Podchaser Logo
Home
From the Vault: The Moses Illusion

From the Vault: The Moses Illusion

Released Saturday, 26th February 2022
Good episode? Give it some love!
From the Vault: The Moses Illusion

From the Vault: The Moses Illusion

From the Vault: The Moses Illusion

From the Vault: The Moses Illusion

Saturday, 26th February 2022
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:05

Hey, welcome to Stuff to Blow Your Mind. This

0:08

is Robert Lamb and I'm Joe McCormick,

0:10

and today we're bringing you an episode from the Vault.

0:13

This one it was about a psychological

0:15

effect called the Moses illusion. It

0:17

originally published February.

0:20

All right, let's jump right in Welcome

0:25

to Stuff to Blow Your Mind production of My Heart

0:28

Radio. Hey,

0:35

welcome to Stuff to Blow your Mind. My name is Robert

0:37

Lamb and I'm Joe McCormick,

0:39

and today we're going to be talking

0:42

about an interesting observation in

0:44

cognitive psychology that deals

0:46

with language that starts off

0:48

as kind of just a funny little quirk about

0:51

the way we process certain kinds of sentences

0:54

but ends up having some some broader and

0:56

more interesting implications about knowledge

0:58

and language and thought both. The best

1:00

way to start here would just be to

1:03

illustrate the prime

1:05

example of the effect we're going to be talking

1:07

about. And to do that, I think we need

1:09

to do a bit of Bible trivia. Rob,

1:11

are you ready to go to Sunday School. Let's

1:17

do it. Let's go to Sunday School. Okay, And if

1:19

you I'm gonna ask you a few questions about

1:21

the Bible. If you get one wrong, you are going to get a

1:23

paddling. Whoa, what the

1:26

domination? Is this one of the ones that means

1:28

business? All right? Okay,

1:30

so how about let's see it

1:33

so, um, in in the garden

1:35

of Eden, what type of animal is it

1:37

that tempts Eve to eat from the tree? Oh,

1:40

that's a snake, that's

1:42

right, the serpent it is? Uh. Okay.

1:45

When after God created the world,

1:47

on which day of the week did he rest? Oh?

1:50

That was the seventh day. You got that

1:52

one, right, Okay, next one, How

1:54

many animals of each kind did Moses

1:56

take on the arc? Oh? Too, of course. And

2:00

there you go. That is the prime

2:02

example. Now, Rob, I know you were playing along because

2:05

you already know the trick in here what I actually

2:07

said when I asked that question. Hopefully you were playing

2:09

along at home as as you're listening, or maybe

2:12

you're not at home, wherever the heck you are. Um,

2:14

you may have thought the same thing, right,

2:16

Moses took two of each animal on the

2:18

arc. But in fact, in the Bible story

2:20

which maybe not everybody knows, but maybe

2:23

you do know this story of of

2:25

the arc and in the Book of Genesis, and

2:27

you do in fact know that it was not Moses

2:30

who did that. It was Noah in the story

2:32

who took animals on the arc. And yet

2:34

you thought after I said the question that the

2:36

answer is too and didn't even register

2:39

the fact that the name was wrong. Yeah,

2:42

It's it's an interesting uh

2:45

phenomenon to uh, to encounter,

2:48

you know, others, but also in yourself because

2:50

you m there's

2:52

several different ways to look at and we'll get into a number of these

2:55

here. But like even just now when you ask me those questions

2:57

like the serpent one, I'm totally firm

2:59

on that, like I of the I know that aspect of the

3:01

story inside and out. Uh. And of course

3:03

I know it's the seventh day that he rested on the

3:05

God rested on he she yet ohever

3:07

you want to look at it. Uh, But

3:10

there was still like this moment of hesitation because

3:12

I was like, it's seven, right, it is seven. I don't

3:14

want to come come off with the wrong

3:16

answer on the podcast. Um.

3:18

But then when one encount and granted already knew

3:20

the answer to the third one, but there is

3:22

this temptation though too, like when you when you

3:24

know why, when you know the

3:27

answer to something like you just you can just

3:29

jump in without hesitation, Like there's a certainty

3:31

that just propels you. Um,

3:33

you're excited to get your answer in

3:36

and then you know, get the acclaim and

3:38

the praise for getting it right. Yeah, there's

3:40

a certain kind of way in which a question, especially

3:43

a question posed in quiz format,

3:45

where you feel you are under performance

3:47

pressure and you're being evaluated

3:49

for whether or not you're going to get the right answer. It

3:52

sort of takes away some amount

3:54

of critical thinking that would normally go into

3:56

reading a sentence and causes you to

3:58

focus more exclusive lee on like just

4:01

can I get the right answer? And

4:03

so it's not hard to see how now, and this of

4:05

course might not be the only explanation for why this

4:07

is happening, but it's not hard to see

4:09

why you could pretty easily miss a

4:11

major error in a question that

4:14

is not you know, that is not necessarily

4:16

something that you're fuzzy on to begin

4:18

with, Like you could know perfectly well

4:20

that it's Noah in the story, and yet

4:22

it just goes completely over your head. Yeah,

4:25

and you know, we've we've been doing this podcast quite a

4:27

while at this point, and occasionally this this comes

4:29

up in our data, not so much in things

4:31

that we've researched for the podcast, because

4:33

I feel like if we've been crunching

4:36

the facts or the numbers, you know, or the you

4:38

know, we're we're more likely to be putting a lot

4:41

of thought into the situation and

4:43

we're maybe just a you know, a little hesitant

4:45

anyway. But the times where I've personally

4:48

like said something that was absolutely incorrect,

4:50

it would be something that I felt so sure about

4:52

that I just belt it out without fact checking

4:54

it at all. You know, Uh, something you generally

4:56

it's something not directly related to the episode,

4:58

but something that just kind of comes up in organic

5:00

conversation. That's exactly right. Yeah,

5:03

it's when you feel so confident that you're

5:05

not even being careful, you know that

5:07

that you can really make some big blunders.

5:10

Uh. There were some other questions I was reading about

5:12

in one of the I think the earliest

5:15

study on this phenomenon we're talking about today.

5:17

Some of the other questions were in the biblical

5:19

story, what is Joshua swallowed by?

5:21

Of course, that's Jonah that is swallowed

5:24

by the whale or the great fish, the sea monster.

5:27

Joshua of course is the the conquering

5:29

leader of the Israelites as they go about Kanaan.

5:31

Another one I really liked was in the novel

5:33

Moby Dick. What color was the whale

5:35

that Captain Nemo was after? I

5:38

think I think I might have fallen for that one. Yeah,

5:40

I mean, I wonder how much of the ego is

5:42

involved here, because it's like, you're kind of like, all

5:45

right, let's get to the part of this where I get to talk

5:47

and get to be the one is correct, like

5:49

like fast followers through all this other stuff. I don't care.

5:52

I have an answer and it is the correct one.

5:55

Yeah. Um, that's that's

5:57

quite perceptive, and I think that's right. Um.

6:00

But anyway, so this question that we're

6:02

looking at today, that this effect of not

6:04

noticing that the question says Moses

6:07

and just barreling right on through to the answer,

6:10

even if you know that it's actually Noah

6:12

in the story and not Moses. This

6:14

effect has a name, and it's known as the

6:16

Moses illusion. It's a particular

6:18

type of semantic illusion that

6:21

occurs when we are trying to process

6:23

certain kinds of sentences,

6:25

and this was first explored in a classic study

6:27

in psychology. It was a study called from

6:30

Words to Meaning a Semantic Illusion,

6:32

published in the Journal of Verbal

6:35

Learning and Verbal Behavior in nineteen eighty one

6:37

by Thomas D. Ericsson and Mark E. Mattson.

