Podchaser Logo
Home
Why Your Memory Seems Bad (It’s Not Just Age)

Why Your Memory Seems Bad (It’s Not Just Age)

Released Monday, 22nd April 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Why Your Memory Seems Bad (It’s Not Just Age)

Why Your Memory Seems Bad (It’s Not Just Age)

Why Your Memory Seems Bad (It’s Not Just Age)

Why Your Memory Seems Bad (It’s Not Just Age)

Monday, 22nd April 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

Get your advanced PhD in WOW

0:02

from Floor & Decor. If

0:04

you're a pro, you're already an expert in tile,

0:06

wood and stone. And with Floor

0:09

& Decor's job site delivery, their free design

0:11

services, and pro rewards that actually reward you,

0:14

your business is set to grow from one client

0:16

to the next. Floor & Decor isn't

0:18

just a couple of vials, it's an entire

0:20

store designed to help your business boom.

0:23

It's Floor & Decor. On

0:27

May 10th, Kingdom of the Planet of the Apes

0:30

is coming to IMAX and theaters

0:32

everywhere. This

0:36

summer, one movie event will rain.

0:39

It is our time. Apes hunt

0:41

humans. That is wrong. Bend

0:45

for your king. Never. Kingdom

0:48

of the Planet of the Apes, only in theaters

0:50

May 10. Tickets on sale now. Rated PG-13. Some

0:53

material may be inappropriate for children under 13. Brett

1:04

McKay here and welcome to another edition of

1:07

the Art of Manliness podcast. Do you sometimes

1:09

walk into another room in your house to

1:11

get something, but then can't remember what it

1:13

was you wanted? Do you sometimes forget about

1:15

an appointment or struggle to remember someone's name?

1:17

You may have chalked these lapses in memory

1:19

up to getting older. And age

1:21

can indeed play a role in the diminishing power of

1:23

memory. But as my guest will tell us,

1:25

there are other factors that play as well. Charin

1:28

Raghunath is a neuroscientist, psychologist, and

1:30

the author of Why We Remember,

1:33

unlocking memories' power to hold on to what matters.

1:35

Today on the show, Charin explains how factors

1:37

like how we direct our attention, take photos,

1:39

and move through something called event boundaries all

1:42

affect our memory. And how our

1:44

current context in life impacts which memories we're

1:46

able to recall from the past. We

1:49

always talk about how to reverse engineer these factors to improve

1:51

your memories. After the show's

1:53

over, check out our show notes at aom.is.memory.

2:07

Charin Raganath, welcome to the show. Thank

2:09

you very much, Brett. Great to be here. So

2:11

you are a neuroscientist and you've spent your career

2:14

20 plus years researching

2:16

memory. And we're going to talk today about

2:18

why we remember some things, why we forget

2:21

other stuff and what we can do to

2:23

improve our memory. But after I read

2:25

your book, one of the big takeaways I got from it

2:27

was that memory is more than

2:29

just an archive of our past. That

2:32

actually memory shapes our day-to-day

2:34

lives. So how does memory influence

2:36

our lives beyond just being able

2:39

to recall events? So

2:42

one just very kind of simple

2:44

example would be, let's say you wake up in a

2:47

hotel room, right? Your

2:49

first question as you wake up,

2:51

as you're a little disoriented and probably

2:53

without even thinking about it, you're having

2:55

this moment of where am I? And

2:59

just to situate yourself in time and space, right? It's

3:01

like you can look around, you know where you are

3:03

in the room, but where's this room? It could be

3:05

in like a prison somewhere or it

3:08

could be in like a resort, who knows? And

3:11

so you have to rely on memory just to get

3:13

to that point to dig you out of that hole

3:15

and tell you exactly where you are. So

3:18

let's take a slightly

3:20

more complex example now. Like

3:22

let's say, for instance, you are

3:25

trying to choose which restaurant you want to go to. It's

3:28

like a usual restaurant that's pretty good, but then

3:30

lately they changed the menu and you know the

3:32

last time you went there, you had a terrible

3:34

meal. So you can use

3:36

memory to basically say, you know what? I'm going

3:39

to go to someplace different this time. And

3:41

then we can take something like a big choice. So

3:44

I decided to go into research in

3:47

cognitive neuroscience, but my training was in

3:49

clinical psychology and I actually had the

3:51

chance to do a clinical internship in

3:53

which I could have been on a

3:56

career path to make lots of money in a

3:59

clinical career. And when

4:01

I look back on that decision,

4:03

what I asked myself was essentially,

4:06

what are the kinds of moments

4:08

that I feel most comfortable in,

4:10

that I'm happiest about? When

4:12

I thought about the instances in which I was

4:14

in the clinic, I thought, okay, I have to

4:16

be dressed up well, I have to be there

4:18

early in the morning, I have to

4:21

be on when people say there's a lot of

4:23

pressure because if I don't get things right, bad

4:25

things can happen, versus the

4:27

times that I could remember from

4:29

being in research where I was staying

4:31

up late, drinking beers and eating pizza in

4:33

the lab while we were working late for

4:35

a conference or something like that, and the

4:38

people that I hung out with in the

4:40

lab versus the more formal

4:42

environments in the clinic, and it was just

4:44

a no-brainer. And so these

4:47

hard life decisions are

4:49

very, very complicated. We're making them based

4:51

on insufficient information. And so we rely

4:53

on memory to give us that

4:55

data that we need to make these decisions about

4:57

our future. And as we'll

5:00

see in this conversation too, memory is connected

5:02

to a lot of other things in our

5:04

lives that we might not think are connected

5:06

to memory. The ability to imagine things, that's

5:08

connected to memory. How we situate ourselves not

5:10

only in place, like in

5:12

that example you gave, you wake up in a hotel room, you're

5:14

like, where the heck am I? But also in time. Well, let's

5:16

get to this question here, I think a lot of people might have this. Why

5:19

do we remember some things but not

5:21

others? And then the follow-up question is,

5:24

what can that answer tell us

5:26

about how memory works? When

5:29

we look at the design of the brain,

5:31

what you see over and over and

5:33

over again, whatever system you look at, is

5:36

that the brain is optimizing to make

5:38

the most of a little bit of information.

5:41

And so what I mean by that is if

5:43

we see the world, we're not literally looking at

5:45

everything, we're only grabbing little bits and pieces of

5:47

the world with our eyes by just moving our

5:49

eyes and focusing at different places, and

5:52

then assembling that into a meaningful picture.

5:54

So we know that even our

5:56

ability to perceive the world is limited and our

5:58

ability to hold the world. things in attention

6:00

is limited. So what

6:03

makes things memorable and

6:05

what makes things grab our attention, there's

6:07

a high relationship between them. They're often

6:10

things that are biologically important. So something

6:12

that you'll probably find this, I imagine

6:14

yourself Brett, if you look back on

6:16

things in your life, you probably

6:19

remember the first things that will come to

6:21

mind will be the highs and lows, right?

