Podchaser Logo
Home
Extinction as Shakespearean Tragedy – The Romeo Error (with Chris Haney)

Extinction as Shakespearean Tragedy – The Romeo Error (with Chris Haney)

Released Monday, 20th December 2021
Good episode? Give it some love!
Extinction as Shakespearean Tragedy – The Romeo Error (with Chris Haney)

Extinction as Shakespearean Tragedy – The Romeo Error (with Chris Haney)

Extinction as Shakespearean Tragedy – The Romeo Error (with Chris Haney)

Extinction as Shakespearean Tragedy – The Romeo Error (with Chris Haney)

Monday, 20th December 2021
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:02

From the Center for

0:02

the Advancement of the Steady

0:05

State Economy, this is The

0:05

Steady Stater, a podcast

0:08

dedicated to discussing limits

0:08

to growth and the Steady State

0:12

Economy.

0:17

Welcome to the

0:17

show. I'm your host Brian Czech,

0:20

and our guest today is Dr. James

0:20

Christopher Haney, President and

0:24

Founder of Terra Mar Applied

0:24

Sciences, an ecological and

0:28

economic research company

0:28

devoted to science-driven

0:32

solutions for land or sea. Chris

0:32

has been a Pew Marine Fellow at

0:36

Woods Hole, a wildlife faculty

0:36

member at Penn State, and a

0:40

forest ecologist at the

0:40

Wilderness Society. But I met

0:44

Chris somewhere around 2002,

0:44

when he was in Washington, D.C.

0:48

as the Chief Scientist for

0:48

Defenders of Wildlife. I know

0:52

Chris to be a creative

0:52

scientist, who happens to be an

0:55

outstanding communicator, a

0:55

talented administrator, and a

0:59

great mentor to students and

0:59

early career professionals.

1:03

Chris Haney, welcome to The

1:03

Steady Stater.

1:06

Thank you, Brian. It's good to be here.

1:09

It's great to have

1:09

you in. Hate to do this, but I

1:11

want to put you on the spot

1:11

right off the bat. Which of

1:15

those places the Woods Hole,

1:15

Wilderness Society, or Defenders

1:19

of Wildlife? Which one was your

1:19

favorite?

1:22

Well, there's

1:22

without a question. It was Woods

1:24

Hole, because I love the sea.

1:24

And not only was my office a

1:29

block away from the ocean, I

1:29

lived in a small cottage that

1:32

overlooks Sippewissett Marsh and

1:32

a beach. So you know, I was

1:38

around the sea every day for

1:38

three years. And it was pretty

1:42

hard to beat that.

1:43

Oh that sounds

1:43

wonderful. A lot of our

1:46

listeners have some familiarity

1:46

with government agencies, and

1:50

nonprofit organizations, and

1:50

colleges, and universities. I'm

1:54

guessing that far fewer know

1:54

much at all about consulting

1:58

firms, and especially the type

1:58

of firm you run. Let me read the

2:02

first sentence from your website

2:02

-- Terra Mar Applied Sciences,

2:06

LLC, conducts ecological and

2:06

economic research in the public

2:11

interest, then applies it

2:11

creatively using principles of

2:15

civic responsibility and

2:15

environmental stewardship. You

2:19

guys almost sound like a federal

2:19

science agency with a statement

2:23

like that. But give us some

2:23

examples of what you actually

2:27

do, Chris?

2:28

Sure. Well, we work

2:28

very, very closely with federal

2:32

agencies. In fact, our core

2:32

suite of projects encompass

2:36

advising and sometimes field

2:36

research on behalf of such

2:41

agencies as the Bureau of Ocean

2:41

Energy Management, the US

2:45

Geological Survey, US Fish and

2:45

Wildlife Service and the

2:49

National Oceanic and Atmospheric

2:49

Administration. So we do this

2:54

often in partnerships that

2:54

involve other entities besides

2:58

the federal government, but we

2:58

include them and also may

3:01

include such things as

3:01

universities and research

3:05

institutes. So it generally

3:05

means that the public interest

3:09

aspect of our portfolio means we

3:09

deal with environmental issues

3:14

that are largely driven by what

3:14

the public can benefit from, not

3:19

necessarily what private

3:19

business benefits from.

3:23

Okay, well, one

3:23

thing I know about your career

3:26

is that you've been able to meld

3:26

a good deal of ecological

3:30

expertise and economic insight

3:30

into the programs you've run.