6:40

And I think it's interesting that this original observation

6:43

about this, this question about Moses, it

6:45

comes out of a mysterious question

6:48

about how we process the

6:50

meaning of sentences. Uh. The

6:52

authors of this study ask, quote, how

6:54

are the meanings of individual words

6:57

combined to form a more global

7:00

description of meaning? And

7:02

if you start to think hard about this

7:04

question about the human capacity for language,

7:07

I would argue it is absolutely astonishing.

7:10

It's almost baffling the way

7:12

that we're not only able to associate

7:15

symbolic meaning with certain sounds

7:17

coming out of our mouths or glyphs on a page,

7:20

but you're able to combine those

7:22

things endlessly to form and comprehend

7:25

infinite variations of combinations

7:28

of those sounds, to create sentences

7:30

that actually means something and other people can understand

7:33

what you mean when you say them. Like,

7:35

I think this type of capacity for language

7:38

is one of the features of the natural world

7:40

that to me seems closest to magic.

7:43

Yeah. Absolutely, And I feel like that the mostest illusion

7:45

is one of those things that that reveals the magic

7:48

that makes you more aware of

7:50

the magic trick that is

7:53

inherent to your just everyday perception

7:55

of reality and how you engage with facts

7:57

and information and the fact that you're just

7:59

like it. It's crazy that we're

8:01

just constantly throwing together sentences,

8:04

almost effortlessly, that are combining

8:07

all these words together. Each word has

8:09

a huge range of of possible

8:11

meanings and associations, and and

8:13

that we are able to do this with such fluency,

8:16

I mean, sometimes with more fluency than

8:18

other times. But uh, but yeah, it is

8:21

truly astounding to me. And so

8:23

the authors here are sort of talking about this

8:25

process and some of the question marks

8:28

that existed at the time in science about

8:31

how we form sentences and how we comprehend

8:33

sentences. So they start in their introduction

8:35

by talking about how quote a central process

8:37

in language comprehension is the construction

8:40

of a global description of the sentence

8:43

meaning from the meanings of individual

8:45

words which make up the sentence. Right,

8:47

So you know what individual words mean,

8:49

but somehow, like we're just talking about, you can combine

8:52

them into these overall gist

8:54

forms of what somebody is getting

8:56

at. You know, like, like, what kind

8:58

of answer is being quested by a question

9:01

that might be made up of ten different words

9:03

that are all you know, throwing your brain in ten different

9:05

directions. Yet you can get the gist

9:08

of the question and figure out what is getting

9:10

at pretty quickly actually, And they

9:12

talk about how there's been a lot of work on how

9:14

language processing works in the realm of

9:16

artificial intelligence, but at the time of this paper,

9:19

there was still a lot that we didn't know about

9:21

the global meaning of

9:23

of a sentence and and how that's constructed

9:26

in the brain. And so they summarize

9:28

the way they're starting this paper by saying, uh,

9:31

it has become widely assumed that sentences

9:33

are subject to exhaustive analysis

9:35

and consistency checks during

9:37

processing, but this is not

9:39

the case. People do not always

9:41

understand what is said to them. Sometimes

9:43

they fail to understand, sometimes they

9:46

misunderstand. And while these failures

9:48

of comprehension are sometimes due to lack

9:50

of appropriate knowledge or error on the part

9:52

of the speaker, there are other cases

9:54

in which such failures occur when the

9:57

understander possesses all the knowledge

9:59

necessary for correct understanding.

10:02

This paper explores such a phenomenon

10:05

and then they give the example of the Moses

10:07

illusion that we already talked about. The question

10:09

that they pose is how many animals

10:11

of each kind did Moses take on the

10:14

arc? And so what the authors here found in their

10:16

original study and eighty one was that the

10:18

majority of people failed to notice

10:20

a problem with the question and simply

10:23

answer to despite later

10:25

displaying knowledge that it was in fact

10:28

Noah and the story and not Moses.

10:31

And so that it's not that they just

10:33

don't know that much about the Bible, like they can

10:35

answer the question correctly when it's posed,

10:37

like, hey, was it Noah or Moses

10:39

who took animals onto the ark? They can

10:42

answer that correctly and yet still fail

10:44

to notice a problem in the question. And

10:46

studies find that people do this even when they're

10:48

not rushed. They still make the mistake

10:50

when they are given unlimited time to think

10:53

about it. Another interesting thing here

10:55

they found was that the the effect is

10:57

not caused by people misreading

10:59

or miss hearing the question, because people

11:02

still make the Moses illusion mistake

11:04

even if they themselves read the

11:06

question out loud, including the name

11:08

Moses, so they are saying Moses out

11:10

of their own lips, and they still might not

11:12

notice it now. In this first study,

11:14

the authors conclude that what's

11:17

very important, because they're getting at things about

11:19

the semantics of words and a sentence

11:21

and how the meanings of sentences are formed. They

11:23

conclude that shared semantic features

11:26

of the mix up are probably significantly

11:29

contributing to the effect. In other words,

11:31

this effect would probably not be nearly

11:33

as pronounced, maybe not even maybe totally

11:36

non existent if the items were not

11:38

in some way closely related

11:40

in the way that's a two Bible characters

11:43

are. If you ask, you know, how

11:45

many of each kind did Captain Hook take

11:47

into the arc, the effect probably vanishes.

11:50

Another study, I was looking at side at an example

11:52

I found really funny, which was how

11:54

many animals of each kind did Nixon

11:57

take on the arc? And yeah,

11:59

and and I like that because they were saying,

12:01

Okay, well, what if it's just like phonological

12:04

similarities, like Nixon and Noah have

12:06

some similarities. They start with the same sound,

12:08

they've got the same number of syllables. But

12:10

clearly when you put Nixon in the sentence,

12:12

people notice. And so

12:14

the Moses illusion is just one

12:17

persistent example from a class

12:19

of mental phenomena that could be called

12:21

knowledge neglect. This is a term

12:23

used by a couple of authors that will cite

12:25

later in the episode. But knowledge

12:27

neglect and simplified terms, is when

12:30

you behave as if you don't

12:32

know something even though you definitely

12:34

do know it. And the Moses

12:36

illusion is of course an example of knowledge

12:38

neglect, because the problem isn't that people

12:41

think Moses was the biblical character

12:43

who built the arc. You can know that it was Noah,

12:45

not Moses. If you're asked directly,

12:47

you'll get the answer right, but you don't

12:50

notice the problem when it's phrased in

12:52

a question like this. And of course

12:54

it's not just Moses and Noah. There are plenty of other

12:56

sentences in studies that have shown the same

12:58

thing. Though it is interesting that

13:01

Moses and Noah are like sort of the perfect

13:04

example of it. I think there might be particular

13:06

characteristics of these two names and characters

13:09

that make it like that make people especially

13:12

prone to the mix up in this case, though it is

13:14

true for lots of other types of you know,

13:16

words and objects. Well, speaking

13:18

of that, let's do a quick breakdown and just

13:20

especially for folks who are not that up on Moses

13:23

and Noah. Uh, just to give

13:26

a little you know, basic information about each of

13:28

them, and give me, give me the magic the

13:30

gathering card on each one. Okay, well let's

13:32

start. Let's start with with Noah. Okay,

13:35

certainly the the older of the two, the first

13:37

that in the chronological order. So

13:39

Noah was h is

13:41

written as as a was an antediluvian

13:44

patriarch in Jewish, Christian, and

13:46

Islamic traditions. The basic story,

13:48

God grows sick of humanity, so he tells Noah to round

13:51

up his family and two of every animal and get them

13:53

on a big old boat the arc, uh,

13:56

the first of two arcs we're going to discuss here, so

13:58

they alone can survive the noble flood

14:00

that's about to happen. Yeah. Now,

14:02

one interesting variation. I think most people

14:05

probably wouldn't even their brains wouldn't go this

14:07

far into the question. Uh.