6:23

Things that are very emotionally, you know, they're

6:25

exciting or times where you were scared or

6:28

times where you felt intense desire. And these

6:30

are biologically important moments where there are

6:33

chemicals in the brain that promote plasticity

6:35

that are released during these moments. So

6:37

that right off the bat tells you

6:39

something about why some events are memorable

6:41

is because they're

6:43

biologically important. Other events that

6:45

would be also important

6:47

would be things that are new or things

6:49

that are surprising. So we often

6:52

remember these events that really surprise

6:54

us because they stick

6:56

out and some of that is related

6:58

to a phenomenon I'll get into

7:00

with regards to interference. But

7:03

some of it is also when we're surprised or when

7:05

we're in a brand new place that we've never been

7:07

to before. Again there's these

7:09

release of neuromodulators, these chemicals in

7:11

our brain that promote plasticity. So

7:14

those are some of the key factors and

7:16

another key factor as I mentioned is

7:19

the fact that memories compete with each other and

7:21

this is a phenomenon called interference. So I think

7:24

intuitively we might think of memory as being like

7:26

I store a bunch of files in my hard

7:28

disk and more or less if I

7:30

store 10 files or if I store 20 files

7:32

it doesn't make a difference. But

7:34

that's not how memory works. In human

7:37

memory the memories are competing

7:39

with each other and so if

7:41

I'm trying to remember Brett

7:43

let's say your name I meet you sometime in

7:45

person we go into the real world as opposed

7:47

to the virtual world. I meet you we have

7:50

a beer something like that then

7:52

later on I meet someone named Brit. Well

7:55

remembering Brit is going to be complicated because

7:57

I've just learned about Brett and there's going

7:59

to be this interference between them.

8:02

So the way that memories can survive

8:04

that competition is if there's something distinctive

8:06

that makes this memory different from something

8:08

else. So if I had

8:11

something about your name and I could

8:13

tie it with something interesting about

8:15

you that I learned and make that all

8:17

into one big story, for instance, then now

8:19

all of a sudden you're very, very different

8:22

from Brit because Brit's just sound that I

8:24

heard. And this is

8:26

the way in which memories can stick

8:28

around, is if we're attending

8:30

to something that allows us to

8:33

capture what's unique about this moment

8:35

in time. So the sights, the

8:37

sounds, the smells, emotions,

8:39

something that you think about that's unique.

8:43

Does our brain store memories in

8:45

a specific part of the brain? Well

8:49

this is a very tough question to

8:51

answer because essentially it comes down to

8:53

what is the memory. So there's many

8:55

different ways memory can be manifest. One

8:57

is your ability to just

8:59

call upon facts, general knowledge that you

9:02

have about the world and that's called

9:04

semantic memory. And then there's

9:06

your ability to remember specific events in

9:08

your life like episodic memory. So

9:11

I know that Def Leppard was a

9:13

British metal band that played very melodic

9:15

songs in the 1980s,

9:17

but that's different than my memory for seeing

9:20

them in the round during the hysteria

9:22

tour which was a little bit after

9:24

they had peaked. But nonetheless, that's an

9:26

episodic memory from one point in time.

9:29

And so those kinds of memories differ from each

9:31

other. Now the hippocampus is

9:33

an area of the brain that's known

9:35

to be very important for forming new

9:37

episodic memories. And

9:39

it doesn't do it by itself, but what

9:41

it does is it ties together all of

9:43

these different parts of the brain that are

9:45

processing the different kinds of aspects of the

9:48

semantics of your world. Does that make sense?

9:50

No, that's making sense so far, yeah. Yeah.

9:54

So a lot of what people think of

9:56

when they think of the memory

9:58

loss, for instance, they that you see

10:00

in the earliest stages of Alzheimer's disease,

10:02

that's related in part to the loss

10:05

of the hippocampus because what happens is

10:07

people start to lose this ability to

10:09

form new episodic memories. They

10:11

still have knowledge of who they are, all the

10:13

people they know in those early stages, but

10:16

they lose this ability to form new

10:18

episodic memories. And so that's why

10:20

the hippocampus is such a big player in memories

10:22

because it plays this role in just arbitrarily

10:26

saying, in some ways the hippocampus,

10:28

I mean, if we were to pretend the

10:30

hippocampus is a person instead of a brain

10:33

area, you could say, well, it's being deliberately

10:35

dumb. It's not thinking about why things should

10:37

go together. It's just saying, hey, I

10:40

happened to see Brett in the pub

10:43

while the song was playing in the

10:45

background all at the same moment in

10:47

time. And that's what the memory is.

10:49

It's just this random coincidence of factors.

10:52

Okay, so the hippocampus is involved

10:54

in episodic memories, something

10:56

that you've researched a lot and found, and

10:59

it's been groundbreaking, is the role the

11:01

prefrontal cortex plays in memory.

11:04

People might be familiar with the idea that the

11:06

prefrontal cortex can be used as

11:08

short-term memory. It's sort of used as the

11:10

analogy is the prefrontal cortex is like RAM,

11:12

it's like working memory. So if you need

11:14

to temporarily remember something, prefrontal cortex

11:16

can take that, but for like longer term memories,

11:18

you go to the hippocampus. What your research has

11:20

found is no, the prefrontal cortex actually

11:23

plays a bigger role in those long-term

11:25

memories. What role does the prefrontal cortex

11:27

play? Yeah, I think

11:29

that I'm really glad you brought up that

11:32

RAM analogy because I think that was very

11:34

popular for a long time in psychology that

11:36

we used to think of humans as being

11:38

like computers in this very kind

11:40

of straightforward way, but we're not. What

11:42

the prefrontal cortex seems to be

11:45

about is it's kind of

11:47

a, again, I'm gonna use these analogies just

11:49

to keep things simple, although I hope people

11:51

with a more scientific background will get mad

11:53

at me for this. But a

11:56

lot of people use the term executive

11:58

to describe what the prefrontal cortex is. financial cortex does.