3:34

And now you've got a new book

3:34

hot off the press called Woody's

3:38

Last Laugh - How the Extinct

3:38

Ivory-billed Woodpecker Fools Us

3:43

Into Making 53 Thinking Errors.

3:43

Chris, before we delve into the

3:48

contents of this very unique

3:48

book, I'm really curious, what

3:52

was the impetus for taking this

3:52

on? And who is your target

3:56

audience?

3:57

Well, you know, I'll start with my target audience, Brian. It's almost

3:59

anyone who wants to think more

4:03

clearly, who wants to make

4:03

better decisions, and who wants

4:08

to be less -- I guess --

4:08

influenced or biased by one's

4:13

own mind, but also the attempts

4:13

of others. So it's really

4:17

something for almost everyone.

4:17

If you're interested in American

4:21

environmental history, if you're

4:21

interested in conservation,

4:25

biology, wildlife biology,

4:25

science, I think it will also

4:29

interest you. How did I get

4:29

involved in it -- it was it was

4:33

a rather long path. I became

4:33

fascinated in 2004 by how crazy,

4:40

nutty, confrontational, and

4:40

downright mean people were about

4:46

this bird, especially after its

4:46

putative discovery and

4:50

rediscovery in 2004 and 2005, so

4:50

that was part of my interest.

4:54

The other thing that piqued my

4:54

interest was it seemed like

4:58

there were contradictions about

4:58

the basic life history of the

5:01

bird. And these two things,

5:01

pulled me in and hooked me. And

5:07

I couldn't make sense of it

5:07

until -- I remembered some time

5:11

that I spent with economists

5:11

back in my fellowship days at

5:15

Woods Hole. And they used to

5:15

teach me about heuristics, about

5:19

these mental shortcuts that we

5:19

take. And so I started an

5:22

exploration into cognitive

5:22

psychology, reading books that

5:26

might be standard in economics,

5:26

like Kahneman's Thinking, Fast

5:30

and Slow. But these ideas are

5:30

really not so well-known inside

5:35

natural science. And as I

5:35

discovered conservation biology,

5:40

and especially about

5:40

ivory-billed woodpecker, we make

5:43

lots of little mistakes.

5:46

Well, you

5:46

definitely took a deep dive into

5:48

human reasoning and cognitive

5:48

bias and the use of those

5:53

heuristics. Actually, let's

5:53

start with that. Because the

5:56

word and concept of heuristic

5:56

trips a lot of people up I

6:00

think, so please tell us, Chris,

6:00

what exactly is a heuristic?

6:05

Sure, Brian.

6:05

Heuristic is simply a shortcut,

6:09

a mental shortcut that we take.

6:09

And when we do, they help us

6:14

make decisions quickly. And so

6:14

that's the good side of them.

6:17

The bad side of heuristic, or a

6:17

shortcut, is our decisions are

6:21

often inaccurate, incomplete, or

6:21

biased in a particular

6:27

direction.

6:28

So what would be an

6:28

example of a heuristic that's

6:31

commonly used in wildlife

6:31

conservation, then?

6:35

Well, there are

6:35

lots of them in biology,

6:38

unfortunately. One of the ones

6:38

that I've been most worried

6:41

about stems from our neglect of

6:41

probability. And in the case of

6:46

wood, claiming extinction for

6:46

various wildlife or plants, we

6:51

often err on the side of

6:51

declaring a species gone or dead

6:56

too soon. That bias comes from

6:56

something called base rate

7:01

fallacy. That is to say, we

7:01

ignore a background rate in

7:06

nature that we ought to pay

7:06

attention to. And we think that

7:09

our reasoning is infallible,

7:09

that we know the answers all the

7:13

time, and we simply don't.

7:15

And that base rate

7:15

fallacy, is that what leads to

7:18

this type of error that you

7:18

wrote about the Romeo error? And

7:22

why is it why is it named Romeo

7:22

error?

7:25

Yes, exactly. So

7:25

Romeo error is named for the

7:29

Shakespearean tragedy, in which

7:29

Juliet wakes up to see that

7:34

Romeo has already killed

7:34

himself, because he thought she

7:39

was dead. And then when she sees

7:39

him dead, she takes her own life

7:43

as well. So it's almost a double

7:43

tragedy here. And so in

7:46

conservation biology, when we

7:46

declare an animal or plant gone

7:51

too early, we might back away

7:51

from protections, we might back

7:56

away from conservation practice,

7:56

we might stop any kind of

8:00

protection that might have been

8:00

in place, and that can lead to

8:03

even further tragedy. So it was

8:03

given the name Romeo error in

8:08

the early 1990s. And it's kind

8:08

of stuck.