14:09

It is actually more complicated than two of

14:11

every kind, because it also says in the

14:13

Noah's ark story that I think

14:15

they're supposed to bring more of every

14:18

kind of like certain types of animals, like

14:20

certain clean animals and just

14:22

two of the unclean animals or something. But

14:25

but yeah, when you get it gets

14:27

a little more complicated right, I mean it's all kinds

14:29

of animal management. Yeah, which

14:31

I would love to see somebody fail the

14:34

test of of the the Noah illusion

14:38

the Moses illusion here by by

14:40

going into a lot of detail about the you

14:42

know, the the actual biblical text while still

14:45

failing. I think that right. Well, it was fourteen

14:47

of every kind of clean animal alright.

14:51

Well, anyway, Noah strengths megaproject

14:54

management and animal handling. Obviously

14:56

weakness, alcoholism, that's

14:59

a major part of the store, right. Um. Actors

15:01

of note who have betrayed him. Uh,

15:03

this is not a complete list, but

15:05

these are the main main ones. John Houston, Russell

15:08

Crowe, David Thrillful.

15:11

This is a guy on Shameless. He also played Dr

15:13

John d and Elizabeth the Golden Age. John

15:16

Voight, David Rentall. Uh,

15:18

David Rentall is the guy who played Aries

15:20

Targarian on the Game of Thrones

15:23

show. Oh interesting, wait a Aries

15:25

to the Mad King. I believe.

15:28

So that's the main areas, right, Okay,

15:30

yeah, well maybe I guess for

15:32

some reason I thought there was another one. I am wrong.

15:34

Um, okay, So the I've got a

15:36

really funny story about John Voight playing

15:38

Noah. I remember seeing this one, oh,

15:41

I have. It was made for TV. I think

15:44

came out when I was in like middle school, and

15:46

it is not at all faithful

15:48

to the Bible, and to say, very

15:51

hollywooded up version of

15:53

the Noah's Arks story. John Voight

15:55

does play Noah and the arc is attacked by pirates.

15:58

What. Yeah, it's attacked

16:00

by like water World pirates. I mean, it

16:02

might as well be Dennis Hopper and the Smokers,

16:04

but it's actually I think they get attacked

16:06

by pirates led by the biblical character

16:09

Lot. Okay, alright,

16:11

well, if that is in the Bible, at least

16:13

they're they're playing around with it. Was this

16:15

brought up at all when um Uh,

16:19

when Darren Aronofsky was being

16:21

criticized for the plot of

16:23

his Noah movie, which has like um

16:25

Uh giants and Nephelim

16:27

in it. Oh, I kind of liked his Noah

16:30

movie. It was way more more

16:32

faithful to I think it included

16:35

stuff from non canonical ancient texts,

16:37

but was actually inspired by ancient texts.

16:40

Okay, alright, I still haven't seen it. It's it's

16:42

been on the list for a while. All Right,

16:44

let's talk about Moses real quick. Okay, So Moses

16:46

comes later. He's an Old Testament prophet

16:49

um central figure in the narrative

16:51

of the Exodus. In the account,

16:53

he helps the Jewish people in their liberation from

16:55

Egypt Egyptian captivity and following

16:58

Tim the tin Plegs of Egypt. He assists him

17:00

in the Exodus, and he also is involved

17:02

with an arc. But it's the Ark of the Covenant,

17:05

which we've discussed on the show before. Not a

17:07

boat, but a golden vessel

17:10

that contains sacred items. Yeah.

17:12

I would assume that the words are related

17:14

because they're both like a container of kinds,

17:16

like a big box. Okay,

17:19

So Moses his strength community

17:21

organizing of course, and sorcery his

17:23

weaknesses. This is this

17:25

is kind of interesting, I guess, because it's either not obeying

17:28

God and everything or obeying God and

17:30

everything, depending on who you ask, right, uh,

17:33

I mean, if you ask God, he would say, well, he

17:35

didn't obey me and everything. That's why I didn't get to

17:37

go into the Promised Land. But especially

17:40

modern critics are like, it seems like he he may

17:42

be followed the letter of the law a little

17:44

bit too. Um uh too.

17:46

Seriously, I seem

17:48

to recall at one point him commanding the

17:50

death penalty for a dude who was working on the

17:52

Sabbath. That seems a little harsh. Yeah, it seems

17:54

seems a little harsh. Um.

17:56

Okay, So actors of note who have betrayed Moses,

17:59

well, Charlton has obviously, Burt

18:01

Lancaster, mel Brooks, Ben

18:03

Kingsley, Val Kilmer, though

18:05

that that may have just been a voice role.

18:08

And Christian Bale. Now, the

18:10

last one is interesting because as I

18:12

was looking at these actors was one of the interesting things

18:14

is even though they're basically interchangeable, like

18:16

the same, Um, you

18:18

know, in most of these cases, you're dealing with

18:20

the same white dude that could play either of these

18:23

characters in a big Hollywood production. Um.

18:25

But it's interesting that I

18:27

don't think anyone has actually played

18:30

both Moses and Noah, though

18:32

Christian Bale reportedly

18:34

came very close because Darren Aronofsky originally

18:37

wanted Christian Bale to play the title

18:39

role in his Noah film, but scheduling

18:42

conflicts prohibited that from happening. Oh

18:44

he couldn't because he was filming like terminator

18:47

Mick g or whatever. Yeah. I

18:49

don't know, but um, but imagine

18:51

if if Bale had played both Noah

18:54

and Moses. What would that have meant for

18:56

the Moses illusion? Would it have made

18:58

the would would it just destroy are semantic

19:00

understanding of reality? Maybe

19:03

there's a secret counsel. There's like no Hollywood,

19:05

no actor can play both of these roles

19:08

because it will totally tear our understanding

19:10

of of of facts and fiction apart.

19:13

I could see that. I mean, so, I

19:15

think what some of the authors here are proposing is

19:17

that the the fact that it's

19:19

not just that Moses and Noah are words

19:22

that kind of sound similar. They've got some similar

19:24

consonants and uh in the same number

19:26

of syllables, similar vowel sounds.

19:28

That's all true, and that does seem to matter, But

19:31

it's also very important that they are semantically

19:34

related, that they are both characters from

19:36

the Torah, from the Old Testament, and

19:39

that sort of links them together. And I think the more

19:41

you could do to link them even further together

19:43

and associate them in in our minds, like

19:46

yes, having one actor play both, I think that

19:48

would actually probably make people even more

19:50

susceptible. Yeah, um,

19:52

I was thinking about this too, Like obviously we've

19:54

already touched on a few extra examples of

19:56

this. But I was trying to come up with with other

19:59

examples that would play on the same energy here,

20:01

and one that came to mind would be, uh,

20:03

if we were to look to Chinese mythology, if we

20:05

were to say, hey, how did the Yellow Emperor

20:08

decide how to order the animals of the zodiac?

20:10

And you might respond with, oh, well, there's this cool

20:12

little story about a race for the animals,

20:15

etcetera. Um, but it wasn't the Yellow

20:17

Emperor. It was the Jade Emperor, who's an even more

20:19

primordial god ruler than

20:21

the Yellow Emperor. Um. So I don't

20:24

know that seems like it could be could play in

20:26

the similar could

20:28

work in a similar way to the Moses and

20:31

Noah illusion. Or how

20:33

about this in Return of the Jedi, what was Django

20:35

fet swallowed by? Oh? I just see.

20:37

For some reason, I feel like that one doesn't work because

20:40

then learn as you as soon as you say the word

20:42

django, like people's alarms

20:44

go off and like, wait a minute, what are we talking about?

20:46

Yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, Well I don't maybe

20:48

it would. Okay, here's one for Avatar,

20:51

the last Airbender fans out there, Um, we're

20:53

hearing from several of them. Which

20:55

nation was the avatar Apa

20:57

born into. I don't know if that one or

21:00

not. But of course ang is the last and

21:02

not the last avatar, and is the avatar uh

21:05

Appa is the sky by sin that he rides

21:07

on. Ah. I see, so I

21:09

don't know ang Appa. Maybe that works not

21:11

sure? Well that went over my head anyway.

