Podchaser Logo
Home
Talk Evidence - Diabetes data, colonoscopies, and researchers behaving badly

Talk Evidence - Diabetes data, colonoscopies, and researchers behaving badly

Released Wednesday, 2nd November 2022
Good episode? Give it some love!
Talk Evidence - Diabetes data, colonoscopies, and researchers behaving badly

Talk Evidence - Diabetes data, colonoscopies, and researchers behaving badly

Talk Evidence - Diabetes data, colonoscopies, and researchers behaving badly

Talk Evidence - Diabetes data, colonoscopies, and researchers behaving badly

Wednesday, 2nd November 2022
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:10

Hello, and welcome back to

0:12

Talk Evidence. We've got Nick

0:14

writing program for you this week, we start with some

0:16

new data on colorectal cancer screening

0:19

using colonoscopy.

0:21

We then turn very excitingly to

0:23

listener request. You've been itching

0:26

for someone to send in their evidence

0:28

request. And finally, we have

0:30

one, so you can hear more about

0:32

that. And in fact, it tends to a bit of feature

0:34

then. This this episode

0:37

feels very dedicated to diabetes

0:39

and and of prediabetes, and

0:41

there's not a bit of COVID insight. And

0:43

we're going to finish off with a little bit of news

0:46

about clinical academics behaving

0:48

badly.

0:50

I'm

0:50

Helen McDonald's, research integrity editor

0:53

for the BMJ and BMJ journals, and

0:55

I'm joined today by Joan.

0:57

Hi, Joan. Do you want to introduce yourself?

0:59

Hi, everyone. This is Joe Ross. I'm

1:01

a professor of medicine in public health

1:03

at Yale and also an associate editor at the

1:05

BMJ.

1:06

I sounded very timid to that, Joe. And

1:08

I'm also joined by Juan. Juan,

1:11

do you guys introduce yourself?

1:12

Hi, everybody. I'm Juan Franco.

1:14

I'm a firm position. I'm

1:16

currently a researcher at the Heineken

1:19

University in Dusseldorf Germany, and

1:21

I'm the Attorney Chief of BMC Evidence

1:23

by medicine.

1:26

In short, a very important people.

1:34

Right. Let's get on to our first

1:36

item for today, which is colorectal cancer

1:39

screening and Juan, you

1:41

volunteered to take a closer look at this one.

1:43

This is a new study that came out in the New

1:45

England Journal of Medicine. A

1:47

few days. Okay?

1:49

Wait

1:49

a second, Helen, isn't this the B and J

1:51

Talk Evidence podcast? Why are we talking about

1:53

a paper in the England Journal of Medicine?

1:55

I'm gonna see if we can bring it back to

1:57

the MJ at the end. But we're we're open to

1:59

all evidence here. It's

2:02

evidence motivations not journals.

2:04

So Juan, tell us about this paper.

2:06

Well, I think it's it

2:08

was very popular in the

2:10

news this past

2:13

days because there is

2:15

one of the first randomized

2:18

control trials that put colonoscopy

2:21

in the under

2:23

scrutiny. And It

2:25

was a a large trial between

2:27

two thousand and nine and twenty fourteen in Poland,

2:30

Norway, Sweden, and Netherlands with

2:34

follow-up data from more than

2:36

eighty four thousand participants. And

2:41

and pay participants were randomized

2:43

to be invited to

2:45

undergo a single screening colonoscopy

2:48

or no invitation to screening.

2:51

So the

2:52

And that will be an important point later, why

2:54

don't you plan? because they were invited to

2:57

come. You didn't hear anyone's gentle

2:59

gentle voice, but I will stress that. So it's not

3:01

necessarily that they actually came. They were invited

3:04

to come. Carry on.

3:04

No one had to describe my voice as gentle

3:07

before, but thank you. I

3:11

yes. And the

3:14

the invitation part is is

3:16

crucial not only because I couldn't

3:18

imagine a trial of people being coerced to

3:20

having colonoscopy, but also

3:22

by the fact that only forty two percent

3:24

of those invited

3:27

under with screening. So very

3:30

little well, like,

3:33

I wouldn't want to put it as subjective

3:37

tone to it, but let's say it's not

3:39

an optimal uptake for

3:41

something that you're randomizing people into.

3:43

And during

3:46

the medium follow-up of ten years, Basically,

3:49

in the main analysis, the

3:54

risk of colorectal cancer

3:56

was point ninety eight

3:58

percent in the invited group and point one

4:00

two in the usual care group. So

4:02

there are say some absolute

4:05

risk reduction and the confidence interval

4:08

is between zero point seven and

4:10

zero point eight three. and

4:12

– but the risk of death

4:14

from colorectal cancer was

4:18

zero point two eight in the divided group

4:20

and zero point thirty one in the

4:22

usual care group and the confidence

4:24

interval for the risk ratio is from

4:26

zero point sixty four to one point

4:28

sixteen. And

4:31

so this is the main analysis.

4:33

And the interpretation is basically

4:35

that And

4:37

it's as consistent with these

4:39

types of screening that identify

4:43

precursors of the disease, which

4:45

is also common in cervical cancer,

4:48

so you remove the polyps, and

4:50

you therefore, you estimate that the

4:52

risk of the disease will be lower.

4:55

and this is what we see in one of the

4:57

outcomes. But unfortunately, we

4:59

can't really identify a

5:02

change in colorectal cancer

5:04

mortality. Most

5:07

of this could be explained by the

5:09

low uptake would reduce the

5:11

power of the study to

5:14

detect this

5:16

difference. And as a matter of fact,

5:18

the authors go into

5:21

an adjusted a per protocol analysis,

5:23

trying to see those people

5:26

who would have undergone screening

5:29

And in that case, they

5:31

identify a difference

5:34

in colorectal cancer mortality. with

5:38

zero point one five percent in the invited group and

5:40

zero point three percent in the usual

5:42

care group, which is an - of

5:44

an estimate of half of

5:46

did that form

5:48

colorectal cancer? So

5:50

you're trying to tease out the does

5:52

the screening program work and what the

5:54

effects are versus if you actually turn up

5:56

for this screening, does

5:58

it does it work then?