12:00

And what that means is, you

12:02

know, an executive who's running a company

12:05

really has no useful skills, right? They're not

12:07

like, you're not going to trust them with

12:09

the accounting, you're not going to trust them

12:11

to like handle the mail room or anything

12:14

like that. But their job

12:16

is really to oversee everything coordinated

12:18

towards a common goal. And

12:20

that's what the prefrontal cortex is all about. So for

12:22

a long time, people used to think, oh, the prefrontal

12:24

cortex doesn't do anything

12:27

because it's not people could

12:29

lose a prefrontal cortex and they would still

12:31

walk and talk and have all the knowledge they did

12:33

before. But they couldn't

12:35

function in the real world because they had no

12:38

ability to use that information to get

12:41

their goals achieved. And

12:44

so you brought up this idea of short term

12:46

memory. And so part of the idea of being

12:48

able to hold a phone number, say in short

12:50

term memory, like if I give you a phone

12:52

number, if I say, hey, here's your temporary password,

12:54

I need you to reset it so that you

12:56

can get back into your bank account. You're

12:59

keeping that information in mind. But to

13:01

do that, you have to keep yourself

13:03

from being distracted, right? So there's a

13:06

kid crying in the background, or maybe

13:08

you're getting a text alert on your

13:10

phone and you have to suppress those

13:12

distractions to focus on what's relevant. And

13:15

that's where the prefrontal cortex comes in. But

13:19

that same ability is also what

13:21

allows you to be present

13:23

in the moment and focus on what's

13:25

important, like where I put my keys

13:27

or where I put my phone, as

13:30

opposed to the things that may be

13:32

less important but could grab your attention,

13:34

like the sound of a dog

13:36

barking or a kettle whistling or something. Things that you

13:38

need to take care of, but they're not necessarily related

13:40

to these other long term goals. And

13:43

so this idea that the prefrontal cortex

13:46

directs our attention to stuff that we want

13:48

to remember, and if we're

13:50

distracted, we might not remember that thing. That

13:53

explains why we forget, where did I put my

13:55

keys or where did I put my wallet? Because

13:57

your prefrontal cortex kind of checked out when you

13:59

just... you dropped them on the counter and you weren't paying

14:01

attention. So it was just like, yeah, we're

14:03

not gonna remember that. That's

14:06

exactly right. Yeah, yeah, that's exactly right.

14:08

And so, and sometimes what happens is

14:10

our prefrontal cortex isn't checking out, but

14:12

it's actually, sometimes we intentionally do it.

14:15

We switch from one task to another.

14:17

We'll go, oh, yeah, I'm gonna

14:19

be like, I'm

14:21

walking in the door, I've got my keys, but

14:24

then I just decide habitually to check my

14:26

email or something. And so the

14:28

prefrontal cortex is saying, okay, let's shift gears. Now

14:31

my goal is to check email. And

14:33

so the next time when

14:35

you go back to your keys, you're already

14:37

a step behind because your prefrontal

14:39

cortex has to use all these resources just

14:41

to shift back from the email task

14:43

back to whatever it was you were doing when you opened

14:46

the door. And so

14:48

as a result, our resources become

14:50

too depleted, spread too thin,

14:52

and we can't focus in on what we need.

14:54

So sometimes the prefrontal cortex is there, but

14:57

we misdirected because we have bad habits.

15:00

Or it could just be overwhelmed. I think you highlighted

15:02

some research, how constantly using

15:04

social media, that can inhibit

15:06

memory because your prefrontal cortex has got

15:08

all this information, you're blasting

15:11

it, and then it can't remember stuff you

15:13

actually wanna remember. That's exactly

15:15

right, yeah. So you can be blasted

15:17

both by switching between these things. And

15:20

again, a lot of this is under

15:22

our control, so to speak, meaning that

15:24

we don't have to check social media all

15:27

the time. Like right now, if

15:29

I was being sloppy, I would be

15:31

checking social media in

15:33

between points in our conversation,

15:35

which would be horrible for my

15:38

ability to remember our conversation later on, which is

15:40

why I turned off all my

15:42

alerts and I went into focus mode for

15:44

this conversation because otherwise I'd be

15:46

having this conversation. And then somebody would say, hey, what

15:48

did you do today? And I'd be like, I was

15:51

on this amazing podcast, but I

15:53

can't remember anything about it. Yeah,

15:55

and this might explain why as you

15:58

get older, I mean, there's a couple. things going

16:00

on as you get older while your memory feels like it's

16:02

not as sharp. But I just say as you get older

16:04

into your thirties and your forties and your fifties, you have

16:06

a lot more going on in your life, a lot more

16:09

stuff to keep track of, keeping track of your kid's schedule,

16:11

your work schedule, things that need to be done on the

16:13

house. And so yeah, you're probably

16:15

going to forget that your glasses are on top

16:17

of your head because you got so much going

16:19

on. That's a very

16:21

good point. You know, so as we get older,

16:23

there's a bunch of things that happen. So one

16:25

is that we have a lot of stresses, we

16:27

have a lot of pressures and we have a

16:29

lot of competing things and deadlines

16:31

and so forth. And so when

16:34

we're under stress, the natural

16:36

response of the brain is to down

16:38

regulate the prefrontal cortex. You want to

16:40

go into a more of a responsive

16:42

mode rather than a mode of planning

16:45

and deliberation. And so

16:47

we're now compromised because of that stress.

16:49

But then on top of it, as

16:51

we get older, on average,

16:53

the prefrontal cortex shrinks a little bit.

16:56

It's not functioning as efficiently as it

16:58

should. And then, you know,

17:00

we're maybe having some health issues. Maybe

17:02

we just got over a bout of

17:05

COVID. Maybe you're not sleeping as

17:07

well as you used to. And

17:09

so all of these factors can compromise the frontal

17:11

cortex even more. So one of the things I

17:14

think a lot about is how

17:16

modern life is just optimized to

17:18

deplete our mental resources and put

17:20

us in the state of perpetual

17:22

amnesia. Okay. So the

17:25

prefrontal cortex plays a role in

17:27

memory by directing where we place

17:29

our attention. And then when we

17:31

don't give something sufficient attention, we

17:33

can't remember it. So, you know, if we got a lot going

17:35

on in our lives, we tend to be

17:37

forgetful because there's just too many things to pay

17:40

attention to. It overwhelms the prefrontal

17:42

cortex. And then when you're multitasking, you

17:44

know, task switching a lot, you can't

17:46

give anyone task enough attention to remember

17:49

what's going on with it and do it well.

17:52

And then all these things, it can cause stress

17:54

and that can deplete the strength of the prefrontal

17:56

cortex, as well as other things like lack of

17:58

sleep, something related to the, is

18:00

how the use of smartphone cameras

18:03

affects how we remember an experience. What

18:05

does the research say there? So

18:09

on average, the research shows pretty

18:11

significantly that when we use cameras

18:13

to document our lives, we actually

18:15

have a paradoxically lower

18:17

memory for those events. And

18:20

I think people have this intuitive idea that if

18:22

I take a picture of this event, I will

18:24

remember it. And in theory, that

18:26

could be true, but what often happens is

18:29

people don't go back to

18:31

the pictures, right? Because we collect gobs

18:33

and gobs of pictures. And

18:35

then on top of it, we're mindlessly documenting

18:37

these things. And you can see this with

18:39

the rise of Instagram walls everywhere, right? So

18:41

it becomes no longer about

18:44

the experience but about the picture. And

18:47

so what happens is that people tend to

18:49

have a poor memory for these experiences when

18:51

they've been focused on taking the pictures and

18:53

posting them. Now, it doesn't have

18:55

to be that way. So you could be more

18:57

selective in the way that you

19:00

take pictures and use the camera as a

19:02

tool for grounding you in the

19:04

moment and say, what's really going on here?

