Podchaser Logo
Home
What if Placebos ARE the Medicine?

What if Placebos ARE the Medicine?

Released Friday, 21st July 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
What if Placebos ARE the Medicine?

What if Placebos ARE the Medicine?

What if Placebos ARE the Medicine?

What if Placebos ARE the Medicine?

Friday, 21st July 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:05

The placebo

0:05

effect is when someone sick

0:07

gets better by taking a fake pill

0:10

that contains no actual medicine. Or

0:12

they get fake injections. Or even

0:15

fake surgery. The effect is not

0:17

in your head. The results are scientific

0:20

and measurable. That already sounds

0:22

crazy. But it gets a lot

0:24

crazier. We have these expectation

0:27

effects that go kind of beyond the medical

0:29

setting into things like how

0:31

easily you can perform a workout, you know, how

0:34

well you can get fit,

0:35

the effects of sleep loss, even

0:38

the effects of your mindset on aging

0:40

and how quickly you age. These are

0:42

all expectation effects that go way beyond

0:45

what we once knew about the placebo effect. I'm

0:47

David Pove, and you're about to hear some

0:50

very strange unsung science.

0:57

Hey, Prime Members, you can listen to Unsung

0:59

Science ad-free on Amazon Music.

1:02

Download the app today. Get

1:27

started at Dexcom.com. Dexcom

1:30

data on file 2023. If your glucose alerts

1:32

and readings from the G7 do not match symptoms or expectations,

1:34

use a blood glucose meter to make diabetes treatment decisions.

1:37

For a list of compatible devices, visit www.dexcom.com

1:40

slash compatibility.

1:41

I'm Candace DeLong, and on my new podcast,

1:43

Killer Psyche Daily, I share a quick

1:46

10-minute rundown every weekday

1:48

on the motivations and behaviors of the

1:50

cold-blooded killers you read about in

1:52

the news. Listen to the Amazon Music

1:55

exclusive podcast, Killer Psyche

1:57

Daily, in the Amazon Music app.

1:59

and unload the app today.

2:02

Season 2, Episode 15, what

2:05

if placebos are the medicine?

2:11

Every modern army employs medics

2:14

to treat the wounded soldiers. And

2:16

in World War II, anesthesiologist

2:18

Henry Beecher was one of them. And

2:21

what he found was that when he was treating

2:23

soldiers, he found that often, you know,

2:26

they seemed to kind of

2:28

not need pain relief when they came off

2:30

the battlefield. And this

2:32

got him kind of wondering whether it was just the pure relief

2:35

of having been saved from the battlefield that

2:37

was kind of producing its own euphoria

2:39

that was then reducing their

2:41

kind of pain that they were feeling. So they actually refused

2:44

to have morphine when they were offered

2:46

it, which is incredible. And this was quite a high

2:48

percentage of people. So that suggested

2:50

there was some way that kind of the psychology

2:52

of the soldier could influence the pain they

2:55

were feeling.

2:56

This is David Robson, author of

2:58

The Expectation Effect, How

3:00

Your Mindset Can Transform Your Life.

3:03

Now, there's this other story that says that

3:05

he actually ran out of his

3:07

drugs at one point. And so his

3:09

nurse started injecting salt

3:13

solution instead of morphine. And

3:15

what they found was that a lot of these soldiers

3:17

really did experience that kind of pain

3:19

relief. And it seemed to be quite profound. And then, you

3:22

know, he studied this in later, more controlled experiments

3:24

and found that that was indeed the

3:26

case. Every

3:29

year, the pharmaceutical industry tests

3:31

thousands of new drugs.

3:33

And in clinical trials, to see if they

3:35

actually work, they usually test

3:37

each drug candidate against pills

3:39

that look identical but don't

3:42

actually contain any medicine. They're

3:44

just sugar pills. They're called

3:46

placebos. The idea

3:49

is not to compare the candidate drug with

3:51

doing nothing. That would be a bad

3:54

study because even if the medicine

3:56

doesn't do anything, some people

3:58

will get better.

3:59

because of the placebo effect. The

4:02

researchers might think that the medicine works

4:04

when it actually doesn't. No,

4:06

the point of comparing the test medicine against

4:08

the placebo is to subtract

4:11

out what's called the placebo response.