21:20

So you might think, well, now that we have told

21:22

you there is such a thing as the Moses illusion,

21:24

uh you know you would never fall for it, right

21:27

because you know you will

21:29

now always having this knowledge

21:31

in your mind. Notice when there will be substitutions

21:34

of this kind in a question or a sentence.

21:36

But it turns out that's not necessarily true. Uh

21:39

So there was this original research from nineteen

21:41

eighty one, but there have been a bunch of studies

21:44

in the decades since then replicating

21:46

the original finding and further

21:48

probing the effect to figure out what's

21:50

going on in our brains. Uh So,

21:52

I wanted to talk about some typical findings.

21:55

First of all, some things that were summarized

21:57

in h in a few literature reviews I was

22:00

looking at. One was in a book chapter

22:02

by Elizabeth J. Marsh and SHARDA

22:04

Umanath. It was a

22:06

book called Processing Inaccurate

22:08

Information published by M. I. T. Press.

22:11

In that book sounds like a scream,

22:14

but their chapter is called knowledge neglect

22:16

failures to notice contradictions with stored

22:18

knowledge and will revisit this chapter a few

22:20

times later in the episode. But they

22:23

summarize some things about the

22:25

Moses illusion. Uh so they

22:27

say that most of the time people will fall

22:29

for the Moses solution even though they actually

22:32

know the difference between Moses and Noah, as

22:34

demonstrated with later interrogation. So

22:36

you can ask people questions like who

22:38

built the arc or who took the animals into

22:41

the arc, and they'll get the answer right, but they

22:43

still fail to notice that it's Moses in the question.

22:46

And this can be accomplished with other similar

22:48

Switcherus actually included rob

22:50

a list for you to look at of questions

22:53

like this one. I like, is um, what

22:55

did Goldilocks eat at the Three Little

22:57

Pigs house? And a lot of people

22:59

will is to answer porridge, even though you

23:01

can later ask them like, hey, whose

23:04

house did Goldilocks go into? The three

23:06

bears or the three Little pigs? And they of course

23:08

know that it was the bears. Now that One's interesting

23:10

because for me anyway, there's

23:13

a there's an associated mental image of

23:15

the bears or the pigs. Uh.

23:18

They they look rather different, uh, and

23:20

and ultimately they have different functions in the stories,

23:23

whereas Moses and Noah are more interchangeable.

23:26

And it is the same sort of character and

23:28

there of course the same species, because the

23:30

pigs are there to be the victims of the big

23:32

bad wolf story and to get beaten, and

23:34

the bears are there too. I don't

23:37

know what, just hang out in their house, I guess right,

23:39

But I can still imagine someone uh falling

23:42

for this or or you know, having airing

23:44

in answering this question, because in a way,

23:47

again, you're you're racing into the finish line. You're picking up

23:49

on the you know, the basics

23:51

of the question, even though you're you're you're

23:53

skipping over this. This this this

23:56

misinformation that's embedded in the middle

23:58

of it. Right. Though, it's interesting

24:00

that you mentioned racing to get to the answer. I do

24:03

think you're basically right about that, except

24:05

it doesn't really seem that time is

24:07

a factor here, because giving people

24:09

extra or even unlimited time

24:11

to think about the question does not eliminate

24:14

the effect does it, So it doesn't

24:16

seem to result from people being in a hurry

24:18

in terms of time, though I think you could

24:20

still think about it as people being in

24:22

a hurry in terms of just like wanting

24:25

to get to the part where they answer the question.

24:27

I don't know, maybe that could be like self imposed

24:29

time limits, even if they're not imposed

24:32

by somebody externally trying to rush

24:34

you through. Now, Also, in a typical

24:36

setup for these Moses illusion experiments,

24:38

readers will be warned that some questions

24:41

will contain incorrect presuppositions,

24:43

so it's not just like a trick question where they don't

24:45

know this is coming. They'll be told, Okay,

24:48

some of these questions will be valid questions,

24:51

in which case you should just answer them,

24:53

but other questions will have incorrect

24:55

presuppositions, and when you come across

24:58

one of those, you should note that the question

25:00

is not valid. Now, the interesting

25:02

thing is, I would think something like that would almost

25:05

completely erase the effect, because you're putting

25:07

people on guard to be like interrogating

25:09

the questions. But it doesn't. You can

25:11

put people on guard like that and they still

25:13

fall for the Moses solution. In

25:16

these experiments, it does seem to be a very

25:18

robust effect, like a substantial number

25:20

of people will fail to detect errors

25:22

in questions, even though they later showed

25:24

that they possessed the knowledge to answer

25:26

them correctly. Uh. The exact

25:29

percentages of the effect, though very

25:31

a good bit UH from that chapter by

25:33

Martian Umanov the

25:35

they right quote. Overall, the Moses solution is

25:38

robust, with readers answering from fourteen

25:40

percent to forty percent to

25:42

fifty two percent to seventy seven

25:45

percent of distorted questions depending

25:47

on the particular experiments. So they're citing a number

25:49

of different results there. The fourteen percent was

25:52

by van Jarsveld

25:54

Dikstra, and Herman's was

25:58

Hannon and Donovan and too, and

26:00

one was Ericson

26:02

and Mattson in one

26:04

and was Barton and Sandford

26:06

inte. And I would imagine

26:08

these differences have a lot to do with like what

26:11

what exactly types of warnings

26:13

you're giving people ahead of time, what exactly what

26:16

exact examples are used? As

26:18

we've said, you know, it's it's clear that different

26:20

questions are more prone than others.

26:23

Like I think more people would probably fall for

26:25

the Moses Noah confusion than

26:27

for the Three Little Pigs, Three bears confusion.

26:30

Yeah, I have to say some of the the examples

26:32

that you included on a list here, it's it's interesting

26:35

to run through this because even though I'm not

26:37

encountering them as actual questions

26:40

like one and someone in one of these studies would be I

26:42

can certainly pick up on the ones that I feel

26:45

like would have been more likely

26:47

to fool me, like, for instance, what

26:49

kind of treated Lincoln chop down? What kind

26:52

of treated Washington chop down? Um?

26:54

Like I can imagine myself sort

26:57

of this being a story I'm not tremendously

26:59

in the it in, but have a version off

27:01

stored away I can instantly

27:04

skip, or even not instantly, but even

27:06

with some thought would be like I think, yeah, cherry Tree,

27:08

cherry Tree, that's the one, you know, even if

27:11

said Lincoln. Yeah, even if it's said Lincoln,

27:13

because also I don't know Lincoln. Something

27:16

about like their stories about him, you know,

27:18

we also have sort of tall tales about him and his exploits,

27:21

and um, the one about him,

27:23

Uh, there's one about him answering

27:26

a duel. Somebody challenged him to a duel and he

27:28

says, well, I get to choose the place

27:31

and the weapon. So I choose, uh,

27:33

what's sledgehammers and five feet of

27:35

water or something? DAVI that he's tall

27:37

and the other person was short, something like that. I have no

27:39

idea if that's an even a legitimate

27:41

story, but I have it in my head. So I have

27:43

an image of Lincoln holding some sort

27:46

of long handled tool, so it

27:48

fits in nicely into the story,

27:50

like I can easily overlay one over

27:53

the other. Yeah. One of the examples

27:56

that I feel extremely confident

27:58

that I would not fall for is the

28:00

one of what is the name of the Mexican

28:02

dip made with mashed artichokes? I

28:06

definitely, I mean, I just know artichokes.