19:06

What's interesting here? And then selectively

19:08

take pictures that are planting cues in

19:10

your mind for later on being able

19:12

to remember them. Because that's what a

19:15

lot of memory is, is

19:17

if you have the right cues, some

19:19

distinctive thing that you're seeing or smelling

19:21

or hearing, that's

19:23

what allows you to go back and revisit

19:26

that moment. And so we can

19:28

be mindful about picture taking and study

19:30

one study found that if people are

19:32

in that kind of condition, you

19:35

can actually improve memory. Another

19:37

way you can do it is by

19:39

actually going back to those pictures. So

19:41

we can think of like an Instagram

19:44

story or a Snapchat post as

19:46

being a metaphor for how photos actually have

19:48

this amnesic quality where you post something and

19:50

then two days it disappears. And this is

19:52

what I think we often do with our

19:55

photos. But if you actually, one

19:57

of the things they do like is what's called

19:59

Facebook memories where they put on a photo

20:01

that you haven't seen in years, but

20:03

you posted it a while back. And

20:05

that's now a cue to recall that

20:07

memory. And the act of recalling that

20:09

memory now makes it more accessible later

20:11

on. So that way you can

20:14

remember it again. So the act of remembering

20:16

makes it more memorable. Okay,

20:18

so if you're gonna take pictures, I think going back

20:20

to what we were saying about the role of prefrontal

20:22

cortex in memory, if you're just

20:24

focused on taking the perfect picture and thinking, oh,

20:26

this would be great for Instagram and what are

20:28

my friends gonna think about, the way you're directing

20:30

your attention, putting it on the

20:33

picture taking itself, I

20:35

mean, you're not really present, you're not there. And

20:37

because of that, you're not gonna remember the experience

20:40

as much. But you could,

20:42

if you direct your attention differently, even while taking

20:44

a picture, that can enhance your

20:46

memory if you wanted to. Yes,

20:48

I would say that if you can use

20:51

your prefrontal cortex to say, if my goal

20:53

is to have a memorable experience, I

20:55

can actually, first of all, think

20:57

about what's in front of me, think about

20:59

the sights and the sounds and the smells

21:01

and so forth and immerse myself in it,

21:04

immerse myself in this moment. But

21:06

then when I do take pictures, you can actually

21:08

ask yourself, what would be a good reminder of

21:10

this moment? What are the points in this moment

21:12

that I wanna remember? And what are the points

21:14

in this moment that I don't wanna document? I

21:17

think lots of times we just take pictures without

21:19

ever even thinking, is this the

21:21

memory that I wanna be calling back?

21:23

Because ultimately, once we start taking these

21:25

pictures, those pictures will have

21:27

a disproportionate effect on what we

21:29

remember. So how many times have

21:32

you taken vacation and you take

21:34

pictures and the events that you

21:36

remember later on are those events that you photographed

21:38

and the ones that you didn't photograph get thrown

21:40

to the side, has this ever happened to you?

21:42

Yeah, not for

21:44

sure. So yeah, so that's, I think part of

21:46

it is the camera can be a tool and

21:49

again, if you use your frontal cortex to say,

21:51

what do I want out of this experience? The

21:53

camera can be a tool to get it as

21:56

opposed to a distraction that just takes you away

21:58

from what you want. So going back

22:01

to this idea of episodic memory, this is

22:03

sort of remembering events in our lives that

22:05

happened to us. Why

22:07

is it that we have a harder time

22:09

with episodic memory as we get older? So

22:11

I think we mentioned some things, right? You

22:14

have just a lot going on in your

22:16

life, there's stress, your prefrontal cortex,

22:18

shrinkage, you get older. But

22:21

I mean, I've noticed this in my own life, and I think you talked

22:23

about this in the book, I can remember

22:25

stuff from when I was, you know, middle

22:28

school through age 30, like very

22:32

vividly. I remember college, I remember

22:34

traveling internationally, I remember, you know,

22:36

high school football. But then

22:38

after age 30-ish, things are kind of

22:40

like, I kind of remember doing that,

22:42

but it's not as much detail as

22:45

those, you know, teenage years. What's going on

22:47

there? Well, this is something

22:49

that's very, very common. In fact, memory researchers

22:51

have a name for it, which is the

22:54

reminiscence bump. And the

22:56

idea behind the reminiscence bump is that if

22:59

you just plot the number of memories that people will

23:01

report, if you ask them about different times of their

23:03

lives, and you just make a little graph out of

23:05

it, there's a big bump in the graph from the

23:07

years between the ages of 18 to 30. And

23:10

there's a number of reasons for that. And one

23:12

big reason is that that's when our sense of

23:15

who we are is actually emerging.

23:18

And so the experiences that we have during

23:20

the time period are very tied to our

23:23

sense of identity. And

23:25

that's the time when we're forming our

23:27

tastes in music and food, and we're

23:29

finding the friendships that will help define

23:31

us and so forth. And so we

23:33

tend to call upon those memories more

23:35

as a result. And

23:37

as I was saying, the memories that you call upon

23:40

the most will be strengthened each

23:42

time you call upon them. Another

23:44

theory as to why we remember

23:46

more from our youth is that

23:48

memory is enhanced when we encounter

23:51

something novel. And when we're

23:53

young, we've got a lot of novel things. There's a

23:55

lot of firsts. We do a lot of new things.

23:58

And so when the brain encounters that... It's

24:01

memory camera is like oh hey. This is novel.

24:03

We this might be important Let's we're gonna take

24:05

a lot of footage of this so

24:07

then when you look back on it There's

24:09

a lot of memory footage to unspool, but

24:12

as adults you know we tend to get

24:14

into a routine We experience less novelty you

24:16

know each day I mean even year is

24:18

just a lot like the last one so

24:21

the memory camera. It's like turns off It's like

24:23

well. I've seen this before

24:25

no need to capture it so when we look

24:27

back There's not a lot of memory footage to

24:29

unspool, so you want more memories in adulthood You're

24:33

gonna have to do more novel things more

24:35

more memorable things We're

24:37

going to take a quick break for your Words More sponsors. It's

24:41

wedding season and while weddings are all about the bride and groom,

24:44

they're also a chance for you to look your best. With

24:46

a fully custom suit from Indochino, you'll walk into

24:48

wedding season looking like a million bucks, even though

24:50

suits started just $499. It's

24:53

easier than ever to be the best dressed with Indochino.

24:55

Order your custom suits now and they'll be ready for

24:57

wedding season. The process of customizing a suit on Indochino,

25:00

very easy to do. It's also a lot of fun.