5:59

the

6:00

Joe, I can see what it to say something.

6:02

Yeah. I mean, Juan, it was great that you

6:05

brought this to our podcast discussion

6:07

this week because it really was a

6:09

heated discussion throughout Twitter and

6:11

other social media forums. There's a bit of a,

6:13

you know, a roshock test

6:15

of of sentiment. It has it gone down,

6:17

Jay. Well, at that's what I mean. Like, it was

6:19

a bit of a Wireshark in the sense that, like,

6:21

people saw in it what they wanted to see in it. There

6:23

were the people who, you know, saw

6:25

the you know, the low participation

6:27

rate and that means that

6:29

screening programs, you know, aren't likely to

6:31

work. You know, we knew that, you know, we're probably

6:34

there's too much over diagnosis, too much

6:36

testing and treatment. This is just

6:38

one more kind of nail on the coffin for screening

6:40

programs. Others people were like, hey,

6:42

now, hey, now, like, it's more complicated than that.

6:44

And, you know, when you do do the intention

6:47

to treat or, sorry, the

6:49

per protocol analysis focused on the

6:51

people who actually underwent

6:53

screening. Right? It actually it's

6:55

it's effective at the individual level.

6:58

And to me, this raises what I think is an interesting

7:00

question. So My understanding is that in

7:02

Europe, for the most part, people don't

7:04

do colonoscopy unless they have

7:06

some symptoms of disease. But in the United

7:08

States, the sort of,

7:10

like, the ground level for colorectal

7:12

cancer screening is colonoscopy. And

7:14

I wonder if the participation rates would

7:16

have been much higher in a population of

7:19

people who are accustomed or like

7:21

prepared at age fifty or now at age

7:23

forty five, I'm supposed to undergo a

7:25

colonoscopy. And if we had seen participation

7:27

rates in the eighty percent as opposed to

7:29

the forty percent, might the results of

7:31

the trial look different?

7:33

Well, that's very interesting. And I did say that

7:36

I was going to bring it back to the BMJ didn't

7:38

I? Because a while ago, a couple of

7:40

years ago, we published

7:42

a paper in a rapid recommendation

7:45

series that looked at screening

7:49

options for colorectal cancer. And

7:51

Joe, you went back to look at that

7:53

paper to see

7:55

how to see how this

7:58

latest study might impact on those recommendations.

7:59

Yeah.

8:01

Well, so first I would just say for listeners,

8:03

you know, we'll put a a link in the show notes

8:05

to the rapid recommendations page

8:10

in the BMJ And I would encourage people to go

8:12

and look at it because the team at

8:14

the you know, at the journal do a really

8:16

terrific job of creating infographics that

8:18

people can engage with in order to look

8:20

through and sort of identify, you know, what

8:22

pathway to go down. And given the time and

8:24

effort they put into it, more people should use it.

8:27

But essentially, what you see,

8:29

right, is you know, that

8:31

when the fifteen year risk of colorectal

8:34

cancer for an individual is

8:36

below three percent there's,

8:38

you know, weak evidence

8:41

that favors no screening. But

8:43

when you when there that fifteen

8:45

year risk is above three percent there's a weak evidence

8:47

that favors screening. And of course,

8:49

that three percent risk is

8:52

estimated using the acute cancer calculator

8:54

that's also right on the site for

8:56

people to look at. Now, how

8:58

is this trial going to change it?

9:00

Well, it will probably move it

9:02

even further away in the sense

9:04

that it

9:05

one will

9:06

disfavor

9:07

colonoscopy and screening

9:10

even a little bit more.

9:12

But there are more trials

9:14

coming and I hope that

9:16

the next few years will have a much better

9:18

sense of the role of colonoscopy

9:21

in colorectal cancer screening. I don't know, Helen,

9:23

if you to talk about Well,

9:24

what I can say is that

9:26

the that sounds like a politician doesn't

9:28

know. What I can say is

9:30

that the team that produce the

9:33

rapid recommendations are going

9:35

to have a look at that

9:37

study in more detail and add

9:40

an update to

9:42

describe the the latest

9:44

evidence and to give some preliminary

9:46

indication of how things might change. But I think

9:48

they might hold on a bit longer for

9:50

those other studies that you mentioned, Joe, to

9:52

do a fuller update of

9:54

that guidance in light of that

9:56

new evidence there.

10:06

Now, let's turn diabetes or prediabetes,

10:08

I should say, and our very exciting

10:10

listener request. So

10:13

here in the KB NHS

10:15

spends about ten billion a year

10:17

on diabetes, apparently. I love this reading around,

10:19

and that's about ten percent of the entire

10:21

budget. And

10:24

our query relates to prediabetes.

10:26

And here it is. It's from

10:29

a UK GP who happens to

10:31

work in Bath, which is actually where I'm

10:33

based. So here we are.

10:37

Hi.

10:37

My name is Nicole House, and I'm a GP

10:39

in Bath. I would like more information

10:41

on the evidence for the National Diabetes

10:44

Prevention Program to discuss with my

10:46

patients living with obesity. We've

10:48

had local education Sally Day

10:50

where the team presented their

10:52

program but with no clinical outcome

10:54

data. Can the talk

10:56

evidence team tell me what is

10:58

the evidence these programs prevent,

11:01

reduce or delay the onset

11:03

of diabetes. Many

11:05

thanks.