4:14

For example,

4:15

if 75% of patients get better with

4:18

the actual medicine, and 50%

4:21

of them get better taking the placebo, you

4:24

know that the actual medicine's effect

4:26

is 25%. Keep

4:28

in mind that the people who got the fake pills

4:31

might be showing improvement because they just

4:33

got better over time,

4:35

or saw a fluctuation in their systems.

4:38

Either way, you now have a better idea

4:40

of what effect the actual medicine

4:42

candidate produces. The

4:45

best studies are double blind,

4:47

meaning that neither the researcher nor

4:50

the patient knows which pills are

4:52

which. That's to make sure that the researchers'

4:54

own words and attitudes don't

4:57

somehow give away whether a

4:59

patient is getting the good stuff or the sugar pill,

5:01

which they worry could skew the results. But

5:05

in the last couple of decades, science

5:07

has started to realize, well, wait

5:09

a minute, why are we subtracting out

5:12

the placebo effect? If it's

5:14

producing positive outcomes in patients,

5:16

maybe we should consider treating them with

5:19

placebos. Like if it's providing

5:21

relief, could we actually harness that

5:24

effect as well, potentially reducing

5:26

the doses of drugs that we give to

5:28

people?

5:29

What's crazy is that the placebo effect

5:31

is not just some woo woo, if

5:33

you dream it, it will come true kind

5:36

of thing. There are measurable biological

5:38

effects.

5:39

There's been one study after another. Most

5:42

of them have to do with pain.

5:44

One of my favorites was that if you give

5:46

people morphine and you

5:48

give it through the IV drip, so it's kind of surreptitiously

5:51

delivered, that produces much less

5:54

pain relief than if you give morphine

5:57

in front of the patient with the doctor telling

5:59

them what they're doing.

5:59

doing. In one study, researchers

6:02

at Columbia and Stanford gave students

6:05

free bottles of a new energy drink that

6:07

contained 200 milligrams of caffeine,

6:10

two and a half times as much as what's in

6:12

a red bowl.

6:14

Or at least that's what they told the students.

6:17

I mean, it was just a bottle of pure water. But

6:20

it was turned into a placebo for

6:22

this kind of energy drink that was meant to leave you

6:24

feeling more alert and, you know, kind

6:26

of energized. And that's indeed

6:28

what they found. It actually produced a change in

6:31

blood pressure and, you know, feelings of

6:33

alertness that just didn't come, obviously,

6:35

when you just drink a glass of tap

6:38

water. Wait, blood pressure?

6:40

See that's another one of those examples where

6:42

it crosses over from the woo woo into the

6:44

physiological. Yeah, exactly.

6:46

In all these ways, there are

6:48

objective measurable changes.

6:51

It's not just the patient self-reporting.

6:54

And it's not just fake medicines, by the

6:56

way.

6:57

There's even been tests of fake

6:59

surgery. You mentioned one study

7:01

in the book where doctors

7:05

would go in to put a stent in

7:07

during heart surgery,

7:09

but not actually put one. Yeah,

7:11

that's right. The surgeon did actually kind of

7:13

make a cut in their skin and, you know, like perform the

7:15

actions as if they were delivering the stent.

7:17

It's just there was no stent attached to

7:19

the catheter that they were inserting. The

7:21

patient fully believed they were they could

7:24

have been receiving the stent. It was quite

7:26

a big group sample of patients.