8:12

It's a lose-lose.

8:12

Do we have any clues about the

8:15

prevalence of this Romeo error?

8:18

We do. A paper

8:18

about -- oh I guess almost 10

8:22

years ago now -- found that in

8:22

the last century and a quarter,

8:27

we have refound somewhere

8:27

between 350 and 400 species of

8:34

animals that were thought to be

8:34

gone, but were rediscovered.

8:37

Wow.

8:38

Yeah, it's a large

8:38

number. In fact, that number is

8:41

just increasing. So we're

8:41

actually getting an acceleration

8:45

in the rate of these

8:45

rediscoveries. Those animals and

8:50

plants that are refound are

8:50

called Lazarus species. Because

8:56

they can that -- from a

8:56

distance, it looks like they

8:58

were dead and then resurrected.

8:58

Of course, we know that's not

9:01

the case. But it's prevalent in

9:01

mammals, birds, reptiles,

9:05

amphibians, and fishes, as well

9:05

as lesser known invertebrates

9:10

and plants.

9:11

What other kinds of

9:11

mental pitfalls influence our

9:15

beliefs about wildlife

9:15

extinctions? And does that mess

9:18

up our approach to the economics

9:18

of conservation, too?

9:22

Yeah, so let me try

9:22

to draw a direct parallel

9:25

between wildlife conservation

9:25

and economics. Suppose that we

9:29

have a belief about how

9:29

something affects our economy,

9:33

just like we have a belief about

9:33

how it might affect a species

9:38

prospects for survival or

9:38

extinction. If we fail to

9:42

incorporate new information when

9:42

it comes in -- that is to say we

9:47

stick rigidly to our original

9:47

beliefs -- we fall to a bias

9:52

called Bayesian conservatism, or

9:52

-- put another way -- a lack of

9:57

Bayesian caution. In other

9:57

words, we become anchored --

10:01

that's called an anchoring bias.

10:01

Your listeners might recognize

10:05

this when they go in and lease a

10:05

car, buy a car, and the car

10:09

salesman tries to anchor you on

10:09

a higher price that you don't

10:13

want to pay. And then when they

10:13

come down 1000 or 2000, you

10:16

think you've got a bargain. But

10:16

when we don't move our beliefs

10:21

enough, we're guilty of two or

10:21

three biases, including

10:25

anchoring and Bayesian

10:25

conservatism.

10:29

What's the one

10:29

where -- I think you talked a

10:31

little bit about if a species is

10:31

merely out of sight, yet we call

10:36

it gone forever? Isn't that like

10:36

what babies do? And their mother

10:41

leaves the room, they conclude

10:41

that she's gone for good?

10:44

Yeah, exactly,

10:44

Brian, and this is one of the

10:48

most fascinating ones for me,

10:48

and it's ubiquitous with the

10:52

ivory-billed woodpecker, and

10:52

frankly, other birds and some

10:56

mammals that disappear from our

10:56

site. We are like young

11:00

children. When a species is

11:00

gone, we declare it extinct just

11:04

because we can't see it. That

11:04

bias is a cognitive one that

11:08

usually we grow out of as

11:08

infants. It's called lack of

11:12

object permanence, meaning that

11:12

if something isn't there, we

11:16

think it's gone forever. And

11:16

that -- object in constancy is

11:20

another term -- is more

11:20

prevalent in some of us as

11:24

individuals than others. That is

11:24

to say, you might have less of

11:28

this problem than I would. So it

11:28

varies among individuals, but it

11:32

is a contribution to why we

11:32

declare animals and plants

11:36

extinct too soon.

11:38

Okay, well, we've

11:38

got some more questions for you

11:41

here. But first, we need to take

11:41

a short non-commercial break

11:45

with James Lamont. And while

11:45

we're on break, Chris, I wonder

11:48

if you can track down an audio

11:48

recording of an ivory-billed.

11:53

Meanwhile, take it away, James.