28:08

No, that is not what it is. You don't

28:10

mash artichokes, do you. I mean, I haven't

28:13

seen it. Could could make

28:15

an artichoke paste, but

28:18

artichoke guacamole. That sounds gross, I

28:20

mean, but yet artichoke depp is

28:22

amazing, but artichoke guacamole

28:25

just says it sound right? But

28:28

anyway, So Marcia and Umanov also

28:31

note that um that that error detection

28:33

is lower when items uh that

28:36

items are swapped are similar in a couple of

28:38

ways. We've already mentioned these, but they

28:40

reiterate that it helps when there's

28:42

phonological similarity. So do the words

28:44

sound close to each other? I feel like, uh,

28:47

Avocados and artichokes, like they have

28:49

some similar vowel sounds, and they start with the same

28:52

letter, but they sound different enough to me

28:54

that I'm immediately strong. I think somehow

28:56

like the hard k sound coming towards

28:58

the end of the word art a choke, but coming

29:01

towards the beginning of or,

29:03

I guess in the middle of avocado. Somehow, that makes a

29:05

big difference in my brain. And

29:08

then, of course, as we've been saying, semantic

29:10

similarity, are the concept somehow similar

29:13

or related? Would we put them in a kind

29:15

of meaning next us together in the brain?

29:18

Uh? And and of course it's notable that the

29:20

Moses versus Noah one meets both of

29:22

the criteria. They sound similar and they're

29:24

related. So anyway, it's just this

29:26

interesting fact about our brains that something

29:29

about being asked a question

29:31

like this. So trying to process a sentence

29:34

like the questions in these studies causes

29:36

us to ignore the fact that the contents

29:38

of the sentence conflict with things that we

29:41

know to be true, and I wanted to mention

29:43

one other study I was looking at that. This one

29:46

is by Hadency Bottoms,

29:48

Andrea and Slick and Elizabeth J. Marsh

29:51

from published in the journal Memory

29:54

called Memory and the Moses illusion failures

29:56

to detect contradictions with stored knowledge

29:59

yield negative memorial consequences.

30:01

Now we can revisit some of the things in this

30:04

more as we go on, but I just wanted to note

30:06

a few things that they bring up. Uh

30:08

So, first of all, they note some other previous

30:11

findings in their introduction. One

30:13

is that um error detection

30:16

improves, so people are less likely

30:18

to fall for the Moses illusion when

30:20

the error appears in what they call the cleft

30:23

phrase or the main focus of

30:25

the sentence. So there are ways that you

30:27

can basically ask the same question but just

30:30

sort of rearrange the words to make

30:32

people more likely to notice the

30:34

problem. So, if you take the sentence

30:36

how many animals of each kind did

30:38

Moses take on the arc? The word Moses

30:41

is kind of syntactically de

30:43

emphasized in that sentence, you know, it's

30:45

not like the main focus of the way the sentence

30:48

is phrased. You can re orient

30:50

the words to make moses more prominent

30:52

in the sentence, in which case people are more likely

30:54

to catch the problem. Yeah, Like

30:56

I also feel like having the word show up

30:58

so late in the sentence. I'm I'm,

31:01

I'm. Like, you're always predicting where sentences

31:03

are going, you know, yes, so you've kind of already

31:06

filled it in to a certain extent, like you know, you

31:08

know who we're talking about. Uh, even

31:10

if you end up using the wrong name. Um,

31:13

yeah, I think you're exactly right about that. Like

31:15

that, once you've heard I don't know, you get

31:17

like four or five words into the sentence, you you sort

31:20

of are like you already know what it's going to

31:22

be, and you're just sort of like okay, you like

31:24

mostly ignoring the words that come after that.

31:27

Another thing that they point out that's interesting

31:29

is that error detection improves

31:31

when questions appear in a difficult

31:34

to read font And they

31:36

say this is because it reduces

31:38

processing fluency, which in turn makes

31:40

material seem less familiar

31:43

and less true. And this was found

31:45

by Song and Schwartz in two thousand and eight.

31:48

And this, of course, this comes back to our old friend.

31:50

Processing fluency, a cognitive

31:52

factor that I believe is one of the most underappreciated

31:56

influences on our thoughts and beliefs

31:58

and behavior. We talked about

32:00

it in our episode on the illusory truth

32:02

effect. Basically, processing fluency means

32:04

how easy is it for this

32:07

stimulus to be processed by the brain and

32:10

uh, and it came up in the illusory truth Effect episode

32:13

because I remember. The illusory truth effect

32:15

is where statements you've encountered

32:17

before seem more true than

32:19

statements that are new to you. And

32:22

one possible explanation for this is that

32:24

familiar statements are easier for the

32:26

brain to process than unfamiliar ones

32:29

are, and at some level, the brain

32:31

makes an equivalence between that processing

32:34

fluency, how easy it is to process

32:37

this incoming sentence because it's familiar and

32:40

factual trustworthiness. They actually

32:42

have nothing to do with one another, but the brain maybe

32:44

uses a little bit of shortcut there. So

32:48

are you saying that in the future for our

32:50

our shared notes, Joe, we should use chill

32:52

or font instead of whatever

32:54

we're using now. Yeah, that would

32:57

that make it less like I mean, I think that would

32:59

generally slow us down and make it

33:01

harder to do the podcast, But it also might

33:03

make it less likely that we would

33:05

just like flub words here and there, because

33:09

it would be a like really effortful, laborious

33:12

process to get through every single thought,

33:15

which you know sometimes it is anyway, but that

33:17

that's on us, um. But anyway,

33:19

So Song and Shorts here in two thousand and eight found

33:22

that simply by making statements harder

33:24

to read so you put them in, you said, Chiller,

33:26

I was thinking, Papyrus. I don't know what what

33:29

actual thought they used, but it would just

33:31

make people more likely to spot errors

33:33

in the questions instead of just rolling

33:35

right over them without noticing. And you

33:37

know that makes sense to me, Yeah, yeah, it

33:41

does. It is interesting that that's how our brains

33:43

work, though, Yeah, it is sort

33:45

of counterintuitive at the same time, like you might

33:47

just assume that if something's harder to read,

33:50

you would be less likely to catch errors

33:52

in it. But yeah, I think there's some kind of process

33:54

where it's like slowing you down. It's not allowing

33:57

you to just like skip over the parts

33:59

that it seemed like yeah, yeah, okay, Moses

34:01

whatever. It's like like a bit of food that's

34:04

extra chewy, so you're going to really taste

34:06

this, You're really going to get a feel for the texture. There's

34:08

no just wolf in this down. Yeah. Now,

34:10

In the study by Bottoms at All, they were

34:13

looking at the question of whether participants

34:15

can detect errors in questions better

34:18

if there are just more errors

34:20

overall in the sample of questions. So, if

34:22

I give you a bunch of questions and like, I

34:24

don't know, seventy of them contain

34:27

errors of this kind in them, are people

34:29

more likely to catch them? And it looks

34:31

like the answer is yes. Like, if you if you've got

34:33

people on guard because they were just constantly

34:36

problems with these questions, their guard

34:38

goes up and they do seem to make the Moses

34:40

illusion mistake less often. And

34:43

it strikes me that that could be possibly,

34:45

or at least partially because once

34:48

you start, you know, showing people questions

34:50

where most of them contain a problem,

34:53

or even just a large minority of them

34:55

contain a problem, people probably

34:57

start uh interacting with the

34:59

question less as questions

35:02

and becoming less focused on just getting

35:04

the answer and start looking at

35:06

them more like a puzzle where you're you're

35:08

trying to parse the sentence very clearly.

35:11

Yeah, Yeah, It's like, how is this trying to trick

35:13

me. Yeah, but

35:15

then there's one kind of scary implication

35:18

from this paper the author's right

35:20

quote. More generally, the failure to detect

35:22

errors had negative memorial consequences,

35:25

increasing the likelihood that errors were

35:27

used to answer later general

35:29

knowledge questions. Methodological

35:32

implications of this finding are discussed, as

35:34

it suggests that typical analyzes likely

35:37

underestimate the size of the Moses illusion.

35:40

Overall, answering distorted questions

35:42

can yield errors in the knowledge

35:44

base. More importantly, prior

35:46

knowledge does not protect against these negative

35:49

memorial consequences. And

35:51

Robert, I think you had a note about that. We can talk a little

35:53

bit more about that in a bit, but yeah, basically,

35:55

there there is some evidence that just

35:58

steamrolling over an incorrect

36:01

fact in a sentence, even when you know

36:03

otherwise, can can later damage

36:06

your ability to recall that fact correctly.