7:28

They found no statistically significant difference

7:31

in their symptoms. And I should I should

7:33

emphasize actually that this was a very particular

7:36

use of the stent. So it was for treating

7:39

angina. So, you know, pain

7:41

in the chest. I'm not saying that doctors don't

7:43

need to use stents at all. But for the treatment of

7:45

angina, it certainly seemed that there was no

7:47

benefit of having the stent over just

7:50

having the placebo surgery. Wow. Well,

7:52

the whole thing starts to make you wonder how

7:55

much of real medicine

7:58

and surgery is benefiting

8:01

from the placebo effect. Like

8:03

when we buy Advil or

8:06

something, I wonder if the

8:08

fact that we expect will get better

8:10

from it

8:11

is boosting its actual medical

8:13

effect. Yeah, I think it is. And actually,

8:16

you know, drugs marketing is really important

8:18

in the size of the placebo

8:20

effect. You can find all kinds of painkillers,

8:23

some of them, you know, like with bright packaging

8:25

and, you know, telling you, will produce

8:27

like 10 times greater pain relief than

8:29

the average painkiller. And, you

8:31

know, what we know is that when you have all of that positive

8:34

reinforcement of the positive expectation,

8:36

then it really is more powerful. And

8:39

you're saying,

8:40

comparing a drug that has 200 milligrams

8:44

of ibuprofen versus a generic

8:46

one that also has 200 milligrams of ibuprofen.

8:49

That's exactly it. Yeah, you're getting

8:52

the exact same chemical. It's just the way it's

8:54

presented.

8:55

It gets even weirder. It turns

8:57

out that bigger fake pills

9:00

produce a more dramatic effect than smaller

9:03

fake pills.

9:04

And even the color matters.

9:07

Yeah, all kinds of these things seem

9:09

to make a difference. You know, if say

9:11

you're trying to receive like a kind

9:14

of tranquilizing drug to reduce anxiety,

9:17

that actually blue pills seem to be more effective

9:19

in that case than if you have a red pill because

9:22

we associate blue with a kind of calmness.

9:25

That's crazy. Do you

9:27

think

9:28

modern pharmaceutical companies know

9:30

that as they decide how to design

9:32

their pills? I expect they probably do. And

9:35

you know, that alone is enough

9:37

reason for them to change the color of the pill. We

9:39

also know that fake capsules have

9:42

a stronger beneficial effect than fake pills

9:45

and fake injections have the biggest

9:47

effect of all.

9:49

Now,

9:50

to be clear, this stuff isn't

9:52

magic. Placebos work on

9:54

some ailments much better than others. They're

9:57

amazing for pain and do a

9:59

good job on...

9:59

insomnia, depression, anxiety,

10:02

and problems with peeing and pooping.

10:05

But they can't shrink tumors, they

10:07

don't lower cholesterol, they don't bring down

10:10

high blood pressure, they can't cure

10:12

malaria, they can't

10:13

make a wound heal faster. The

10:15

placebo effect is primarily

10:18

reducing symptoms. People

10:20

say, you know, medicine's really into the underlying pathophysiology.

10:23

We get rid of this chemical, fix

10:25

this organ that has a word, just to repair

10:28

trauma. But actually, patients'

10:31

symptoms are really important. Ted

10:33

Kappchuk is a professor of medicine at

10:35

Harvard, where he's the director of the program

10:38

in placebo studies.

10:40

And what do they do there? We do nothing

10:42

but placebo research. We try to

10:44

find out how we can amplify the placebos, make them

10:47

smaller, make the effects higher or smaller.

10:49

Few people have been working as hard, for

10:52

such a long time, to advance the

10:54

idea of placebos as

10:56

treatment.

10:57

So let me tell you a very

10:59

simple example of a placebo effect.

11:02

I could pick headaches,

11:04

I could pick irritable bowel syndrome,

11:07

chronic pains in the belly. But

11:09

let's start with a low back pain. You fall down,

11:12

hurt yourself, get an injury, trauma

11:14

in your back, you're hurt.

11:17

You're hurt, so the doctor prescribes aspirin

11:20

or Advil. Maybe try some physical

11:22

therapy. And in time, the

11:24

injury heals, and the pain stops.

11:27

For most people. But for

11:30

many people, 50 to 100 million

11:32

Americans, the

11:35

brain doesn't shut off the single.

11:37

It keeps firing in the brain. That's

11:40

what most chronic pain is. Your nerves

11:43

change their functioning structure, and

11:46

they keep firing, telling you you're in pain.

11:48

Because we do know, when you respond to placebos,

11:51

endorphins, dopamine,

11:53

cannabinoids are released.