12:01

Hello, listeners,

12:01

we hope you're enjoying our

12:03

conversation with Dr. Chris

12:03

Haney, President and Founder

12:06

Terra Mar Applied Sciences. We

12:06

at CASSE just want to take a

12:10

moment to thank everyone

12:10

listening for their support

12:12

during 2021. We also want to

12:12

wish you all a safe,

12:15

sustainable, and steady-state

12:15

holiday season. And if you

12:19

should bump into Santa

12:19

delivering lumps of coal, show

12:22

him our position statement on

12:22

economic growth. It would surely

12:25

make his Christmas a lot less

12:25

stressful. And now, back to the

12:28

show.

12:30

Welcome back to the

12:30

show. And you know what? We

12:34

built people's expectations up

12:34

for a sound file of the

12:38

ivory-billed. What did we come

12:38

up with there, Chris?

12:40

Well, I just sent

12:40

you a file... [sound] ...and it

12:57

sounds much like a tiny tin

12:57

horn. And it's not a very loud

13:06

sound for such a large bird. But

13:06

it is the only recording that we

13:11

have from a known ivory-billed

13:11

woodpecker ever in history, is

13:15

just this one. And it was taken

13:15

in the late 1930s by James T.

13:22

Tanner and a Cornell team of

13:22

acousticians in the singer track

13:27

of Northeast Louisiana.

13:29

The famous clip,

13:29

yes. And to me a lot of

13:32

woodpeckers sound like they're

13:32

laughing and so, you know, your

13:35

book is loaded with not just

13:35

metaphors alone, but all kinds

13:39

of figures of speech. And I'm

13:39

thinking well is that have a

13:43

little bit to do it to Woody's

13:43

Last Laugh title, but. Now,

13:48

Chris, how did you react to that

13:48

recent proposal by the US Fish

13:52

and Wildlife Service to delist

13:52

23 plants and animals as

13:57

extinct, including the

13:57

ivory-billed?

14:00

Well, Brian, I laughed.

14:03

You laughed!

14:04

I did. Because you

14:04

know, we've been declaring this

14:07

bird extinct since 1913. So for

14:07

more than 100 years, we've been

14:13

declaring this bird is gone. And

14:13

you know, the bird just doesn't

14:17

get the memo. It just keeps

14:17

popping up after long intervals

14:21

of apparent absence. And I laugh

14:21

because we are much like the old

14:27

cartoon strip that you might

14:27

have heard of from the 1930s and

14:31

40s. With Woody the woodpecker.

14:31

We are Wally Walrus, we get set

14:36

up again and again by Woody the

14:36

Woodpecker, and Woody tricks us

14:42

over, and over, and over again.

14:44

Hey, well, you must

14:44

have had a number of different

14:47

titles in mind for this book

14:47

before you settled on this one.

14:52

I did. They weren't

14:52

nearly as good, you know. This

14:57

is actually -- Brian -- I felt

14:57

this way myself. Can I share a

15:01

story of how I felt I was

15:01

tricked?

15:04

Yeah.

15:05

So as a young boy,

15:05

my grandfather would give me old

15:09

1900s -- or late 1800s, early

15:09

1900s books about birds. And

15:16

from reading them, I just

15:16

assumed that the ivory-billed

15:18

was extinct. And, you know, I

15:18

didn't think much more about it,

15:22

it just put it away, filed it

15:22

away, and went on. And when the

15:26

US Fish and Wildlife Service

15:26

completed their recovery plan

15:29

for the ivory-billed woodpecker

15:29

in 2010, I raced over to the

15:34

appendix. And believe it or not,

15:34

in its Appendix E, I tallied 100

15:42

incidents of people seeing this

15:42

bird since the 1940s. That was

15:48

when it was supposed to do have

15:48

died off.

15:50

Oh.

15:52

And I thought, this

15:52

is not how an extinct bird

15:55

should behave. And so you know,

15:55

what is this? What kind of trick

16:00

is being played here? And I

16:00

really did feel pranked. I was

16:04

kind of annoyed, actually.

16:07

Well, Chris, what

16:07

makes us so eager to declare

16:10

this bird gone, then?