25:02

I've talked about my navy blue suit I've gotten from

25:04

Indochino. First off, you can decide how the jacket is,

25:06

how you want the pockets on the jacket to be,

25:08

how you want the lapels, the vent

25:10

on the back, the lining on the pants. You can

25:13

decide you want pleats or no pleats. I want

25:15

no pleats on my pants on this one. Then

25:17

you do the measuring process. You'll need someone to help

25:19

you, but they have this very easy to follow

25:21

guide. Send that all in. In

25:23

a few weeks, you have a custom made to measure

25:25

suit sent directly to your door and you're going to

25:27

pay about the same price for a suit that you'd

25:29

pay for an off the rack suit at a department

25:32

store. Look your best this wedding season at the table

25:34

or on the dance floor when you wear Indochino. off

25:40

any purchase of three hundred and ninety nine dollars

25:42

or more That's I N D

25:44

O C H I N O dot-com

25:46

promo code manliness check it out today

25:49

Spring is here and the McKay's gonna be really busy. I'm

25:51

coaching flag football again My daughter's playing basketball and on those

25:53

nights when we don't have time to cook a full meal

25:55

and clean up We like to eat

25:57

factor meals factor meals are ready-to-eat

25:59

meals that are on the table in

26:01

less than two minutes. They're fresh, they're not

26:03

frozen, they're chef-crafted and dietitian approved. You eliminate

26:05

the hassle of prepping, cooking or cleaning up.

26:07

You just simply heat and savor the good

26:10

stuff and all these meals are really, really

26:12

nutritious. They're macro-friendly if you're tracking your macros.

26:14

That's another reason why I like them. So

26:16

we've been using factory meals for a while.

26:18

One thing I've enjoyed lately is the green

26:20

chili chicken. Super tasty. I also like the

26:22

creamy mushroom chicken and rice. Very good. It

26:24

also comes with garlic green beans. And again,

26:26

you just warm these things up in two

26:28

minutes and then you have this really delicious

26:31

meal. And best of all, clean up some breeze. If

26:33

you wanna try this out, head

26:35

to factorymeals.com/manliness50 and use

26:37

code manliness50, that's manliness50, to

26:40

get 50% off your first month plus 20% off

26:43

your next month. It's a good deal. That's

26:45

code manliness50, manliness50 at

26:48

factorymeals.com/manliness50. Get

26:51

50% off your first box plus 20% off your next

26:53

box while your subscription is active. Check it out today.

26:55

Have you ever been to an event where you just

26:57

had to get the best seats? Maybe it was a

26:59

concert for your favorite band or maybe it was an

27:01

important game for your favorite team. Well,

27:03

to do that, you had to set an alarm to get the

27:05

tickets as soon as they went on sale. When

27:08

you want the best, you have to act quickly or someone

27:10

else will get it instead. It's like if

27:12

you are trying to hire for your business and you

27:14

wanna find the most talented people for your open roles

27:16

before the competition scoops them up. So what's

27:18

the best way to do that? ZipRecruiter. ZipRecruiter

27:21

finds qualified candidates fast. And right now,

27:23

you can try it for free at

27:26

ziprecruiter.com/manliness. ZipRecruiter's powerful matching technology

27:28

takes center stage to identify

27:30

top talent for your roles.

27:33

Immediately after you post your job, ZipRecruiter's

27:35

smart technology starts showing you qualified people

27:37

for it. Amp up

27:39

your hiring performance with ZipRecruiter and find

27:41

the best people fast. See

27:43

why four out of five employers who post a

27:45

job on ZipRecruiter get a quality candidate within the

27:47

first day. Just go to this

27:50

exclusive web address right now to try ZipRecruiter

27:52

for free. ziprecruiter.com/manliness.

27:56

Again, that's ziprecruiter.com/manliness.

28:00

L-I-N-E-S-S, ZipRecruiter, the

28:02

smartest way to hire. All right, picture that

28:04

thing you've always wanted to learn. Now picture

28:06

learning it from the person who's literally the

28:08

best at it in the world. That's

28:11

what you get with Masterclass. This year,

28:13

learn from the best to become your

28:15

best with Masterclass. Masterclass offers over 180

28:17

world-class instructors, and a lot of them

28:19

are former AOM podcast guests. So for

28:21

example, you can learn the art of

28:23

negotiation from Chris Voss, we had him

28:25

on the podcast, leadership from Jocko Winnilink,

28:28

another AOM podcast guest, writing from Malcolm

28:30

Gladwell, another AOM podcast guest. You name

28:32

it, Masterclass has it. With Masterclass, you

28:34

get unlimited access to intimate one-on-one classes

28:36

with the world's best. Plus, every

28:38

new membership comes with a 30-day money-back guarantee,

28:40

so there's no risk. So I did the

28:42

Masterclass with Chris Voss on negotiation. Very well

28:44

done, he talks about tactical empathy, how

28:47

to mirror somebody so that you can develop rapport

28:49

with them. So if you enjoyed that podcast I

28:51

did with Chris Voss several years ago about his

28:53

book, Never Split the Difference, you're gonna love this

28:55

Masterclass. And right now, AOM listeners will get an

28:57

additional 15% off an

28:59

annual membership at masterclass.com/AOM.

29:02

Get 15% off right

29:04

now at masterclass.com/AOM, masterclass.com/AOM.

29:07

Check out the negotiation

29:09

class with Chris Voss.

29:15

And now back to the show. How

29:18

can understanding how episodic memory

29:20

works help us understand why

29:22

it is when we go into

29:24

the kitchen for something, we

29:26

forget why we went into the kitchen? What's

29:29

going on there? This is

29:31

one of my favorite topics and actually something

29:33

that we're studying a lot in my lab

29:35

right now. Even though our

29:37

lives are continuous, what happens is we

29:40

tend to remember our lives as

29:42

a series of events, like I went

29:45

to the kitchen, I went to someone's

29:47

21st birthday party, blah, blah, blah. And

29:50

so what we think happens

29:52

is that as we go about our

29:54

day, you're creating a little story

29:56

in your head that's okay, so my job

29:58

right now is to. talk to Brett and

30:01

answer this question. I'm keeping your question in

30:03

mind. I'm thinking of all these ways of

30:05

answering it. But then we move

30:07

on to another question and I flush that

30:09

information out. I focus on the new question

30:11

you're asking. And that time when

30:13

I pivot from question one to question two

30:15

is what we would call an event boundary.

30:18

It means that one event is over, another has

30:20

begun. And what we can see

30:22

when we scan people's brains is at those

30:25

moments, there's almost a tectonic shift in the

30:27

patterns of brain activity where you

30:29

see this change in patterns

30:31

as people's story about the

30:33

world changes in a moment. And so what's

30:35

interesting is that even the act of just

30:38

moving from one room to another can give

30:40

you that. So if I take

30:42

a few steps right now, if I were just to

30:44

stand up, take a few steps to my right, I'd

30:46

be in my room and then one more step and

30:48

I'm crossing into the hallway. And

30:50

even though it's just another step, I

30:53

would psychologically feel like I'm in a

30:55

new place. And that change in your

30:58

spatial context is enough to

31:00

create an event boundary. And

31:02

because context is so important for memory that

31:04

as episodic memories are so tied to a

31:06

time and a place, that in

31:08

the time it takes me to go to the kitchen, now

31:11

I've shifted across two or three rooms.