11:59

treat it like opioids or

12:02

use the same pathways as the placebo

12:04

effect. So you're saying that the symptom

12:06

of pain continues long after

12:08

the actual injury. Right. The brain

12:11

is misfiring. It's a false alarm. That's

12:14

very, very common. 15 to 20%

12:17

of every medical kind, especially, has patients

12:19

coming in saying,

12:20

you gave me this drug, I did the surgery and

12:23

I'm still in the same pain. And doctors, what

12:25

they do, they send them to psychologists. And it's not

12:27

a psychological problem. Most of the time

12:30

it's a question of how the nerves work.

12:33

A lot of people haven't quite up to that explanation.

12:36

What Capchuck has discovered after 30 years

12:39

of research is that there's a lot more

12:41

to the placebo effect than the pill

12:43

itself. Well, first of all, it's

12:45

not a pill, the effect of a pill, because

12:48

it has no effect. It's everything that surrounds

12:50

the pill. The ritual, the symbols,

12:53

the acts of kindness, the

12:56

smells, bells and whistles of a clinical

12:59

interaction.

12:59

That's really an incredible

13:02

important drama of every person who

13:04

seriously wants to get healing. If

13:06

you need any proof that the doctor's attention

13:09

really makes a difference, consider one

13:11

of Capchuck's most famous studies.

13:13

It was a study of 262 people

13:16

with irritable bowel syndrome, IBS.

13:20

It's like a perpetual stomach ache accompanied

13:22

by various pooping problems. It's

13:25

a really nice study. It's one of my more well-known

13:27

studies, but it was published in British Medical

13:30

Journal 2008. He divided these

13:32

people into three groups, or

13:35

as they call these groupings in the clinical trial,

13:37

biz arms, three arms.

13:40

We randomized 260 patients

13:43

to three arms. One was

13:45

no treatment control. The

13:47

second arm was it's a needle

13:49

that looks like an acupuncture. It is

13:51

an acupuncture needle. It goes in and

13:54

patient feels it. It stands there straight

13:57

up, but in fact, it's a magic sword.

13:59

The needle goes up the shaft. You can't tell

14:02

the difference between it and real acupuncture. Oh,

14:04

man. Fake acupuncture. This

14:07

second group had no doctor-patient

14:09

relationship at all.

14:10

An acupuncturist breezed in, asked

14:13

if the patient was comfortable, and breezed out. But

14:16

then there was the third group.

14:18

Now you also got the fake needle, but then the

14:20

doctor, the acupuncturist would say,

14:22

so I've read your chart. I have a good

14:24

idea what's going on, but I want to hear in your

14:27

own words what is

14:29

going on. How does this affect

14:31

you? What symptoms are the worst? What makes it better?

14:34

How does it make you feel? What things can you do

14:36

that you

14:37

still can do? What things do you can't do? And why

14:39

do you think it's the core? A real schmaltzy relationship.

14:42

It was a little probably over the top. The article

14:44

about the study that I read, it said that in

14:46

the third group, practitioners were required

14:49

to touch the hands or shoulders of members

14:51

of the third group and spend at least 20 seconds

14:54

lost in thoughtful silence.

14:57

You know, it was great. We went over the top. By

14:59

this incredible result, as

15:02

good as it could get, where the

15:05

intense doctor-patient relationship, 60%

15:09

of the people got better. If

15:12

they had only the

15:14

paraphernalia fake acupuncture,

15:16

about 40% got better. And those

15:19

people who are in no treatment control, they

15:21

had 27% improvement. Time

15:23

heals. And I would say that it's

15:26

clear that patient-doctor relation can

15:28

modulate placebo effects

15:30

from that study. It's so mind-blowing. By

15:32

a third, by a third better. It's really amazing.

15:35

So this seems like a valid

15:38

study. The numbers were big enough. It was well-designed.

15:41

Why didn't that instantly translate into

15:44

a universe of doctors who do not

15:46

rush you through the office?

15:48

Two reasons. The evidence wasn't good enough, but

15:50

the real evidence is doctors don't mostly

15:52

care about placebo effects. I

15:54

teach in a medical school. The students get

15:57

taught

15:58

that if it's not more than a... placebo,

16:00

it's worthless,

16:01

right? The placebo is junk.

16:03

I mean, that's what you get taught when you're studying the development

16:05

of pharmaceuticals and devices

16:08

and surgery even. So it's

16:10

always been tending to be marginalized placebo.