36:09

Yeah, yeah, so it Yeah,

36:11

as as well discuss here. It's it's

36:13

not just a situation where oh, well this is a quirk.

36:16

This is interesting. The brain does this. I mean, it

36:18

is that, but it it has it has

36:21

greater implications. Yeah. Now

36:23

I want to go back on the other side and say

36:25

that when we encounter

36:28

things like this, you know, illusions that humans

36:30

often fall for. When you read about a certain

36:32

type of I don't know, cognitive

36:35

bias or or something. I

36:37

think our tendency is often to at

36:39

first react like, wow, our dumb brains

36:41

were so stupid. But but I think

36:44

there's another way to think about it, and

36:46

that's this. How amazing

36:49

is it that we have such a powerful

36:51

command of language based reasoning that

36:54

we can answer questions

36:56

even though key elements of the sentence

36:59

do not match with our knowledge base.

37:01

I mean, think about the trouble that a

37:03

computer would run into trying

37:06

to do the same thing. Like, While

37:08

it's an interesting case of an illusion

37:10

failing to notice facts that conflict with

37:12

our existing knowledge, it's also

37:15

a demonstration of an absolutely

37:17

amazing capacity for language

37:19

comprehension, even when there are severe

37:22

errors in the questions or sentences that

37:24

we're trying to comprehend, Like somehow

37:26

our brains are so good at

37:29

getting what seems to be the gist the

37:31

intended global meaning of a sentence,

37:34

even when pivotal items in that sentence

37:36

are wrong and should be pointing you off

37:38

in the wrong direction and make you totally confused.

37:41

Yeah, yeah, um, you

37:44

know, I can't help but be reminded in all this about

37:46

the drawing of the bicycle that we've touched on

37:48

before about often, I mean it's different.

37:50

We're not dealing with language, We're dealing with a uh

37:53

like a mental image. Like we all

37:55

think we have the mental image of a bicycle pretty

37:57

firm in our heads, and yet when put to the

38:00

test, when acts asked to draw a bicycle,

38:02

um, we're often floored. You know.

38:05

Yeah, that was a different one of our cognitive Allusions

38:07

episodes. That was the the illusion of explanatory

38:10

depth. Yeah, the issue where people

38:13

they tend to think like that they understand

38:15

how something works until they're asked to

38:17

explain it. So somehow the brain has

38:20

a way of representing a sort of

38:22

pat Tempken comprehension, you

38:24

know that it puts up this facade of yeah, you

38:27

know how that works. I I I know how

38:29

I know the parts of a bicycle. I know all

38:31

the parts of a can opener. I could make one basically.

38:34

But then if you are asked to like explain the

38:36

steps of how it works or draw all the parts,

38:38

you're like, uh, yeah,

38:41

I thought about this a lot watching the Outlander

38:44

TV show about the time traveler goes back

38:46

in time and she's recreating various things

38:48

that she knows about from the future, and like,

38:50

God, like, how many of us, you know, we go if we

38:52

were to do that, if we were to go back in time, we

38:55

might tell somebody about all these marvelous things

38:57

like oh yeah, penicillin and uh

38:59

you know, by sickles and whatnot, and it'd be like, oh great,

39:01

how did it work? And it'd be like, uh yeah, no, no idea,

39:04

I have some some vague so I have some

39:06

of the facts that I had, but not near enough

39:08

to reproduce anything that I'm talking about.

39:14

Thank you, thank you, thank you. Coming

39:17

back to this thing about how the Moses

39:19

ilusion is, it is and could be looked

39:21

at as an example of how

39:24

amazingly adaptive at

39:26

comprehension our brains are. Actually

39:28

found a book chapter discussing this very

39:30

aspect of the effect. So the

39:33

authors here were hik Young Park

39:35

and Lynn M. Rader. Uh

39:37

And this was a chapter in a book,

39:40

and the chapter was called the Moses Illusion.

39:42

I think it was published in two thousand four. And

39:44

so they're talking about different potential explanations

39:47

for the Moses illusion. What's going on in the brain,

39:49

and they conclude that they, or at least they

39:52

argue that the most likely explanation for

39:54

what's going on when we fall for this is

39:57

something they call the partial match HYPOTHESI

40:00

us so I just want to read from their conclusion

40:02

that's along the lines of what we've just been talking

40:04

about. Quote. Research

40:06

on the Moses illusion demonstrates that people

40:08

have difficulty in detecting distortions

40:11

or inaccuracies when a distorted element

40:13

is semantically related to the theme of the sentence.

40:16

Why should our cognitive system be so

40:18

tolerant of distortions and find

40:21

it so difficult to do careful matches

40:23

to memory. It might seem that partial

40:25

matching is a less than ideal way to process

40:28

information. However, the partial

40:30

match process is not only common

40:32

and normal, but also a necessary

40:34

mechanism of our cognitive system. This

40:37

partial match process enables useful

40:39

communication and comprehension. Very

40:41

few things that we see or here will

40:44

perfectly match the representation

40:46

that we already have stored in memory.

40:49

In order to answer questions, we need to

40:51

be able to use an acceptable match.

40:53

In order to understand a new situation

40:56

and map it onto something we have already seen

40:58

or done, we must apt slight

41:00

variations every day. At many

41:03

levels, we accept slight distortions

41:05

without even noticing the process. Occasionally

41:08

we notice a distortion and choose to ignore

41:10

it, but more frequently we do

41:12

not even realize that distortions

41:15

have occurred. A rigid comprehension

41:18

system would have a difficult time. Indeed,

41:20

many of our cognitive operations are driven

41:23

by familiarity based heuristics

41:25

rather than careful matching operations.

41:27

The Moses illusion is an example of how

41:30

the adaptive human cognitive system

41:32

works. Everyday, cognitive processing

41:35

must be based on simple heuristics,

41:37

such as matching sets of features,

41:39

rather than exact matches, as very

41:41

few tasks require exact

41:43

matches. Sentences do not match

41:46

stored information. Faces change,

41:49

voices may change slightly, even

41:51

our pets and friends change over time.

41:53

Therefore, it makes sense that people do use

41:56

partial matches in the normal course

41:58

of matching to memory. Rcial

42:00

matching is immutable because it is

42:02

the most efficient way for memory to operate

42:05

given the nature of the environment in which we

42:07

live. And so, yeah, this really

42:09

makes me think along the lines of what

42:11

we were just saying a few minutes ago. Like the Moses

42:13

illusion is kind of funny when you notice

42:16

yourself doing it, but it's also it's

42:18

also kind of a superpower. Yeah,

42:21

Like, imagine if you went to a video

42:24

store, which we still have one in Atlanta.

42:26

Imagine you went there and you were to say, um,

42:29

yeah, I'm looking for a particular movie. Um it

42:31

started Anthony Hopkins and it had a puppet

42:34

in it. And instead of being able

42:36

to piece that together and tell

42:38

you which movie you're talking about, what if

42:40

they were to say, Okay, keep listening, I need you

42:42

to list the entire cast. I need all

42:44

of the details. We have to make acent

42:46

match here. Or Yeah, imagine somebody comes into

42:49

the video store and they say, I'm looking

42:51

for The Godfather too, and they

42:53

say, sorry, we don't have that. What

42:56

they actually have is The Godfather Cole and Part

42:58

two. Oh man, that that's

43:00

not completely unbelievable and not with

43:02

our video store, but just sort of like the cliche video

43:06

store. You mean the

43:08

god Father part to Philistine.