31:14

And now when I go back and try to remember why

31:16

I went to the kitchen, I have

31:18

to engage in this act of mental time

31:20

travel to recall what I was doing back

31:23

in that time period when I was in

31:25

my room. And that's why

31:27

it's often helpful if you go back to the room you

31:29

were in originally, you'll remember why you

31:31

went to the kitchen. That's

31:33

right. Yeah, that's right. So then what

31:35

happens is for me personally, I'll go to the

31:38

kitchen, I'll say, oh, what was I here for?

31:40

Then I'll just grab some food and eat it. And

31:42

then I come back to my office and I'm like,

31:45

oh my God, I left my phone in there. Yeah.

31:47

I realized, okay, over the course of the

31:50

past year, I've probably consumed thousands of calories

31:52

because of these damn event boundaries. As

31:55

I read about that idea about event

31:57

boundaries, it made me wonder if this

31:59

can help. explain whenever

32:01

I read stuff on a digital device like my

32:03

smartphone with the Kindle app, I

32:05

don't remember as much as when I'm reading from

32:07

a paperback book. I think it could be because

32:09

when you're on a device, I could

32:12

be on the Kindle app and then immediately I could

32:14

talk over to Instagram or my email. That

32:16

event boundary, when I'm reading on my phone,

32:19

it's just really porous. I'm switching back and

32:21

forth between events, so I'm

32:23

remembering less about each. But with

32:25

a book, like a paperback book,

32:28

there's a clear event boundary in its pages.

32:30

When I'm reading, I'm reading. I'm just in

32:32

the book. It's just one

32:34

event. I've noticed that whenever I

32:36

read a paperback book, I remember

32:38

it more. I can find things and I can

32:41

remember where some quote that I highlighted is.

32:43

I know which part of the book it's in,

32:46

but I don't have that experience when I'm reading on the

32:48

Kindle app. Yeah. So there's

32:50

definitely physical aspects of holding a

32:52

book that are different than the way we

32:54

interact with a Kindle, for instance. That

32:58

can lead our reading experience change,

33:00

which is going to change memorability. One

33:04

of the things that you mentioned is just the fact that

33:06

if you're using a device that has more than just a

33:08

reading app on it, it's just

33:10

so tempting to think of other things. When

33:14

you pick up that device, your brain

33:16

is considering all the possible tasks you

33:18

could do on that device. It's

33:20

almost like you're at a buffet. I don't

33:22

know about you, but if I ever go to a buffet,

33:24

I'm eating one thing, but I'm thinking about all the other

33:26

things I could be eating. So again,

33:28

I'm never really there. With

33:31

a book on the other hand, you have no choice. You're sort

33:33

of stuck with it. Also

33:35

with a book, there's a way in

33:37

which there's a spatial sense of where

33:39

the plot is because essentially there's a

33:42

physical place for each word on this

33:44

book. But on the screen, it's

33:46

a little different because every page appears on the same

33:48

screen. It

33:50

gives you a little bit more distinctiveness. All

33:53

these factors put together, I believe,

33:56

make it easier. I think you brought this up

33:58

in your example. If I'm

34:00

reading page 100, I

34:02

often have to think back to what happened in page

34:05

70 in order to

34:07

be able to understand what's happening in page

34:09

100. And that's

34:11

easier to do, I think. At least it

34:13

feels more natural with a physical book because

34:15

it's on a different page. So I

34:18

can think about it in a way that actually

34:20

takes me back to a different place in a

34:22

different time. And what we've found is that actually

34:25

there's a little burst of activity and

34:28

a pattern of activity in the hippocampus

34:30

that tells us that people are mentally

34:32

time traveling back at these

34:34

points where you can make a connection between the current

34:36

part of the story and the previous part of the

34:39

story. And so I think

34:41

that act of being able to link things together

34:43

and build them into a bigger narrative is

34:45

just mentally easier with

34:48

a physical book. Okay,

34:50

so if you feel like your brain is kind of like, oh,

34:52

I'm not remembering as much, a few things we can do there.

34:55

Don't blast your prefrontal cortex as much. Maybe

34:58

turn off the fire hose of

35:00

social media. Don't task, switch so

35:03

much. Take care of your prefrontal cortex,

35:05

sleep, reduce stress, eat right. That can help out a

35:07

lot. I thought it was really interesting you have this

35:10

chapter about the role that

35:12

imagination plays in memory.

35:15

What's the connection between the two? So

35:17

I loved writing this chapter. It was just

35:19

so much fun because it allowed

35:21

me, one of the things about writing this book that was

35:23

so much fun is I got to take

35:26

a beginner's mind and start to look at things

35:28

that I'd seen in different things that

35:30

I had read and put it together in a new way.

35:33

And so there's a very old idea going

35:35

back to a researcher named Bartlett in 1930,

35:37

where he

35:39

argued that we don't replay the past,

35:41

but we really create what

35:43

he called an imaginative construction. And

35:46

by that he means that

35:48

we don't play the past, we actually imagine

35:50

how the past could have been. Instead

35:53

of replaying it, we stage a play in our mind

35:55

about how it could have gone out. And

35:57

so we do get some details, but then we... use

36:00

imagination to fill in the blanks and

36:03

add meaning to our past. And

36:05

likewise, he suggested this and then in

36:08

neuroscience, this idea really took off about

36:10

15 years ago, that

36:13

we actually use memory to supplement imagination.

36:15

That is, when we imagine things, they're

36:18

not coming out of thin air. They're

36:21

based on this combination of all these,

36:23

you know, semantic knowledge that we have,

36:25

and then all these little episodic memories,

36:27

these random bits of experience that we've

36:30

had at different moments in our life

36:32

that allow us to anticipate and

36:34

imagine things that have never happened before.

36:36

And it's sort of the root of

36:38

creativity. So this

36:41

is an interesting question. If memory is

36:43

us just imagining how things might have

36:45

gone, how do we know if

36:47

what we're remembering actually happened,

36:50

that we're not just imagining it? This

36:52

is one of the coolest things about science.

36:54

When somebody comes up with a problem that

36:57

nobody had previously realized was a problem.