16:14

The other reason doctors don't care about

16:16

it is that placebo

16:18

is tainted with deception and trickery

16:21

that you have to conceal or deceive

16:24

the patient and make them think it was

16:26

a real drug or a real intervention. So

16:29

I study with the greats

16:32

and people that my predecessors,

16:34

my teachers, and everyone

16:36

believes that. And here's where

16:38

things get really nuts. Ted

16:41

Kaptchuk is the first man who ever ran

16:43

a study to answer the question, would

16:46

a placebo still work if

16:48

you knew you were getting a placebo?

16:51

After the ad break, I'll

16:53

tell you how he tracked the answer down. Casey

17:00

Shane was murdered in the middle of an August night, shot

17:02

point blank while idling in his Dodge pickup truck in

17:05

North Indianapolis. There was

17:07

no physical evidence, no known motive, and

17:10

no one coming forward with information.

17:12

Except one woman who swears to this day she

17:15

saw Leon Detroit Benson pull the

17:17

trigger. Leon Benson was

17:19

sentenced to 60 years in prison, all

17:21

because one person swore they saw something.

17:24

But what if she was wrong? And

17:26

what if we could prove that she was wrong? From

17:29

Wondery and Campside Media

17:30

comes season three of the

17:32

hit podcast Suspect, co-hosted

17:35

by me, Matt Cher, alongside

17:37

attorney, Laura Bazalon. This

17:39

is a story of a botched police investigation,

17:42

the dangers of shaky eyewitness testimony,

17:44

and a community who feared law enforcement,

17:47

with good reason. Listen to Suspect,

17:50

five shots in the dark, wherever

17:52

you get your podcasts, or binge all

17:54

eight episodes ad free on Wondery

17:56

Plus. Find Wondery Plus in the Wondery app,

17:59

or on Apple.

18:02

We've been talking to Ted Kapschuck, who

18:05

runs a Harvard group dedicated to turning

18:07

the placebo effect into an actual

18:09

medical treatment. But remember,

18:11

most doctors have encountered placebos

18:14

only in the context of studies, where

18:17

you're giving half the patient's real medicine and

18:19

the other half a fake pill that has no active

18:22

ingredients. And in those double-blind

18:24

studies, they tell you that you

18:26

may be getting a placebo.

18:28

You could never give a placebo pill

18:30

to a sick person and pretend

18:32

that it's medicine. You can't lie

18:34

to them, first do no harm, right?

18:37

Informed consent and all that stuff. That

18:39

would be really unethical. And

18:42

so, as a result, few doctors give

18:44

a placebo to patients and claim that it's

18:47

real medicine. Well, few

18:49

doctors admit to it. So

18:51

Kapschuck had what may sound like the craziest

18:54

idea in all of crazy

18:56

plesibology. What

18:58

would happen if we gave patients a placebo

19:01

and told them it was a placebo?

19:04

They said, let's try this. Let's

19:07

do an experiment

19:09

where we tell people it's placebo.

19:11

We have to try it. He approached the

19:13

gastroenterologist he'd worked with on a recent

19:15

experiment. And I said, Tony, I

19:18

got to do this. I got to do this.

19:20

I want to give patients a placebo and tell them it's

19:23

placebo. And he was great. He

19:25

said, Ted, the

19:26

craziest thing I've heard. I'm on board. And

19:28

I said, we have to get a grant. And he said, you're never going

19:30

to get a grant. And he said, Ted, can

19:32

you recruit the patients and pay for parking? I

19:35

said, yeah, I'll do the work for free

19:37

and we'll sneak them into the research part

19:39

of the hospital and not have to

19:41

pay the fine. And we came out there.

19:44

It actually worked pretty good. 60% of

19:47

people got better on the open label, plus

19:49

usual care. 30% got better

19:51

without an extra treatment. That

19:54

blew us all the way. I don't know. It's really

19:56

hard to believe. In other words, compared

19:58

with the people who got. no treatment at all,

20:01

twice as many people got better when they'd taken

20:03

a placebo that they knew had no

20:06

active ingredients. What Cap Chuck calls

20:08

an open label placebo

20:10

or honest placebo.