11:09

So that was Nicole's message. For

11:12

those people who are not listening in

11:15

the UK or in England, this

11:17

is a program

11:19

that's offered through the NHS.

11:22

It's a mixture of some

11:24

coaching

11:25

information. It's delivered digitally.

11:27

It's centered around education.

11:30

An educational intervention and to try

11:32

and prevent or delay people from developing

11:34

type two diabetes, and it focuses on

11:36

healthy eating, lifestyle,

11:38

help to lose weight and physical

11:41

exercise programs. And the

11:43

program both says that these have been

11:45

proven to reduce the risk of

11:47

develop up in type two diabetes. And

11:50

so what I've done when I

11:52

gave Joe and Juan the homework

11:54

on this is thought of kind

11:56

of two questions One is

11:58

what is the evidence

11:59

that programs a

12:00

bit like this work, so sort of

12:03

lifestyle and event to prevent

12:05

people from going from having

12:07

pre diabetes to diabetes. And then

12:09

we'll come back to this specific one.

12:12

that's run-in England. So

12:14

Juan, with your connections

12:16

to Cochrane and systematic reviews,

12:18

I thought you would be the perfect person

12:20

to tellers first, where the program's a

12:22

bit like this work?

12:25

Well, yes, there's

12:27

a Cocon review from

12:29

twenty seventeen

12:31

about diet, physical

12:34

activities are a combination

12:36

of both in different

12:38

programs to prevent or delayed

12:40

type two diabetes. They

12:43

included different

12:45

studies. And especially, it's it's

12:47

it's important to have, like, a different time

12:49

points with different different credentials

12:52

of diabetes. But the

12:54

main gross evidence

12:56

from eleven trials highlight

12:59

that this these

13:01

programs may reduce slightly

13:03

some of the risk of

13:05

diabetes. As particularly

13:07

in numbers, there's moderate

13:09

sense, you have evidence of every

13:13

for a thousand people, that

13:15

do not go out this program with

13:19

a two hundred and fifty seven will

13:21

get diabetes. And with

13:23

the program, one

13:24

hundred and forty six. So

13:28

a reduction that, of course, has

13:31

a pretty high confidence interval.

13:34

And what kind of time

13:34

period is that over one? Is that sort of

13:37

The

13:37

median the mean duration follow-up is

13:40

three point eight years. Okay. So

13:42

we're looking more at this sort of delaying

13:44

rather than Life

13:45

time prevention of

13:47

disease. It's difficult because most of

13:50

this study is not had

13:52

very long follow-up time.

13:54

And most of all, most

13:57

of this study is did

13:58

not

13:59

provide enough data

14:02

for other type of

14:04

important outcomes. For example, complications

14:06

related to diabetes or health quality

14:08

of life. or

14:11

possibly adverse events, which

14:13

one would expect that there would

14:15

be minor But for example, even if you take

14:17

cardiovascular mortality, that

14:20

seven studies did report on cardiovascular

14:22

mortality, the evidence

14:25

was too uncertain to

14:29

to to to figure out whether there's a

14:31

difference or not between those

14:33

receiving receiving the

14:35

program or not. So, basically,

14:39

one of the questions perhaps,

14:42

I see, I have as a

14:44

clinician thinking about this, is

14:46

how much of these programs are trying

14:48

to prevent like the chemical

14:50

diagnosis of diabetes or how much

14:52

are we preventing the actual morbidity

14:54

that is related to

14:56

diabetes and the burden of the disease?

14:58

So

15:00

yeah.

15:01

I I would I I

15:04

think that there is some is

15:06

that is positive one.

15:09

But I I mean more.

15:10

Well, it's it's really interesting. I mean, I

15:13

think much of these programs that

15:15

while, you know, that cochrane review

15:17

summarizes a wide breadth of

15:19

evidence, but the seminal

15:22

study that was actually

15:24

published a long time ago in the the Journal, I

15:26

think two thousand two, two

15:28

thousand three, something like that.

15:30

You know, that was the first three year follow-up,

15:32

you know, in the diabetes prevention program.

15:34

I identifying folks at high risk, those with

15:36

pre diabetes, you know, as

15:38

assessed by elevated

15:40

fasting and post load plasma

15:43

glucose concentrations. So they were randomized

15:45

to placebo metformin

15:47

or lifestyle interventions. And the people who were

15:49

randomized to lifestyle interventions did better

15:51

even than those who started taking metformin

15:54

in terms of the incidence

15:56

of diabetes and with weight loss.

15:59

that Lancet published their ten year outcome rates

16:01

in the lower incidence

16:04

of diabetes persisted. But

16:06

again, they don't look at cardiovascular

16:09

outcomes like WAM Notes. But it it

16:11

was those studies that actually

16:13

led in the United States

16:15

a big program evaluation led

16:18

by Medicare, the Centers for Medicare

16:20

Medicaid Services. So Medicare did this big pilot

16:22

demonstration project essentially

16:24

say, well, everyone, you know, should

16:26

have access to lifestyle

16:28

interventions as part of, you know,

16:30

diabetes prevention. Among those, quote

16:32

unquote, with prediabetes. Right?

16:34

And that's actually been a large

16:37

rollout that I think is now

16:39

mirrored in terms of what's happening in

16:41

the UK. And the

16:43

evaluation of that effort

16:45

is gonna be like what happened with the

16:47

colonoscopy trial we just discussed. So

16:49

they're gonna offer lifestyle, interventions,

16:52

and or metformin. I'm not sure if

16:54

that's even part of the program. And then it'll

16:56

depend on participation rates to know if

16:58

it actually works at the population

17:00

level as opposed to an randomized trial.