16:11

Well, I think one

16:11

is that we let a cognitive bias

16:15

called anchoring fixate us on

16:15

the birds extinction. And so

16:21

there are a couple of pieces

16:21

that go into this anchoring. One

16:24

is that the story of its

16:24

extinction has been repeated so

16:28

often. And that constitutes an

16:28

availability cascade. That is to

16:32

say -- you repeat something

16:32

often enough, and people will

16:35

believe it's true, even if it

16:35

isn't. That's one thing that

16:39

sort of predisposes us towards

16:39

extinction. And another one --

16:43

and this one really fascinates

16:43

me. It's called the negativity

16:47

bias. And we're not an

16:47

instinctually optimistic

16:52

species, that is to say, our

16:52

brains, our minds, don't just

16:57

naturally go into a kind of

16:57

happy, cheerful optimism. And so

17:03

if we're presented with death,

17:03

and life, death has a whole lot

17:07

more salience -- that is to say,

17:07

it's a lot more available to our

17:11

mind, we will fixate on it. And

17:11

of course, death is a bit more

17:16

serious than life. And so it has

17:16

a greater power to kind of

17:20

influence us. So these things

17:20

contribute to -- it's really

17:25

hard for us to get out of the

17:25

mindset that this bird may not

17:29

be extinct.

17:31

Well, that's very

17:31

interesting in it -- in a way it

17:35

resonates. And yet, we have this

17:35

optimism, that's -- I would say,

17:41

plagued the conservation

17:41

community for decades now,

17:45

because it's a false optimism. I

17:45

think, you know, what I'm

17:48

talking about the win-win

17:48

rhetoric, you know, there is no

17:52

conflict between growing the

17:52

economy and protecting it, we

17:56

expose that all the time on The

17:56

Steady Stater. And that's been a

18:00

political tool, I guess, running

18:00

in the face of that tendency

18:05

toward negativism.

18:07

Well. And actually

18:07

this problem that you point out

18:10

-- this picture that the economy

18:10

can just continue to grow

18:14

without there being any kind of

18:14

serious consequences -- that's a

18:18

framing effect, Brian. It's sort

18:18

of, it's a way of anchoring the

18:23

the listeners or the viewers

18:23

expectations about a topic in

18:28

advance, and creating a story, a

18:28

narrative that's hard to get

18:34

away from, because it's sort of

18:34

put out there first. And I am an

18:37

environmental science, not an

18:37

economist. But I do believe that

18:41

this idea that economic systems

18:41

could continue to grow came

18:45

first. And so it's hard for us

18:45

to undo that. And that's a

18:49

framing effect. And that's

18:49

another bias that we have to be

18:53

very, very careful about. And

18:53

it's one that both economics and

18:58

conservation have to learn more

18:58

about in order to get the truth

19:03

out to the public.

19:05

Right. So the 53

19:05

thinking errors, most of them

19:10

are biases of one type or

19:10

another. Is that the case?

19:15

They are. Both of

19:15

these are cognitive biases, but

19:18

I also included some of the more

19:18

frequent logical fallacies. One

19:24

of my, I guess pet peeves with

19:24

the ivory-billed woodpecker is

19:28

something called the argument

19:28

from ignorance. The argument

19:32

from ignorance is a

19:32

philosophical fallacy, a logical

19:36

fallacy, basically goes like

19:36

this -- it tries to prove a

19:40

point by the absence of data. So

19:40

in the case of -- you know where

19:49

this is going in the ivory-billed...

19:51

It's going to fake news, I guess.

19:53

Yes, indeed. So the

19:53

ivory-billed is extinct, because

19:57

we cannot prove that it's alive.

19:57

And of course, the absence of

20:01

evidence is not evidence of

20:01

absence. And this is a common

20:06

error with conservation biology

20:06

and declarations of extinction.

20:12

Because it's very, very

20:12

difficult to prove a negative.

20:16

It's very, it's quite difficult

20:16

to demonstrate even with high

20:20

statistical significance that an

20:20

animal or plant isn't there,

20:25

that's hard to do.

20:27

Well, you know, the

20:27

list of 23 species that fish and

20:32

wildlife proposed for delisting,

20:32

and once again, they'd all be

20:36

delisted, due to real or

20:36

supposedly extinction. Now, 11

20:43

of them are from the contiguous

20:43

48 states, almost all in the

20:46

South and the other 12 are from

20:46

Hawaii, which of course, is

20:49

ecologically messed up with all

20:49

the invasive species especially.

20:54

But you know, what stood out

20:54

about the ivory-billed

20:56

woodpecker to me, Chris, is that

20:56

when Fish and Wildlife Service

21:00

described the causes of its

21:00

endangerment, it listed only one

21:05

cause -- and that was logging.