36:59

And so my old advisor, Marcia

37:01

Johnson, just came out with this

37:04

as a young researcher in the 70s. She just

37:06

said, how do we tell the difference between imagination

37:09

and things that we've actually experienced? Because

37:11

it's all in our heads. A

37:14

memory for something that happened and a memory for

37:16

something we just thought about are

37:18

both just mental experiences. And

37:21

so the way that we have to do

37:23

it is, again, surprise, you have to use

37:25

your prefrontal cortex to do a

37:27

little bit of extra detective work. And

37:29

so what that involves is saying, okay,

37:32

when I remember this thing, what are the bits

37:34

and pieces that are coming to mind? Are

37:36

they things that I can see, you

37:38

know, or are they things that I

37:40

can hear, or, you know, something that

37:42

gives me some grounding in that past

37:44

event? Or is it just stuff that

37:46

I thought about? So I don't know

37:49

about you, but like for me, I have

37:51

these issues where I ask myself, did I

37:53

send that email or did I just think

37:55

about sending the email? Did I take my

37:57

medicine today or did I just think about

37:59

it? and then get distracted. And

38:02

I have to actually ask myself, okay,

38:04

can I feel myself pushing the send

38:06

button? Can I visualize myself

38:08

or can I taste like putting the

38:10

medicine in my mouth and drinking the

38:12

water? And if so, do I bring

38:14

back a sense of today versus some

38:16

other day? And so those

38:19

kinds of sensory experiences ground us in

38:21

things that we've actually experienced in the

38:23

real world. But the

38:25

information that we think about could very

38:27

easily be imagined. And

38:30

then also, whenever we're

38:32

doing that imagining memory thing going on,

38:34

other stuff might mix in as we're

38:36

trying to recall a memory of our

38:38

childhood. There might be something that we

38:40

picked up, like we

38:43

read a book or something and we

38:45

saw a movie, and we unintentionally spliced

38:47

that into the childhood memory. And it

38:49

might turn into something that actually, that's

38:51

not how it happened. Yeah, and

38:54

often, I mean, we need this

38:56

because this less is more principle that

38:59

we're using schemas as

39:01

the scaffold for episodic memories. So we

39:03

don't have to keep rebuilding our memories

39:05

from scratch. If I went to

39:07

a cafe every Monday and met up with

39:09

a different friend, if I

39:12

formed a blank memory of that every

39:14

time, I would be

39:16

wasting enormous amounts of resources when

39:18

instead, I could just take all

39:20

my knowledge about what generally happens in cafes

39:23

and then tack on to that the specific details

39:25

of what I did this week versus what I

39:28

did last week. Now,

39:30

the problem is, is that our schemas allow

39:32

us to fill in those blanks, but sometimes

39:34

we fill them in incorrectly. And

39:36

then what's worse is when we recall those events

39:38

and we fill in the blanks incorrectly, now

39:41

that new information can creep into our

39:43

old memory because the memories get transformed

39:45

every time we recall them. And

39:48

so that's why often people's, when they tell the

39:50

story of something that happened in their childhood over

39:52

and over and over again, or your parents probably

39:54

do this, what happens

39:57

is that they get more and more of

39:59

these little errors that started. accumulating. Does

40:02

this idea explain why sometimes

40:04

people confess to crimes they

40:06

didn't commit? Yes,

40:09

because what you

40:11

can typically do in these interrogation

40:13

situations, and there's actually manuals that

40:15

there's a manual called the read

40:18

manual that talks about an interrogation

40:20

method, which relies on this, where

40:23

what they do is they ask a person

40:25

to, so first of all, you

40:27

start off with somebody who's an authority figure,

40:29

like a police person. You put

40:31

the defendant under stress,

40:34

and then you give them some misinformation, like

40:36

somebody else has ratted you out. We already

40:39

know that you did this. So

40:41

now there's a little bit of a seed of doubt

40:43

planted in the person's mind, and they're

40:45

stressed out, so they're not applying this kind of

40:47

critical thinking that the prefrontal cortex would normally let

40:49

them do. And then you ask

40:52

them, OK, well, if you don't remember it, just

40:54

imagine how it could have played out. And

40:57

so now they think about it. And if they

40:59

have a vivid imagination, they might actually

41:01

be able to come up with a very vivid

41:03

mental picture of how the crime could have played

41:05

out. And the

41:07

next day you ask them, and now they

41:09

remember something, but they don't remember what happened.

41:12

They remember what they imagined. And

41:14

so if you do this across multiple days

41:16

while a person is stressed out, sleep-proprived, in

41:19

case of it some interrogation of somebody

41:21

abroad, like what the CIA does with

41:23

their enhanced interrogation tactics, maybe they're being

41:26

tortured. And so as

41:28

a result of all this, people can develop

41:30

quite a rich, false memory for things that

41:32

never happened. And this has been simulated in

41:35

a lab by Julia Shaw and Elizabeth Loftus.

41:37

And this has been shown to happen in real life.

41:41

OK, so memory can be

41:43

squidgy because our imagination

41:46

plays a role in recalling memory. Here's

41:48

another thing I've noticed in my

41:50

life. It goes to the squidginess

41:52

of memory. Sometimes you're talking to

41:54

a friend, and you'd be talking about

41:57

when you were in high school or in college, and you

41:59

say something like. Oh yeah, I remember

42:01

you were really for the war in Iraq. And

42:04

I remember how adamant you were. And

42:06

the person, your friends, they actually know I

42:09

wasn't. I mean, I might have said some things, but

42:11

I actually wasn't. You're like, no, you seem pretty adamant

42:13

about that at the time. Do

42:16

we sometimes change our memories in

42:18

order to match how we see ourselves today? So maybe

42:20

we thought something in the past, but then our politics

42:23

has changed or beliefs have changed. But

42:26

we update the way we remember things so that it

42:28

matches how we think of ourselves today. Does

42:31

that make sense what I'm asking? Absolutely. And the

42:33

answer is yes. So our

42:36

ability to recall anything in a given moment

42:38

is based on who we are and how

42:40

we feel in our mental context at a

42:42

given moment. So just as if you hear

42:45

the right song or if you're in the

42:47

right place, you can access a memory for

42:49

a particular moment that matched up with that.

42:51

It can kind of send you back in

42:54

time. Likewise, when we're searching

42:56

for information, the goals that we

42:58

have and the beliefs that we carry with us affect

43:00

what we can pull out and what we can't. So

43:04

it can be something more unconscious.

43:06

So for instance, it could be something along the

43:08

lines of you're having a fight with your partner.

43:11

And so now all of a sudden you pull up all

43:13

these things recently that they did to piss you off. And

43:16

it's just so easy to come up with them. Then

43:18

you make up and then a week later you can't

43:20

remember what you even thought about all

43:22

those other memories that popped up. Right.

43:25

And so what changed was your mental context,

43:28

this emotion, this intense emotion that you felt.

43:31

And this also works for beliefs too.