17:02

And I

17:02

did find a couple of papers, which will

17:04

put in the notes of the episode,

17:07

which were largely service evaluations

17:09

and people's experiences of having

17:11

been in the program. So I think

17:13

the answer to the precise answer in

17:16

Nicole's question is I don't

17:18

think we entirely know

17:20

the outcomes for this

17:23

program or necessarily the

17:26

answers about for how

17:28

long it might delay diabetes

17:31

or ultimately whether it fully prevents it. But I

17:33

think Joe and

17:35

Juan, I feel

17:35

I feel persuaded that

17:38

there is at least some evidence

17:40

of these programs having an

17:44

effect,

17:45

certainly feels like they're unlikely to

17:48

do harm when targeted at people

17:50

who

17:51

have pre diabetes.

17:53

And and, also, in general, right, they're

17:55

focused on lifestyle modifications

17:58

-- Yes. -- which are general -- I'm saying

17:59

wow. -- not just decided,

18:02

but for lots of lots of

18:04

reasons. So, yes, I think, you know,

18:06

feel pretty good about that. Well, we can

18:08

look forward to Nicole's feedback maybe.

18:11

on whether we have

18:13

helped her remotely have a conversation

18:15

with her patients. You

18:16

know, perhaps I would add

18:19

is how this affect the whole system

18:21

in the sense.

18:22

When you shift the

18:25

resources and time and

18:27

efforts into prevention, I would have

18:29

to be absolutely sure that the

18:31

people with the condition in this case with

18:33

diabetes are receiving the

18:35

most option care because if and

18:37

so thinking about the whole system,

18:39

if you if you're a policy communicator

18:41

have to allocate money, where

18:44

would you put it in this case?

18:46

So there's some

18:48

let's look a bit of

18:50

an equity situation. Right? So

18:52

it depends on the system. Since I'm not in the NHS,

18:54

I cannot say that.

18:56

So but I I would argue that

18:58

in other systems, which people with

19:00

with diabetes are struggling to

19:03

get their medication or

19:05

or have a strong pathway

19:08

of care, allocating

19:10

resources to the prevention of the

19:12

condition may seem slightly

19:15

You are

19:15

really confusing everyone's CPT

19:18

now, hon.

19:27

So

19:27

coming up in December, we've got BMW

19:29

Research Forum at BMA House. And

19:31

joining me is dot to Helen Serrano from the events team.

19:33

Hi, Helen. Hi, Helen. Helen's

19:38

meet. So tell us a bit about this

19:40

event. You're speaking. So

19:42

the event is to bring together

19:44

the research community. We

19:47

recognize that there's loads of people

19:49

involved in research, not just

19:51

the people who submit to us and the people

19:53

who edit the journals here at B and J.

19:56

But there's funders, there's policymakers,

19:59

patient experts,

19:59

But of course, the researchers are the main part

20:02

of that. And we're just having this

20:04

event alongside the B and J

20:06

editors meeting to make

20:08

sure all those people are talking with each other

20:10

about the same things rather than

20:13

just to their own groups. Some of the

20:15

speakers we've got are

20:17

from some of the big funding organizations.

20:19

We've got Ken Gabriel

20:21

from Welcome Leap, who

20:24

is talking about how they fund for

20:26

impact by using big amounts

20:29

of venture capital money

20:31

alongside investment from organizations like

20:33

welcome. We've got

20:35

Chris Witty, the Chief Medical Officer, talking

20:37

about how research happens in

20:39

the UK and how governments involved

20:42

in it. And we've got a

20:44

really interesting panel

20:46

from

20:46

from

20:47

researchers and funders and

20:50

patients talking about Parkinson's disease

20:52

research and how that is becoming

20:54

a real spectrum and

20:56

a virtuous circle of patients

20:59

getting involved with research

21:01

questions and funders looking

21:03

at impact as well as

21:05

basic science research and further

21:09

clinical

21:09

research as well. So there's

21:11

loads, there's something for everyone

21:13

involved in research, and

21:15

there's some detailed workshops

21:16

and sessions along

21:19

different lines for different groups

21:21

as well.

21:23

Well, I will certainly be there and you've got

21:25

a special offer I understand for

21:28

our loyal listeners of

21:31

Talk Evidence we have. Well, we know that

21:33

the talk evidence community

21:35

are very interested

21:37

in how research gets done. So

21:39

we really hope that some of you will be able to

21:41

join us. And we've got a twenty

21:43

percent discount code, which

21:45

you can use when you

21:48

register at the website. The website

21:50

is BMJ research forum dot BMJ

21:52

dot com. That's all one word. And the discount

21:55

code is BMJRF

21:58

twenty two. So

21:59

that's BMJ research forum is the thing

22:02

you need to remember. And from that, you'll be able to

22:04

remember the website and the

22:06

discount code. so we really hope to see loads of your listeners

22:08

there. Well, thanks for

22:09

dropping and telling us all about it, Helen. Brilliant.

22:11

Thanks for having

22:12

me.

22:18

We are going to stay with

22:20

diabetes for

22:21

our next item. and

22:24

Joe, we discovered that you are a

22:26

co author of a paper that

22:28

has recently been published

22:30

and with your big

22:33

data obsession, you

22:35

have tried to emulate

22:37

a trial looking at the effectiveness

22:39

of adding different

22:42

second line drugs to

22:45

metformin, which is often use first line

22:47

for type two diabetes

22:49

because that's not enough for

22:52

many people. Tell us about your paper and

22:54

what you did.