21:05

For all the other 23 species,

21:10

there are multiple causes

21:10

identified like agriculture,

21:14

mining, road construction,

21:14

urbanization, industrial

21:18

development, reservoirs, and

21:18

even outdoor recreation. Does

21:22

this honing in on logging give

21:22

us any kind of insights into the

21:26

biases committed on behalf of

21:26

the woodpecker?

21:30

Well, yes, it does,

21:30

in both directions. And what I

21:34

mean is that we don't actually

21:34

have strong evidence that

21:37

logging harmed the woodpecker.

21:37

We've made some inferences that

21:42

that's the case. We've made some

21:42

deductions, we've made some

21:46

indirect conclusions. But the

21:46

woodpecker was nomadic, it was a

21:51

very strong flyer, and most

21:51

woodpeckers, including the

21:55

smaller ones, can move away from

21:55

areas that get logged, and just

21:59

go to other places. So that's

21:59

kind of a strange thing. I was

22:03

also amazed that the declaration

22:03

of presumed extinction for

22:08

ivory-billed did not mention

22:08

direct persecution, shooting,

22:13

hunting, collecting.

22:14

Right.

22:15

Many people believe

22:15

that was a strong contribution

22:19

to the woodpeckers scarcity. And

22:19

I don't know why that

22:24

information was left out, quite

22:24

honestly.

22:27

Maybe a little bit

22:27

of explanatory value for that as

22:32

that ivory-billed was from that

22:32

class of 1967 -- you know, that

22:36

was the first group of species

22:36

listed pursuant to the

22:40

Endangered Species Act, and that

22:40

included 14 mammals and 36 bird

22:45

species, 6 reptiles, 6

22:45

amphibians, and 22 fish species.

22:52

And I was wondering if you found

22:52

any patterns, or if you looked

22:56

at the list of 23 that was

22:56

recently proposed for delisting,

23:00

several of them were from that

23:00

class of 67. And they probably

23:06

had back then, well, more

23:06

simplified, simplistic, even I

23:11

suppose approach to describing

23:11

the causes of impairment.

23:16

Yes, I -- what

23:16

stood out to me is that in the

23:20

case of the island birds, those

23:20

that were found -- there was one

23:24

bird, I think, from Guam, and

23:24

the rest were from Hawaii, with

23:27

two exceptions, the ivory-billed

23:27

woodpecker, and the Bachman's

23:31

warbler. And then there were

23:31

several freshwater clams or

23:35

mussels, which are, in some

23:35

cases, were or are restricted to

23:39

a single watershed, a single

23:39

river system. When an animal or

23:43

plant has that local a

23:43

distribution, they're already

23:46

uncommon or rare to begin with,

23:46

just due to the fact of such a

23:50

highly restricted range. And so,

23:50

first of all, it's easier to

23:54

show that they're not there --

23:54

I'm not saying that this was

23:57

satisfactorily demonstrated in

23:57

each of those 23 species cases

24:03

-- but it's easier to tell

24:03

something isn't there, say in

24:07

one mountain range on one

24:07

island. The problem with the

24:11

ivory-billed woodpecker is it

24:11

occurred across a dozen states

24:15

in its original range. And the

24:15

Fish and Wildlife Service's

24:19

recovery plan show just between

24:19

2000 and 2010, 26 sighting

24:25

reports across about 7 or 8

24:25

states. And it's very much

24:30

harder to safely conclude that a

24:30

species isn't there when the

24:36

range is that large. So I would

24:36

say if I looked at all of those

24:40

23 species in the recent

24:40

proposal to delist, I would say

24:45

that this one is the head

24:45

scratcher. This one is the one I

24:49

believe is more problematic. It

24:49

doesn't mean that the other ones

24:52

are extinct. It just means that

24:52

the absences for sighting

24:57

reports for these other animals

24:57

is probably longer, and they

25:01

have smaller ranges. So

25:01

extinction would be more likely

25:04

just on those grounds. But

25:04

that's the big difference I see,

25:08

is that we had two birds in that

25:08

list, the Bachman's warbler and

25:12

the ivory-billed woodpecker that

25:12

had large ranges. And here's

25:15

another interesting thing,

25:15

Brian, we do not have 100

25:19

sighting reports of Bachman's

25:19

warbler since the 1940s. But we

25:24

do for ivory-billed, and that

25:24

puzzles me. I don't see a good

25:28

reason for that. Except that

25:28

maybe one of them isn't really

25:32

dead.