43:13

I mean, that's a kind of silly example, but I think the

43:15

authors of this chapter are exactly right that every

43:18

basically, every single moment of our lives,

43:21

we are testing reality against

43:23

our memories, and we have to do so in a fast

43:25

and loose way, and our ability

43:27

to do so in a fast and loose way without

43:30

relying on every detail to be an exact

43:32

correct match is is

43:34

what allows us to live adaptively,

43:37

to sort of like be thinking creatures

43:40

looking for exact matches between

43:43

the current case you're observing and what's

43:45

stored in your memory. Like I made

43:47

the comparison to a computer earlier.

43:49

Today, I guess we're more familiar with more adaptive

43:52

types of computer functions that are based on like

43:54

AI or like huge amounts of machine learning

43:57

or something like that. It makes me think about like the

43:59

early old days of dealing

44:02

with the you know, computer programming, where like

44:04

if you slightly misspelled, like you

44:06

know, um, you're you're playing Zork

44:09

or something and you type like wolke

44:11

north the w o l K, it's not is

44:13

to be like that is not a valid action. Like

44:16

yeah, it's amazing nowadays, how just like

44:18

how much thumb fumbling I can

44:21

put into typing something in search and

44:23

it still knows what I'm talking about. I still

44:25

um able to floor it every now and then because I'll get really

44:27

reckless and u and

44:30

it'll just have no clue. But but more often

44:32

than not, it'll it'll guess what I'm going

44:34

for. But that is amazing because that

44:37

is the the the input

44:39

receiver whatever, you know. This piece of technology

44:41

it's called AI because it's becoming

44:43

more like our brains. It's becoming usefully

44:46

sloppy and and loose in the way our

44:48

brains are. Now.

44:50

I guess we could talk about a couple of other possible

44:52

examples of knowledge neglect or implications

44:55

of knowledge neglect. One that I came

44:57

across that I thought was pretty funny is something

44:59

that seems fairly

45:01

narrow, but it's known as the yolk phenomenon.

45:04

Uh, So it goes like this apparently was originally

45:07

described in an article in the Psychological

45:09

Review by Gregory Kimball

45:12

and Lawrence Pearl Mutter. Uh

45:14

this was in the year ninety if I didn't already

45:16

say that, But it consists

45:18

of asking somebody a list of questions

45:21

and and it's designed to produce a

45:23

certain answer. So you say, what

45:25

do we call the tree that grows from acorns?

45:28

And you say an oak? And then you say,

45:30

what do you call a funny story joke?

45:32

What's the sound made by a frog croak?

45:35

What's another word? For a cape cloak.

45:38

What do we call the white of an egg? And

45:40

most people say yolk um,

45:43

which is obviously wrong, And people are not

45:45

confused about the white of an egg

45:47

being called the yolk. But it seems like instead

45:49

the implication is that there's a certain kind of

45:52

pattern seeking that overtakes

45:54

semantic processing here, like

45:56

the brain starts to conclude while you're answering

45:59

these questions because of the

46:01

established pattern that rhyming is more

46:03

important than the actual meaning

46:05

of the word that rhymes and you know it

46:08

rhymes march exactly. It's the rhymes

46:10

reason effects sort of. I mean, uh, which

46:13

I think we talked with that in our episode on anti

46:16

metaboli. But I was wondering,

46:18

I wonder how many items in a list

46:20

like this it takes before the majority

46:22

of respondents will give the

46:24

yolk type answer, will ignore the known meaning

46:27

of a word and just supply the nonsensical

46:29

rhyming match. I don't

46:31

know. I feel like I'm very susceptible to this

46:33

one, because I I recently was

46:35

trying to do a recipe and it got

46:37

kind of confusing, and I had a moment where

46:40

I had to ask myself, wait, which part

46:42

is the yolk and which is white.

46:45

Um, it was only a momentary lapse, but

46:47

there were a lot of things going on. There was a lot. I was like

46:49

having to take them apart, you know, as one of those

46:52

we have to have the egg white and one bowl and

46:54

the yolks and the other. And it was I was making

46:56

a su flight That's what it was, and

46:58

an applicated dish. Yeah, and I did

47:01

had I had not had coffee yet either, so I had

47:03

that going for me. Um. It was

47:05

successful. But yeah, there was that moment where I'm

47:07

like, okay, I have to have so many egg whites and

47:09

then a different number of yolks and

47:12

which ones which? Now? Uh so I

47:14

would totally fall for this. I mean, did

47:17

you succeed? Did it rise? Yeah? I had

47:19

Rise. It was good. Yeah. I don't think I want

47:21

to put it in regular weekly rotation, but it

47:23

was. It was good for a special treat. I feel

47:25

like the soufle a that is just one of the most notorious,

47:28

tricky, tricky dishes for people

47:30

who aren't I guess like working in you know, kitchens

47:33

or bakeries every day. Yeah, it was still it was

47:35

tricky. It was tricky for me even though I went

47:37

with a very what seemed like a very simple recipe

47:39

that that didn't steer me too wrong, but still

47:42

I got lost a little bit for a moment. Well,

47:45

I'm impressed, So I was. I was reading

47:47

through this book chapter as well, um on

47:50

Knowledge Neglect by marsh and Humana,

47:53

and uh, yeah, this was this was very interesting.

47:55

Um so yeah. They point to a couple of

47:57

other misconceptions. I don't think we've mentioned these

48:00

on on the episode thus far, but one

48:03

of them was Toronto is the capital

48:05

of Canada, and a

48:07

blow to the head cure's amnesia, which I guess

48:09

is like a TV you know, cartoon kind of a thing.

48:12

But these are all like examples of misconceptions

48:15

that you might have in your head that are are not true.

48:17

They point out that you know it tries, we might misconceptions

48:20

are impossible to ignore, and

48:23

uh, your best hope if you can't

48:25

avoid hearing misconceptions altogether,

48:27

which again is probably impossible, uh,

48:29

is to have them immediately corrected. But

48:33

that would be difficult, Like you'd have to have like a

48:35

standing conversation with somebody who

48:38

would not fall for your miscommunication,

48:40

you know, or you'd have to just be constantly, uh

48:43

like with with paranoia, just fact

48:45

checking everything you come across. Otherwise

48:48

some of them are going to get past your your

48:51

guard and they're not going to be instantly corrected.

48:54

And then they're just kinda they're just kind of in there.

48:56

Like even if you hear otherwise later, you

48:58

might still fall back to the earlier misconception.