22:56

Yeah. So

22:56

and thanks for giving the chance to talk a

22:58

little bit about it. So among

23:01

the many hats I wear,

23:03

one is I lead a research

23:06

group at Yale that

23:08

jointly works with folks at the Mayo

23:10

Clinic to collaborate with the FDA

23:12

on regulatory science research projects

23:14

that are a priority for the agency

23:16

And as you both are

23:19

aware, there's a

23:20

the

23:22

deep interest in

23:25

leveraging real world data sources

23:27

to try to better understand product,

23:29

medical product, safety and

23:32

effectiveness, particularly once products are approved

23:34

for use. And so how can

23:36

we, you know, with

23:38

the better data that are available to

23:41

us today, you know, advance our thinking and

23:43

and generate better evidence. So this

23:45

was actually part of a project

23:47

that we collaborated with with

23:49

folks in office of

23:51

medical policy to try to

23:53

understand, you know, essentially, lots

23:55

of people have looked back and said,

23:57

alright, these clinical trials have been done. Could

23:59

we have gotten the same results if we

24:01

had used observational data.

24:03

Right? And and confirmed essentially,

24:05

you know, that when you apply the same inclusion

24:07

and exclusion criteria, you you get

24:10

basically the same findings for the most

24:12

part. Right? This and that's been shown over

24:14

and over John Ionidas did a great

24:16

systematic review of all of the studies that have done

24:18

that in they they they pretty much match up, but

24:20

there's always this interesting risk of

24:22

bias when you're looking backwards as opposed to

24:24

forwards. And so Working

24:26

with the FDA, we actually identified two

24:29

ongoing clinical trials

24:31

that were kind of relevant to

24:33

the agency where we asked,

24:35

well, could we emulate

24:37

that trial with observational data

24:39

sources? And we wondered whether our

24:41

results would look similar.

24:43

And so we did one trial looking at cardiovascular

24:45

safety outcomes for a

24:47

prostate cancer treatment. And then we did

24:49

this trial, which emulated

24:52

what was considered to be kind

24:55

of the landmark grade

24:57

study. This was a study that

24:59

was launched way back in

25:01

twenty thirteen that asked the question,

25:03

okay, you have a patient with

25:05

diabetes. They're already prescribed

25:07

metformin. What do you give

25:09

them as a second line

25:11

agent? and they tested

25:13

four different medications

25:17

representing four different class

25:19

classes. I think tell

25:20

us the classes, Joe, because I feel like

25:22

the drugs vary in the different places, but

25:24

tell us the classes.

25:25

Yeah. So the

25:27

a short acting insulin or sorry.

25:29

A a

25:32

m they

25:35

tested insulin, insulin glargine,

25:37

the synthetic insulin, they tested

25:39

a sulfonylurea, in this case, glumab

25:41

glumab peride. They tested

25:45

GLP-one receptor, liraglutide,

25:47

and they tested

25:49

a DPP-four cidaglyptan. Now

25:53

you guys

25:54

see patients all the time.

25:56

What's missing? I tell you from that.

25:59

No.

25:59

Well, Juan, I'm

26:02

sure you quickly realize they're

26:04

missing the -- The t's. -- the new this

26:07

sGLT2 inhibitors, which are like the

26:09

actual diabetes medication

26:11

that now is often --

26:13

most often used for second line treatment

26:15

because it's -- there's been good evidence that

26:18

demonstrates not only does it work for glucose

26:20

lowering for patients with diabetes, but it prevents

26:22

cardiovascular risk, heart failure risk, and other renal

26:24

outcomes. And so you know,

26:26

this is the madness of clinical

26:28

trial design. Right? Way back, you know,

26:30

over ten years ago, they thought they

26:32

identified the four best sort of second

26:34

line agents and it

26:36

turns out they were wrong. Right? And

26:38

they missed one. So

26:40

what we did is we we those

26:43

results were actually published

26:45

very recently. But before those results were

26:47

published, we – our study

26:49

was published, which looked

26:51

at testing these these

26:53

four these four different medication classes using,

26:56

you know, essentially health insurance

26:59

claims data that were linked

27:01

to electronic health

27:03

work data. So we could use the lab

27:05

values to identify the

27:08

sort of main outcome, which

27:10

is of the time to a hemoglobin A1C

27:12

greater than seven seven

27:14

percent. Oh, no. You want to jame down?

27:18

Right.

27:18

right So you want

27:21

the

27:21

- essentially, you start with

27:23

a patient. It's very complicated the

27:25

way they designed it, but they essentially said

27:27

they looked for failures of treatment. Okay.

27:30

Yeah. It it so we've tried to

27:32

emulate exactly We did wanna make this easy

27:34

for anyone to understand. You wanted

27:37

to tell bus drug

27:39

that was most least

27:41

likely to cause you to fail. Is that

27:43

is that right? Right. because because you

27:45

want it because essentially what they tested

27:47

is which drug or drug class was

27:49

most effective at maintaining a

27:52

controlled hemoglobin -- Mhmm. -- agency level. Okay.

27:54

It's making a good service now. Yeah.

27:56

Yeah. But it's all everything is complicated

27:58

and and all you know, everything that we did

28:00

was also complicated. I'll try to just keep

28:02

it simple by focusing it on

28:04

two points. the first of which

28:06

was we could not

28:08

actually test the insulin glargine

28:10

arm because when you're doing this

28:12

using observational data sources, your

28:14

data reflect real world practice. And

28:16

as it turns out, no one

28:18

was prescribing insulin engardene as a second

28:20

line agent. Everyone basically got

28:23

some other, you

28:25

know, oral therapy, you know,

28:27

one of these other therapies So we just

28:29

didn't even have enough patients even with

28:31

a massive data set

28:33

to emulate that arm of the

28:36

trial. The

28:36

second thing is, in

28:38

the real world, people don't have their

28:40

A1C measured every three months like

28:42

they do in a trial. So that was

28:45

super complicated to operationalize in

28:47

terms of like, how many A1C

28:49

measures did you have to have in order for

28:51

us to look at you? And is that biasing

28:53

the sample? and all sorts of stuff like that.

28:55

But essentially, we what

28:57

we found was that liraglutide was more

29:00

effective at maintaining glycemic

29:02

control than both glimepyride or sotaglyptin.