25:33

Well, yeah, of

25:33

course, those warblers. They're

25:36

so alike and hard to spot a lot

25:36

of times, whereas -- what about

25:40

the pileated? You suppose some

25:40

of these ivory-billed sightings

25:43

may have just been pileated

25:43

woodpeckers?

25:46

Well, I would say

25:46

that the likelihood of that

25:48

happening is high. But here's

25:48

the other puzzle, a conundrum,

25:53

enigma. We've been demanded to

25:53

-- sort of say -- all cases of

26:00

ivory-billed woodpeckers were

26:00

expected birdwatchers, who went

26:05

out, and we're finding what they

26:05

set out to see and they really

26:08

saw pileated. There's many

26:08

problems with that. One is we've

26:12

only allowed the similarities of

26:12

these two big woodpeckers to run

26:16

in one direction. We're expected

26:16

to have people confuse pileated

26:20

for ivory-billed. But where are

26:20

the warnings about confusing

26:24

ivory-billed for pileated. We

26:24

don't even have those. The other

26:28

thing is that the expectation

26:28

for the last 70 or more years is

26:32

the bird is dead. So people

26:32

don't knowingly go search for

26:38

dead birds. And then you have

26:38

people that have seen them that

26:42

have spent 6 or 8, 10 in some

26:42

cases, 20 or more years looking.

26:47

And it becomes rather

26:47

implausible to say that if

26:49

somebody is impetuous and hasty,

26:49

why spend 20 years proving that?

26:56

You know what I mean?

26:56

There's sort of a

26:56

synergy of these cognitive

26:59

biases, it sounds like.

27:00

Exactly, exactly.

27:00

And so what I discovered in my

27:04

book is that there's actually

27:04

something -- a predisposition to

27:08

not report, the ivory-billed

27:08

woodpecker. One, is it's dead.

27:14

Two, let's protect it. I don't

27:14

want anybody to disturb it.

27:17

Three, if you're a hunter

27:17

fisherman living in the South,

27:21

you wouldn't know who to report

27:21

this to. You're disconnected in

27:25

social space from scientists and

27:25

so forth. These and other things

27:31

predispose us if the woodpecker

27:31

is still there to actually

27:35

under-report it, not

27:35

over-reported.

27:39

Yeah, that makes

27:39

sense. I do want to acknowledge

27:42

that our very last guest

27:42

actually on the podcast, Ann

27:46

Vileisis, who you probably know

27:46

as the author of that

27:49

outstanding book on wetlands in

27:49

wetland loss, discovering the

27:54

unknown landscape. She wrote at

27:54

length, and we discussed it a

27:59

little bit in the episode about

27:59

the wipeout of bald cypress

28:04

swamps in the South. And

28:04

certainly that was sort of the

28:09

top habitat in the broadest of

28:09

terms -- top, you know, canopy

28:14

cover and feeding tree and

28:14

nesting tree, you know, habitat

28:18

at large for the ivory-billed,

28:18

so. The logging would have been

28:22

very important. But yeah, you

28:22

almost have to wonder if there

28:27

were some policy implications

28:27

that were coming into the

28:32

picture when that was listed.

28:35

Yes. And it's

28:35

interesting, if you go back and

28:38

read what some naturalist said

28:38

in the 1950s, they were not as

28:44

pessimistic as everyone else.

28:44

And they said, you know, it's

28:48

true, the heavy logging at the

28:48

end of the 1800s and in the

28:54

early part of the 1900s kind of

28:54

created a bottleneck. It really

28:59

-- I think the big unanswered

28:59

question is whether the

29:03

ivory-billed woodpecker could

29:03

have gotten through that logging

29:09

bottleneck without going

29:09

extinct. I don't know the

29:12

answer. I really don't write.

29:12

But if it did, it might actually

29:16

be facing a Southeast United

29:16

States that might have been

29:19

slapped. That is slightly better

29:19

shape today than it was that.

29:23

Right. Well, that's

29:23

true. Well, I still I just have

29:27

to ask you if you were forced to

29:27

a gambling table in Vegas, and

29:32

all these cognitive biases and

29:32

mental mishaps aside, you with

29:37

your scientific chops, where

29:37

would you place the odds of

29:41

extinction? Extinct per se.