49:02

Yeah, or it's just or it's something that doesn't come up

49:04

in daily life, you know, so you just there's

49:06

never been an opportunity for it to be corrected. I'm

49:08

reminded of that episode of This

49:11

American Life where they started off

49:13

by talking about this, uh, this this particular

49:15

individual who had just grown up

49:17

thinking that unicorns existed, like

49:20

it had never been corrected for, and so she

49:22

just had that misconception in her head until

49:24

finally she's at a party in

49:26

there and there's a conversation, like just a random

49:29

chatter about, hey, what are your favorite animals or something,

49:31

and she she mentions the unicorn and there's

49:33

like this awkward silence. So

49:36

why would that be all that awkward? I mean, would

49:39

she like the unicorn which is real? Well,

49:42

I think it was, it was probably why if I'm remembering

49:44

it correctly it was. There's a certain bit of ambiguity

49:46

where people are like, is she joking or oh

49:49

my goodness, she's not joking. She thinks there. But

49:51

it also makes all of us, I think, wonder, which,

49:54

what what misconceptions do we have just

49:57

rattling around in our brain right now? We

49:59

have no idea, but they're just they're ready to go at

50:01

any moment, you know, they can be loaded into

50:03

the torpedo tube of conversation or podcasting

50:06

or the next job interview, just just

50:09

just ready to go when you have no idea. I'd

50:11

say one of the most common edits I have to

50:13

make to this show before we release it as I

50:15

realized that I just sort of said something

50:18

that I knew was true. And then later

50:20

I'm listening back to it, I'm like, wait a minute, I

50:22

don't think that's right. Yeah, yeah,

50:24

I've definitely definitely done that before. But

50:27

well, I mean when I said it, I wasn't even wondering, you

50:29

know, just now now. The authors

50:31

here, they they touch on, of course, the fact

50:33

that the prior knowledge seems like it should

50:36

be able to protect us, uh, you know, and

50:38

and yet quote surprisingly, the effects of exposure

50:41

to misconceptions are not limited to cases where

50:43

people are ignorant of the true state of the world. We

50:45

touched on that already. Um. Another

50:48

great example they bring bring out is

50:50

a plane crashed, where did they bury the

50:52

survivors? Okay, which

50:55

you know obviously you're not going to bury survivors,

50:57

you were going to bury the dead. But again,

50:59

this is another question where you've kind of filled in all

51:01

the blanks, you know. Uh, they by

51:04

the time the survivors is the last word

51:06

in the sentence. Uh, and you fall for

51:08

it, right, So it's not like you think that

51:10

the survivors get buried, but you could be trying

51:13

to answer the questions just because like that's gone

51:15

straight past you. Yeah, And they

51:17

really drive home in this that knowledge neglect isn't

51:20

just a momentary lapse in memory, but rather something

51:22

with real consequences for memory. If

51:24

you don't recognize the error, the error

51:27

can become coded into your memory, into

51:29

your worldview as fact. Uh.

51:32

And because that error was recently encountered,

51:35

it's more easily accessed. So

51:37

again we have to remember that items in our

51:39

memory are not made of stone, they're

51:42

made of clay. Merely accessing

51:44

them can change them. And our most accessed

51:46

memories are the most changed memories

51:49

of all the ones we can trust the least.

51:51

Um So, an air that pops to mind

51:54

quickly is more likely to be thought of as

51:56

fact, not Oh I heard once

51:58

that X. I'm not sure about X, but I

52:01

think X, but rather just X is true,

52:03

X is the answer. Yeah, So I guess this is This

52:05

is connecting back to that finding we talked

52:07

about earlier that you know, um that

52:10

even against your existing prior

52:12

knowledge, like misconceptions or

52:15

errors that get by you unnoticed

52:17

in one of these Moses solution type sentences

52:19

can later damage your ability to remember

52:21

the actual fact of that sentence correctly.

52:24

Um, it can undermine your knowledge that it was

52:27

in fact Noah, potentially. And

52:29

this makes me think about the broader phenomenon of people

52:32

who are really trying to argue a point will often

52:36

structure sentences to try

52:38

to get something past you really

52:40

quickly. In the non pivotal part

52:43

of the sentence. It's almost like we have an intuitive

52:45

grasp of the Moses solution type

52:47

thing, where like a, I

52:50

don't know, you see people like like arguing about politics

52:52

on TV or something, and like so one

52:55

person will pose a question to the other person,

52:57

and the the pivotal part of

52:59

the sentence that's supposed to be in dispute, maybe

53:02

is is one part of the sentence, but then

53:04

in a different part of the sentence, there's also

53:06

like a disputable claim that's

53:09

just like shoved in there and goes by real

53:11

quick, right right, Yeah. If you end up

53:13

with a statement that has some some

53:15

mistruths sort of sprinkled in there

53:18

that are not key to the like the main you

53:20

know, talking point, or even the main untruth,

53:22

you know, that's that can often be the

53:25

nefarious thing too. It's like you catch the larger

53:28

um misconception or lie

53:30

in the statement, but then there are other lies in there that

53:33

you're not paying attention to because of the big one. Now,

53:35

the authors here they point out that improved monitoring

53:37

can help, you know, this is stuff like we're talking about,

53:40

like putting things in a different font, etcetera.

53:42

Um, But drawing attention

53:44

to errors can have the opposite effect,

53:47

increasing suggestibility, which

53:50

is is weird therefore to it as an ironic effect.

53:52

Um. Plus, many manipulations

53:55

designed to promote monitoring may

53:57

actually fail to do so, And they say it's difficult

53:59

to predict which manipulations will actually

54:01

work. So again, there's no there's no like

54:04

one guy, like, here are the three steps

54:06

you need to take to uh to keep

54:08

this misinformation from leaking into your brain.

54:11

I think a lot of what I take away from this is that,

54:13

uh, I don't know, being well informed

54:16

is an ongoing process that last

54:18

your entire life. And it's not a question

54:21

of like just getting the right facts in

54:23

the bank one time and then you're set.

54:25

You know. Yeah, there's

54:27

a lot of upkeep involved and a lot of

54:30

just continual pruning and not just new weeds,

54:32

weeds that have been in there your whole life

54:34

sometimes or seeing right bap the very

54:37

least. Um. Yeah.

54:39

The authors why, they also drive home that ultimately

54:42

we know a lot more about how people come

54:44

to misremember events versus

54:47

misremember facts, especially

54:49

when errors are are the errors

54:51

involved contradict stored knowledge.

54:53

So uh, you know, you know, again we

54:55

get into the complexity of memory, the

54:57

different types of memory that we have going on the

55:00

brain. Um, and we we still have

55:02

a lot more to learn about just

55:04

how this all comes together. Yeah.

55:07

Now, you know, here's a question that comes to mind. Um, I

55:09

wonder if anyone has constructed a Moses

55:11

illusion statement using Bilbo and Frodo.

55:14

Oh yes, that might so. Um,

55:17

like what was Bilbo carrying into

55:19

the fires of Mountain Doom? Yeah,

55:22

that's sort of thing. I don't know, of course, I guess you would want

55:24

to you'd want to try and construct it right

55:26

so that you get Bilbo there at the very end or

55:29

Frodo at the very end, depending on how you're you're you're

55:31

messing around with its rum

55:34

who was who was the dragon whose lair

55:36

was infiltrated by Frodo Baggins. Yeah,

55:39

yeah, that sort of thing that might work. Yeah, I

55:42

said, Bilbo and Frodo or even closer together

55:44

than Noah and Moses. Yeah, I mean they

55:46

are certainly that they actually

55:48

overlap, as opposed

55:50

to being separated by by long stretches of

55:53

time. Very very similar

55:55

characters actually related, right, they are related?

55:57

Yeah? Um, yeah, it's

56:00

that they would work. What Uncle, great

56:02

uncle Uncle, So I always forget what happened

56:04

to Frodo's parents. I've read it and I

56:07

still forget it. I'm gonna say, uncle,

56:09

all the all the Hobbits are cousins. Yeah, they're

56:11

all related. Actually, yes, all

56:14

right, well there you have it. We'd love to hear from

56:16

everybody about this, because of course this just touches on

56:18

how our brains work and how it now they are

56:20

brains work with with new

56:23

information, be it accurate or

56:26

or or or misconception. Uh

56:28

So I think everybody out there has something to

56:30

share. Which of these Moses

56:33

illusions worked the most on you? Which

56:35

ones I've worked on you in the past. Uh, we'd

56:37

love to hear from you. All right. If

56:40

you want to check out other episodes of Stuff to Blow your

56:42

Mind, you know where to find it. You can find

56:44

the Stuff to Blow your Mind podcast feed wherever you get

56:46

your your podcasts, and we'll have core episodes

56:48

of Stuff to Blow your Mind on Tuesdays and Thursdays,

56:51

you've got listener mail. On Mondays, you've got them,

56:53

We've got the Artifact on Wednesdays. You've got Weird

56:56

House Cinema on Fridays in a vault episode

56:58

on the weekends, Huge things.

57:00

As always to our excellent audio producer Seth

57:02

Nicholas Johnson. If you would like to get in

57:05

touch with us with feedback on this episode

57:07

or any other, to suggest a topic for the future,

57:09

or just to say hello, you can email us at contact

57:12

at Stuff to Blow Your Mind dot com. Stuff

57:22

to Blow Your Mind is production of I Heart Radio.

57:24

For more podcasts for my heart Radio, this is the i

57:26

heart Radio app, Apple Podcasts, or wherever

57:29

you're listening to your favorite shows.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features