29:04

And now we're actually in

29:06

the process of comparing our results to

29:08

the actual trial results, which just came

29:11

out. And we're actually

29:13

gonna extend our emulation to include

29:15

the SGLT2 inhibitors because if we

29:17

feel like, you know, what we did match

29:20

the actual grade study, then we can feel pretty confident in what we

29:22

find with the SGLT2 inhibitors. And

29:24

we hope that this evidence is going to be

29:26

useful. But at the end of the day,

29:28

This is these types of exercises I think

29:31

are kind of what's going to help

29:33

FDA and other regulators in

29:35

terms of like how it use

29:37

these types of data, these types

29:39

of evidence to complement, you know, what we

29:41

get from the clinical trials.

29:43

Do you

29:43

think you can ever use them if you don't

29:46

have clinical trials? because it feels a

29:48

bit like almost you

29:50

need quite

29:52

a good idea of what

29:54

you're gonna do in the trial to

29:57

design this

29:59

the It

30:00

is very complicated because

30:03

how, for instance, do you

30:05

emulate a placebo arm

30:07

right, people who have the disease but don't

30:09

get treated. Like, there's there is

30:12

bias happening there among the, you know, in

30:14

terms of using that as a

30:16

population, as control arm. That's

30:18

very hard to do. If you're comparing

30:20

two active treatments, it's a little

30:22

bit easier because you can do kind of a new incident

30:25

user study with big data

30:27

sets. Right? But it's

30:28

very difficult to just say,

30:31

these these types of data analysis

30:33

are gonna replace a clinical trial. It's

30:35

very hard to emulate

30:38

that opportunity to randomize a

30:40

person and truly get

30:42

a more unbiased the the most

30:44

unbiased estimate of the treatment

30:46

effect. So I am very reluctant

30:48

to think that we'll get there. But

30:50

if you have, you know, datasets

30:52

from nine different countries representing

30:54

nine different populations and

30:57

you examine it and

30:59

pretty consistently find that drug

31:01

a were better than drug b, I think pretty close to the

31:03

truth. We may not have the exact estimate of the

31:05

fact that we get from a clinical trial. So you would

31:07

see kind of the dream that you have the

31:08

trials and then

31:10

these big data things. They can confirm

31:13

things. They can maybe look

31:15

at

31:15

some subgroups.

31:18

Some populations who are representative?

31:20

Yeah. Divested population. So all the

31:22

people all the people. Exactly.

31:25

Uh-huh. I see how you're working here. And

31:27

of course, safety. Right? because clinical

31:29

trials are generally too

31:31

small to get real signals of safety if

31:33

they exist. So -- So how do you

31:35

think? -- the FDA decided what

31:37

they they favor? Oh, as if

31:39

they would say it bold and outright and be

31:42

very clear in their intentions, that's

31:44

not how federal agencies is worth And

31:47

what does what professor

31:49

Ross, what are you pro prescribing in

31:51

your colleague, Alan? Oh, I

31:53

generally You know, I

31:55

I think that the SGLT2 inhibitors,

31:58

not because of the work that we're done, but

31:59

because of the basis of the clinical trial evidence

32:02

tends to have the most rigorous

32:04

evidence behind it in terms of the

32:06

benefit to patients. So I think

32:09

Those are the ones I would prescribe as second

32:11

line. I don't know one, if you agree.

32:13

But to me, there's there's more

32:15

trial evidence that supports their

32:17

Juan, he's bringing you in hand. Do you agree? Yeah.

32:19

I mean, the the

32:22

SGL to

32:24

inhibitors were they

32:28

they came later in my

32:30

practice in that's

32:32

a few years. And for for the country,

32:34

it's working. It was very

32:37

difficult to to have them as second

32:39

line. and when the national guidance in Argentina

32:41

were being updated. When

32:45

I have years go. Basically, there was

32:47

a big push by the societies to put

32:50

in the second line, but it

32:52

was it was not feasible,

32:54

basically, because of the cost. So

32:57

But, yes, they they do come

32:59

as a revolution like one

33:01

of the people compared to

33:03

to statin. rights and further

33:05

effects that go beyond

33:07

the the change in glucose and

33:10

and

33:10

the possible effects of cardiovascular

33:13

fidelity and and they're very

33:15

promising. Perhaps what one question that I

33:17

had was so you said

33:19

that you back now to

33:21

extrapolate, to follow the same

33:23

analysis and include

33:27

the SGLT two

33:31

inhibitors, would that make so

33:35

but that would that would

33:37

be the extrapolation, right, not without the

33:39

comparing it to trial data?

33:41

Right. We would essentially repeat

33:44

the emulation -- Mhmm. -- including an arm

33:46

for people who took an SGLT2 inhibitor.

33:48

And I just I don't want to be

33:50

remiss the, you know, this work, which

33:52

I'm up part of is being

33:54

led by Rosalina McCoy

33:56

at the Mayo Clinic, an incredible

33:58

endocrinologist and clinical researcher

33:59

along with Yi Hung Dan. So there and

34:02

there's a big team of other people. So I'm just wondering Sorry. We

34:04

won't give you too much credit. Joe, you

34:06

can really borrow from me now anyway. But

34:08

while done

34:11

all those people.

34:18

So to our

34:20

final item, chemical

34:23

academics behaving badly, I'm gonna be I'm

34:25

gonna do something which

34:27

I really do, which is I'm gonna be

34:30

silent and see see

34:32

what Joe and Juan wants to

34:34

say because that this is think

34:36

you guys are gonna ask me some questions.