20:12

But what about people who got an honest placebo

20:15

versus people who got

20:17

dishonest placebos, pills

20:19

they thought might contain actual medicine

20:21

in those double blind trials?

20:24

Yeah, we've compared them directly many times,

20:26

several times. There's no difference.

20:28

I've been published for great studies

20:31

with 300 people, an earlobal,

20:33

and there's no difference to double blind, no label. It's

20:36

so hard to believe. There have been now over

20:38

a thousand patients randomized to this kind of

20:40

thing in different conditions like low back

20:42

pain, migraine headache, knee

20:44

osteoarthritis, all kinds of

20:46

pain conditions, and also lots of non pain conditions.

20:49

Cancer related fatigue, it's been done three times

20:51

now. Perimenopausal hot flashes. So

20:55

there's a lot of evidence for it. It's

20:58

hard to believe, but I guess it's

21:00

because the nerves

21:03

are in a context of healing. It happens

21:05

automatically. It's like

21:07

the rituals, the bells and whistles,

21:09

the doctor. It's not a mind cure.

21:12

It's actually the nervous system regulates

21:14

itself. It's much smarter than the mind.

21:17

The non-conscious world

21:19

of our being is what's doing it. And

21:22

that's really a breakthrough. But it occurs

21:24

to me that there would be no side effects either. No,

21:27

in person, honestly, placebo has no side

21:29

effects. When I was a kid and I found out placebo

21:31

effect,

21:33

about the same time I found out about

21:36

this effect that the placebo

21:38

effect still works if you

21:40

know it's a placebo. Yeah. You're

21:42

the guy who figured that out?

21:44

Yeah, that's my first experiment and I've done 15

21:46

of them since then.

21:48

That was you? That

21:50

has been a cornerstone of my cocktail

21:53

party conversations. I really want to take

21:55

the taint out of placebo, the

21:57

idea that you need deception in some ways.

21:59

And this healing rituals

22:02

tricks our minds to turn down

22:05

the amplified pain sensation.

22:08

It turns down the false alarm.

22:10

Absolutely incredible. So do we have any

22:12

idea what the physiological

22:15

explanation is for that? Oh, thank

22:17

you. Thank you. Thank you, that's a really important

22:19

question to ask. We don't 100%

22:21

know everything what's going on there, but

22:25

the simplest explanation is that

22:27

the nervous system amplifies symptoms,

22:30

not only pain,

22:32

and sometimes it gets stuck there and up

22:34

in the brain. The

22:37

pathways that make the pain light

22:40

up, the false alarms, are the same pathway

22:42

that the placebo sometimes

22:45

in some people turned down.

22:47

We know for a long

22:49

time that for double blinds and deceptive

22:52

placebo, neurotransmitters were

22:54

involved like endorphin.

22:56

If you respond to placebo, there's a release

22:59

of maybe endorphins, cannabinoids,

23:01

dopamine. We now know, recent

23:03

experiments tell us that even with open label, you

23:05

get the endorphins involved, right?

23:07

So we know that in many cases,

23:10

after you take a fake pill, your

23:12

brain releases real chemicals, which

23:15

produce genuine improvements in your

23:17

symptoms. We know that the placebo

23:20

effect gets magnified if you get a lot

23:22

of attention from the doctor. Schmaltz,

23:25

as Gaptchuk calls it. Okay,

23:27

so in three groups in general,

23:29

a group that gets no care might

23:32

get less pain over time just because

23:34

it fades.

23:36

Placebo effect with no Schmaltz

23:39

does better than that, but

23:41

a placebo pill plus Schmaltz

23:44

will get a bigger placebo effect.

23:47

Now, remember

23:48

our author friend, David Robson? I'm

23:51

David Robson. I'm 35 years old. I'm

23:54

the author of The Expectation Effect.

23:57

There are actually only two chapters in his book

23:59

about the

23:59

placebo effect. To him, the

24:02

placebo effect is only one form

24:05

of the expectation effect. Placebos

24:08

are a subset of the effect that

24:10

expectations can have on your life. One

24:13

of the best examples concerns exercise.

