics may say of transgender

39:38

and gender affirming, only 1%,

39:40

whereas people who

39:43

have cosmetic surgery, a

39:45

significant percentage have regrets. So,

39:47

you know, are you more likely

39:49

to have regret about a

39:52

nose job than a transgender? Yes,

39:54

by factors, by

39:57

many factors, right? So,

40:01

the facts can often confuse your thinking.

40:03

But, yeah,

40:05

so I think we need to really look at

40:08

a lot of the cultural

40:11

and demographic differences

40:13

and group differences in regret. And

40:17

then how people spontaneously

40:20

deal with regret. I mean, a lot of times,

40:22

if you're a therapist, you listen to a patient and you think,

40:26

gee, that's a great idea and I wish I thought of

40:28

it. And then you realize, yeah,

40:30

my wife has been telling me that for five years.

40:33

Why am I more likely to listen

40:35

to a patient than my partner? You

40:37

know? So, a lot of times people

40:39

come up with their spontaneous cures

40:44

or strategies or techniques.

40:47

Therapists don't have a monopoly on wisdom. And

40:50

the question is, even if they have any

40:52

wisdom, but they don't have a monopoly on

40:54

it. So, I'm looking at how

40:56

people make the best of what

40:58

they have. And what

41:01

I'm struck by, I'm not a religious

41:03

person. I was raised Catholic, but I'm

41:05

not practicing anything. But I

41:08

had a Orthodox Jewish

41:10

patient who said,

41:12

you know, Bob, you know the mazza, right

41:14

outside the threshold of the door,

41:17

you see an apartments in New York. I

41:20

touched that when I leave the

41:22

apartment to remind me of

41:24

God's presence throughout the day. Now,

41:27

even though I'm not a believer, I

41:29

thought, what a beautiful reminder of

41:32

gratitude and humility to take throughout

41:34

the day. And

41:36

I don't think you have to be like a

41:38

Zen warrior to think this way. But

41:41

I think there's a lot to be

41:43

said for recognizing life has trade-offs.

41:46

You're never going to get everything you want on

41:48

your terms. So forget about demanding

41:50

it. Be flexible

41:53

about your expectations. Recognize

41:56

your mind is not going to be pure mind. It's

41:59

going to be a color. light of scope filled with noise.

42:03

And you can be ambivalent about your

42:05

partner for the rest of your life and still

42:07

have a good relationship because all

42:09

your friends who know you are ambivalent

42:11

about you because they know you. And

42:15

that to me is living in the

42:18

real world. And regret

42:20

is always about a fictional

42:23

world. It's about what

42:27

psychologists call a counterfactual

42:29

world. What does counterfactual

42:31

mean? Against the facts.

42:34

It means it's not a fact.

42:36

It's what you think could be.

42:38

It's a possibility emotion as

42:40

opposed to living in the real world and making

42:42

the best of what you

42:44

have while still trying to

42:46

do better. It doesn't mean you are

42:49

resigned and give up. You

42:51

can always do better. You can always grow. But

42:55

we often do recognize how fortunate

42:57

we are until we

42:59

talk to people living in other parts of

43:01

the world who are suffering or

43:04

see people who are disabled

43:06

and struggling just to get down the

43:08

street and to live. Dr.

43:12

Leahy, I want to thank you for chatting with

43:14

me today. I found this to be very interesting

43:17

and informative. And I think that our listeners will

43:19

learn a great deal from it. Thank you. Well,

43:21

Kim, thank you so much for having me. And

43:23

I really sincerely appreciate your having me on. Hope

43:25

you have a great day. Thank

43:28

you. You can find

43:30

previous episodes of Speaking of

43:32

Psychology on our website at

43:35

www.speakingofpsychology.org or on Apple,

43:37

Spotify, YouTube, or wherever you get

43:39

your podcasts. If you have comments

43:41

or ideas for future podcasts, you

43:43

can email us at speakingofpsychology at

43:46

apa.org. Speaking of Psychology

43:48

is produced by Lee Wineman. Our

43:50

sound editor is Chris Condayan. Thank

43:52

you for listening. For the American

43:54

Psychological Association, I'm Kim Mills. for

44:02

which 30 John

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features