34:40

So

34:41

though I

34:44

started following this this

34:46

debate on online because I

34:48

saw this editorial of the

34:50

British Journal sports medicine about the

34:53

retraction of many of the

34:55

early trials and was a little

34:56

bit confused at first because I

34:59

started seeing all pop up of subtraction,

35:02

subtraction, mass massively the

35:04

journal. And In ABMJ journal,

35:06

no less. In ABMJ

35:08

journal. And of

35:11

course, I I was

35:13

immediately intrigued. Calvin, could you can you

35:15

tell us a little bit Well,

35:18

I'm not sure how much is the backstory, Matt,

35:20

that is all out of it. keep it

35:24

brief. Yeah. Helen, tell us what you've been doing for the past two

35:26

years. He's not as long as she

35:28

is. So a few

35:30

months back, we did a

35:31

track piece.

35:33

It

35:33

was a comedy piece written by a

35:36

clinical academic called Dr.

35:37

Paul Macquarie. He is

35:39

a sports

35:39

medicine doctor House

35:42

in Australia. And he was also the in chief the Journal

35:44

of Sports Medicine,

35:44

BTSM, which is published by

35:47

The Journal. And

35:48

the

35:49

we retracted that 30s piece

35:51

because he plagiarized some content from the early

35:53

thousands. And soon after we

35:55

retracted that piece, we

35:57

were alerted to

35:59

some

35:59

other potentially

36:01

problematic pieces, which might

36:03

contain plagiarism.

36:04

There was

36:06

an instance of misrepresentation

36:09

of someone else's

36:11

quotation in

36:11

Macquarie's work

36:13

and some

36:15

examples of potentially duplicate publications, self

36:20

plagiarism. And so for

36:21

the last few months, once strand

36:23

of my job has been working with the

36:25

current editor in chief of BGSM,

36:27

and

36:27

he and my colleague

36:29

in

36:29

research integrity PMJ have been looking into the allegation.

36:31

We've now provided an update on our

36:34

investigation and our

36:36

next steps.

36:37

So the news

36:39

is is so the

36:41

news

36:41

is we've retracted nine more

36:43

papers on which Macquarie was

36:45

a soul or single office, and

36:48

he was just writing these pieces alone. They were all commentary style pieces,

36:50

so not academic research

36:52

pieces.

36:55

five

36:56

contained plagiarism,

36:58

so similar to

36:59

the first example. Three were

37:01

duplicates or redundant publications where

37:03

he'd been inappropriately reusing

37:06

chunks of his own work

37:08

without reasonable attribution.

37:11

And in one

37:12

case, there was this instance where he

37:15

misquoted the view of another clinician

37:17

from nineteen fifty. And this

37:18

was a US doctor called

37:21

Thondike. He was seeing patients, he had

37:23

sports injuries at a university

37:25

and and the full driver's recording

37:27

and

37:28

making recommendations based

37:30

on his

37:31

experience observation. This

37:32

is

37:33

quite an old paper but it

37:35

included some cases of concussion. And Thond dupe

37:38

made a recommendation that after

37:39

a certain number, the

37:41

barriers to concussions,

37:42

there's a concussion somebody,

37:44

basically, should have played contact sports again.

37:46

That was the insinuation

37:48

of the of the piece.

37:50

But the way that Macquarie had quoted it was

37:54

inaccurate but it also altered the

37:55

mirror because

37:58

Macquarie quoted Thondike is saying

37:59

that people in these

38:02

circumstances shouldn't play sports until

38:04

the next season.

38:06

And that and

38:07

that wasn't quite

38:09

right. So that

38:10

and That dealt

38:11

with allegations, but we also needed

38:13

to take stock of

38:15

where we are to

38:17

communicate our findings,

38:20

to others,

38:20

and

38:22

and our concerns

38:22

because our trust is broken

38:25

in in this esoteric chief.

38:27

And we

38:27

needed to think about how we handle the

38:30

rest

38:30

of Macquarie's work published in our

38:32

journals

38:32

and by others to to

38:34

play their part in that.

38:36

And so on that basis, we decided to place expression

38:38

of concern upon any content,

38:40

which is written

38:41

by Macquarie

38:44

Ware he is a single or sole author. So his commentary

38:46

style pieces, but

38:47

he's also published a lot of other content,

38:50

particularly in

38:52

BGSM.

38:53

where he's published

38:56

research papers and also this

38:58

consensus statement for

39:00

concussion sport. which is very key to that

39:02

community

39:02

of doctors and and management

39:04

of that particular condition. and

39:08

we're going to ask his institution to look into the standing of

39:11

his research. And for

39:12

the makers of their consensus statement to

39:15

look into whether they consider

39:18

that there's anything

39:18

to be concerned about

39:19

in that regard. We we scream through

39:21

just for plagiarism in the

39:23

in the latest

39:26

twenty sixteen. version and

39:28

didn't find anything there. But

39:30

I still think there's a considerable way

39:32

to go

39:33

on this journey.

39:36

So assuming the best, for

39:39

example, and it's just editorial my

39:41

my Because you are kind,

39:43

haunted present. Wow. Yeah.

39:46

and and that no no harm to patients or will

39:48

come out of it. And to

39:51

what degree do you think

39:54

it is something that we're just seeing one end

39:56

of the spectrum and of

39:57

something that might be happening more

39:59

more globally. Because

39:59

I imagine

40:02

there might be a lot

40:04

of academics listening to this podcast who are quote quoting

40:08

and paraphrasing all the

40:10

time. No. No. No. No.