43:33

So we have certain

43:35

beliefs and we tend to find memories

43:37

that are consistent with our beliefs. If

43:40

my belief is the past used to be

43:42

great and I was so cool

43:44

when I was in high school, then

43:46

I'll remember all these great things that happened in

43:49

high school, but I won't remember all the negative

43:51

things that happened in high school. And

43:53

then finally we view the world through

43:55

a particular perspective. And so we

43:58

can actually access other information we can do. change

44:00

this perspective. So for instance, two people

44:02

who are members of different political parties

44:05

might watch the same presidential debate and

44:08

come away with memories of completely

44:10

different experiences of who won and

44:12

who lost based on little one-liners

44:14

and so forth and the talking

44:16

points that they selectively remember. But

44:19

people can switch perspectives and say, well, what

44:22

if I was, instead of being a Republican,

44:24

what if I was a Democrat or vice

44:26

versa? They can start to pull up these

44:28

exceptions that they might have normally missed, just

44:30

like you can probably pull up information

44:33

about the positive aspects

44:35

of your relationship with your partner

44:38

when you're not fighting with them. Okay,

44:40

so that's interesting. So how do you manage that?

44:42

Are there any tips on how to make sure

44:44

you're remembering things correctly and you're

44:47

not messing things up just

44:49

so it updates and matches your current

44:51

state? Yes, I

44:53

think one factor to keep in mind

44:55

is just, first of all, how

44:57

much you're going to search for information and

45:00

memory that confirms your beliefs. So on average,

45:02

people tend to think of them recall memories

45:05

that are more positive and

45:07

that make themselves look better than they really

45:09

were. So if I

45:12

recall some experience from some

45:14

time in my life, I might actually think of

45:16

it, think of an experience

45:19

that's going to be more positive, but I'll

45:21

also remember myself in a way that's

45:23

maybe been more of a positive role

45:25

than actually transpired. So being aware

45:28

of these biases, I think, is the first step. Another

45:31

step is allowing ourselves the

45:33

time to think critically. And

45:35

again, what often happens is we're under stress,

45:38

you shut down the prefrontal cortex, you move

45:40

on to the next thing very quickly. And

45:42

it makes us very susceptible to misinformation. It

45:44

makes us very susceptible to manipulation. But

45:47

likewise, I think one thing we can do to

45:49

help ourselves is surround ourselves

45:52

by diverse Perspectives and give ourselves

45:54

a chance to remember things from

45:56

other perspectives and think that maybe

45:58

the way I. See the world Now

46:01

is just one view of how the world could

46:03

be. What's one thing that people

46:05

can start doing today to get more out

46:07

of their memory? Oh

46:10

so much. What I would

46:12

say is probably the one

46:14

thing that I would say

46:16

is be comfortable with discomfort.

46:19

And what I mean by that and I

46:21

don't think that necessarily be a madman up

46:23

it I'd over in the art of manliness.

46:25

But what I do you mean is is

46:27

that I think we often assume that memories

46:30

be effortless. Six. Is easily come

46:32

to mind and we should be able

46:34

road for memorize things easily. And you

46:36

look as a kid you get straight

46:39

A's like all that person smart. that

46:41

person's doing great and school. but really

46:43

the purses getting straight A's as.learn enough.

46:45

In theory, if you're learning, it means

46:48

that you're actually struggling, you're failing to

46:50

recall thing sometimes and then you can

46:52

get the most learning by pushing yourself.

46:55

And exposing the weaknesses in your

46:57

memory so that you can then

46:59

capture those weaknesses and six them.

47:01

Likewise, if you want to be

47:03

more creative, you need to expose

47:06

yourself to sources of memories that

47:08

are very idiosyncratic. and we're. If

47:11

you just kind of expose yourself

47:13

to gobs and gobs of the

47:15

same media whether it's reading material

47:17

or music or people who you

47:19

interact with and they're all from

47:21

the same demographic group, same culture

47:23

assemblies, you might as well be

47:25

chatty P T V that creative

47:27

or it's just states. And if

47:29

you want to be accurate and

47:31

you don't want to be remembering

47:33

things in a way that's basically

47:35

being you susceptible for me, feel

47:37

a sense you need to surround

47:39

yourself with sources. Of Information and

47:41

people who have different beliefs. again

47:43

so the you can really constantly

47:45

challenge yourself to challenge your view

47:48

of how the past transpired and

47:50

all those things can be uncomfortable

47:52

with. A can also be source

47:54

of curiosity and curiosity is a

47:56

major driver of learning and hasn't

47:58

it's effects on the brain as

48:00

we've shown in our lap. Or

48:03

sure there's been a great conversation. where to people go to

48:05

learn more about the book in your work? Well,

48:07

are you deadly? Read my book. Why

48:09

we remember am You can also go

48:12

to my website, turn around and ask.com

48:14

to get on our mailing list for

48:16

more information and you can find me

48:18

on Instagram a repost periodically. It's including

48:20

some tips about memory from time to

48:22

time and that's at the Memory Doc.

48:25

And as you will turn rogan us six much for

48:27

time, it's been a pleasure. Thanks for having me breathless

48:29

been fun! My.

48:31

A Swiss chard rocket off you. The Author: The

48:33

book Why we remember to be of on amazon.com

48:35

A book stores everywhere. You. Find more face

48:38

by his work at his website for a rug

48:40

and us.com also taken a send Us day when

48:42

the I as last memory and finding some resources

48:44

and delve deeper in this topic. Well

48:53

that wraps up another just to be a when

48:55

podcast makes your check it or what's that it

48:57

out of balance.com refiner, podcast or guys and while

48:59

you're there Senate for newsletters get the Elite. Eighty

49:01

three options are both free the best way. Still

49:04

top of what's going on a whim and vivid

49:06

and thirty at appreciated. Take one minute to his

49:08

view, not the podcast a spot. a final set

49:10

a lot and done that already Think you please

49:12

consider say the show the friend or family members

49:14

you figure something out of it as always save

49:17

the confused important to like size Brett Mckay we

49:19

might I was the on pa just put put

49:21

put you've heard into action. Time.

49:37

for a quick break to talk about

49:39

mcdonalds mornings of for mixing and matching

49:42

at mcdonalds for just three dollars mix

49:44

and match to have your favorite breakfast

49:46

items including a sausage mcmuffin sauces biscuit

49:48

sauces burrito in hash browns make it

49:50

even better with a delicious medium iced

49:52

coffee with mcdonalds mix and match he

49:54

can't go wrong price and participation may

49:56

vary cannot be combined with any other

49:58

offer a combo me Single item

50:00

at regular price. Sometimes.

50:03

It takes a different approach. To help

50:05

you unlock your true potential. With.

50:07

Capella University's game changing flex path

50:10

learning format. You. Gain relevant skills

50:12

you can apply to your career right away.

50:14

On your degree from an accredited

50:16

university. Imagine

50:20

your future differently at capella.edu. Capella

50:24

University is accredited by the Higher

50:26

Learning Commission. Learn more at capella.edu.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features