29:44

You're putting me

29:44

on the spot here, Brian. I would

29:48

first seek to avoid Bayesian

29:48

conservatism, a lack of Bayesian

29:54

caution. And I would run away

29:54

from 100% or 0% on either one.

30:01

Okay. So I would allow that the

30:01

probability is not 100%, but not

30:10

0, either. For either extinction

30:10

or survival, it's somewhere in

30:14

the middle. And I -- if I were

30:14

to ask myself, Chris, if you

30:19

could actually know the truth,

30:19

which one would surprise you

30:22

more? To really learn that the

30:22

bird was dead? Or that the bird

30:27

was still alive? And so I think

30:27

I would probably say, it would

30:31

surprise me just slightly more

30:31

to learn that the bird was

30:36

extinct. So maybe, let's say, my

30:36

odds of survival would be 55%

30:43

survival, 45% extinction, which

30:43

is that's not that's not quite

30:47

50-50. But it's close. And

30:47

that's because I can explain

30:51

most of the nuttiness around

30:51

this bird -- the contradiction

30:55

due to cognitive bias, I can --

30:55

by the fact I can -- explain all

30:58

of it to myself, you might not

30:58

be convinced, but I am. On the

31:02

other hand, because I'm a

31:02

population ecologist, or I'd

31:07

like to be one, we've never had

31:07

good numbers for the bird. So I

31:11

could believe that anyone that

31:11

shows up would be the last one.

31:15

You know, there's no way of knowing.

31:17

Right. One of the

31:17

main points you've made, though,

31:20

I think, all along is that --

31:20

well, extinction is forever, of

31:24

course, but an extinction

31:24

listing is pretty much forever

31:28

as well, it closes a lot of

31:28

managerial and policy doors. And

31:33

that's one of the biggest

31:33

problems with the types of

31:37

outcomes from these cognitive

31:37

biases.

31:40

It is. And I wish

31:40

we would adopt the standards of

31:43

the International Union for the

31:43

Conservation of Nature, BirdLife

31:48

International. And we'd have

31:48

this in between category, which

31:52

is highly imperiled, presumed

31:52

extinct. It's not quite final,

31:58

but it's right at death's door.

31:58

And elsewhere around the globe

32:03

around the planet, that kind of

32:03

category is where we park --

32:07

birds and mammals and plants,

32:07

fish and so forth -- where we

32:11

don't know. I think that's a

32:11

more logical and more cautious

32:16

and a more reasonable place to

32:16

put our uncertainty.

32:20

Well I think that

32:20

gives us a nice follow up

32:23

episode. How does that sound,

32:23

Chris?

32:26

Sounds great to me, Brian.

32:27

All right. Well,

32:27

thank you so much for being on

32:29

the show today, and we'll talk

32:29

to you down the road a bit.

32:33

All right. Thank

32:33

you, Brian. Good to be here.

32:51

Well, folks, that

32:51

about wrap her up. We've been

32:53

talking with Dr. Chris Haney,

32:53

Founder and President of Terra

32:57

Mar Applied Sciences. What a

32:57

unique and exceptional journey

33:01

he's taken us on, exploring some

33:01

of the depths of human folly.

33:06

You know, it's hard to find a

33:06

silver lining in an extinction

33:10

announcement, whether it's

33:10

correct or not. But given such a

33:14

sour ecological lemon, we got to

33:14

make some kind of lemonade

33:19

bittersweet as it might be.

33:19

We've got to take this

33:22

announcement of the ivory-billed

33:22

extinction, as yet another

33:26

warning to humanity. That's the

33:26

closest thing to a silver lining

33:30

we'll get. So we'd better take

33:30

it. Each one of these species in

33:35

the USA and in the world upon

33:35

its extinction gives us one more

33:41

chance to stop and reflect. In

33:41

addition to being a beautiful,

33:47

evolved, priceless creation in

33:47

its own right, each one of these

33:52

species is like a canary in the

33:52

coal mine. Or maybe we should

33:56

look at it like a feather of the

33:56

canary in the coal mine. How

34:01

fewer feathers can that canary

34:01

take? And where does that leave

34:05

us? I'm Brian Czech, and you've

34:05

been listening to the Steady

34:09

Stater podcast. See you next

34:09

time.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features