40:12

No. No. No. It won't

40:14

be very very paranoid because we

40:16

have this. III thought

40:18

in authentic CADE system

40:20

and and Scholar one and the art system

40:22

that sort of catches like

40:24

this gross

40:26

plagiarism. But

40:28

if so, I I once had a

40:30

paper that came and said thirty percent

40:32

overlap and he started seeing all this

40:34

color coded system of the plagiarism system. And

40:37

it was very confusing, like,

40:39

plain jenga. And and but

40:42

beyond that, I

40:44

I couldn't quite III understood that

40:46

the authors were trying to do the

40:49

best of paraphrasing, but they were

40:52

doing very badly. But it's And also, Helen,

40:54

I'm just curious before you answer that is,

40:56

like like how much of this was

41:00

self plagiarism where the,

41:01

you know, essentially, the author's writing the same thing kind of over

41:03

and over as opposed to plagiarizing somebody

41:05

else's writing, which I think is Yeah.

41:07

Mostly Even plagiarism of

41:10

other people's

41:10

content, which I think we can we can all accept

41:14

is is worse

41:15

than reusing the

41:18

AI content.

41:20

And I think, you know, there there are efforts going

41:22

on to help researchers, you

41:24

know, well intentioned people with that paranoia

41:27

of am I

41:29

and my par you know, do I

41:31

need to paraphrase this a bit more so I'm

41:33

not plagiarizing myself? And I

41:36

think this is ah publishers

41:38

are have to

41:38

be pragmatic about these things. If you're so

41:40

so

41:41

the things that we might start

41:43

to look at is

41:45

you know, if you're

41:46

reporting work from

41:48

a research study and the methods

41:50

of that research study

41:53

are the same as methods that are used in a different paper,

41:55

there isn't a need to rephrase

41:58

that for

42:00

the

42:00

sake. trying to

42:01

evade the plagiarism detection

42:04

software. You just need to reference the

42:06

methods and say they're the same, and it's

42:08

absolutely okay to reproduce those.

42:10

That's that's fine. That's

42:12

transparent and honest and useful, I

42:14

think. And That's

42:15

what sorry. That's what called

42:17

text recycling. Text recycling.

42:18

Yeah. So that so it I

42:20

suppose it's sort of like a euphemism, but it is

42:22

but it is I think I think it's

42:24

important to conceptualize it as

42:27

a totally different thing. And

42:29

it's also okay

42:30

to write about the

42:32

same research in different

42:33

ways. You know, you could publish your research

42:36

paper. Joe was

42:36

talking about his research today. He

42:38

might

42:39

then want to write an

42:41

opinion piece or he might want to write an

42:43

editorial for a diabetes journal and

42:45

to talk about that study

42:47

and he might repeat some of the results. And again, there's

42:49

no need to be constantly paraphrasing

42:52

all of that information. You just need to

42:55

make it clear and be clear that

42:57

you're not publishing redundant materials.

42:59

So if Joe were to take his research paper

43:01

and so then try and publish it somewhere else,

43:03

that would clearly be inappropriate. That would

43:05

be And just for

43:06

the record, I would never do that. I

43:09

would never do that. I

43:12

didn't want I'm coming off the major

43:14

declaration. No. But there is this practice and

43:16

we see it with trials. I was

43:18

just looking at this when

43:22

when when Ross was summarizing the diabetes

43:24

prevention trial,

43:26

there were, like,

43:27

over forty publications in,

43:30

like, a very short period of the And this is, of

43:32

course, many of them might

43:34

be super relevant, but there's

43:38

a very gray

43:38

concept that is this salami slicing where you have a study

43:41

and you make a lot of papers out

43:43

of it and which

43:45

is set up to the

43:47

point of redundancy. Right? And some

43:49

of these decisions aren't

43:51

easy. And I think this relates to,

43:53

I mean, the art sort of the

43:55

answer to your question, although I've kind of forgotten where your first question started by,

43:57

which is that in general, this

43:59

work is

44:02

is

44:02

quite complex. You have

44:04

to to weigh up a

44:06

a

44:06

lot of information. There's a lot of

44:08

judgment involved. Sometimes it's very

44:12

obvious. things have been cut and pasted. But

44:14

as you say, looking at different publications and thinking, okay, what are the

44:16

new outcomes here or what new audience

44:20

you're trying to reach, you have to you have to weigh those things

44:22

up and come to a decision just like you would

44:24

with any with any sort of

44:28

primary content. So would you

44:28

say just to wrap up, would

44:31

you say that copy and

44:33

pastes someone else's comment

44:36

or publishing your article several times is a

44:38

no no, but perhaps text

44:40

recycling for the methods that

44:44

far. They're always the same crowd. I think we should do a future

44:46

episode on plagiarism and

44:49

fraud. No. Fraud

44:51

is like that another category. I

44:52

feel like this episode has not a

44:55

very practical information for sort

44:57

of continuing professional development. CPD,

44:59

as we call it, over

45:01

here in the UK. And I think while though it's something that you'd

45:03

have to go and reflect upon

45:06

now. It's hard to have a

45:07

good all think about. How many c

45:09

me c m credits you. The listeners

45:11

get it. Well, it depends on how that we

45:13

talk to. Well, not

45:17

sure it should depend on that. It shouldn't depend on the

45:19

quality of the conversation. But Joe tells

45:21

us there's somewhere else he has to be enough. So

45:23

we we we've gotta wrap it up

45:25

for this week. bring the show to

45:27

a friend. So I would like to thank you both, Juan and Joe for

45:29

joining

45:29

us. I'd like to thank

45:31

you for listening. you

45:34

can subscribe wherever you

45:35

listen to a podcast, you can rate us

45:38

especially if you like to rate us

45:40

very highly.

45:42

we really enjoyed looking into Nicole's query

45:44

this month. And if

45:45

you have a query that you'd like us

45:47

to look

45:48

into, or you'd like

45:50

to give further query to me so I can

45:52

give mine and do some homework to do,

45:54

then that would be wonderful.

45:56

So until next month, it's goodbye for me. Goodbye

45:59

for me.

45:59

Goodbye for me.

46:02

Take care.

46:02

the cow that

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features