24:16

They gave these students a genetic

24:18

test for a variant that's known to affect

24:21

your kind of capacity for endurance

24:23

exercise. If you have one variant

24:26

of the CREV1 gene, it seems

24:28

that you are a bit better

24:29

able to do endurance exercise and that's reflected

24:32

in physiological measures like the gas

24:34

exchange within the lungs. So they gave

24:37

these students this genetic test, but

24:39

then they gave them sham feedback. So

24:41

the students didn't initially find

24:44

out what variant they had. And

24:46

what the researchers found was that those expectations

24:49

alone, independent of the

24:51

genes they were carrying, influenced

24:54

their performance in this endurance exercise.

24:57

And in some cases, the influence

25:00

of the expectations was actually greater

25:02

than the influence of that gene.

25:04

So the gas exchange within

25:07

the lungs, the expectations were a bit

25:09

more powerful. So if I told

25:11

you, oh, lucky you, you've got the gene,

25:14

you'll run better, longer. You believe

25:17

it and you do?

25:19

Yeah, exactly. That's what they found. And,

25:21

you know, it also affected feelings of how hard they

25:23

were working out. If you thought you had the good

25:25

gene, you could be really like going for it on the

25:27

treadmill, but it didn't feel like so

25:30

exhausting. So I think it speaks more

25:32

broadly to, you know, the narratives we carry

25:34

around us. Like if you've always just assumed

25:37

that you just aren't cut out for exercise,

25:39

maybe because of, you know, what you see in your family members,

25:41

maybe because of, you know, memories from

25:44

high school of not really enjoying gym class.

25:46

Well, that is actually going to have a similar

25:48

kind of expectation

25:49

effect on you. And

25:51

then there's the aging study performed

25:53

in 2002 by Becca Levy at

25:55

Yale.

25:56

So that's the one that really blew my mind. She

25:59

found that people... People's kind of self-reported

26:01

expectations of aging, you

26:03

know, midlife seemed to predict their

26:05

longevity by seven and a half

26:08

years. So if they thought that things

26:10

would get better of age, they lived longer. If

26:12

they expected that things would automatically get

26:14

worse with age, they lived seven

26:16

and a half years less than

26:19

the other people. So a huge effect.

26:21

That includes getting the various diseases

26:23

you can get when you're older too. Here's

26:26

Becca Levy herself in an

26:28

American Medical Association video. We

26:30

were able to look at people who had the risky

26:33

gene for developing dementia.

26:36

And we were able to look at people who were free of dementia

26:38

at baseline. And then we followed them over

26:41

six years to see whether they

26:42

developed dementia. And we found that even

26:44

in this high risk genetic group, if

26:46

they take in more positive age beliefs,

26:49

they actually have, we found had a 40% reduced

26:52

risk of developing dementia.

26:55

And their risk of developing dementia was as low

26:57

as people who were

26:59

not born with that risky gene.

27:01

There's a really big body of evidence

27:03

that's built up over the last 20 years

27:05

that shows this to be the case. So the

27:08

big question is, well, how could that happen?

27:10

Well, one explanation is that if you

27:12

think things get better with age, you

27:15

take better care of yourself.

27:17

But Robson says there's more to it than that.

27:20

The expectation effect messes with your

27:22

release of damaging stress

27:24

hormones.

27:25

So what you see is that people

27:27

who have the negative expectations of aging,

27:30

they start to feel a

27:32

lot more vulnerable.

27:33

And they're going to see the challenges around

27:36

them, even something like going to post a letter,

27:39

you know, going to the store

27:41

to get you groceries, you're going to start

27:43

worrying about, you know, losing your way

27:45

or, you know, having a fall. And that sense

27:48

of vulnerability increases the

27:50

kind of stress that they're feeling. So you see

27:52

for these people, a steady rise

27:54

in the stress hormone cortisol. And

27:57

again, that then causes a steady rise

27:59

in inflammation. which we know causes

28:01

bodily wear and tear.

28:03

And over time, the

28:05

consequences add up and

28:08

it just puts you at a higher risk of all of these

28:10

different illnesses associated with aging and

28:12

eventually your mortality. Well, what

28:14

should we do with that information? Should

28:17

medical science

28:19

at every checkup tell

28:21

you getting older is nothing to worry about?

28:24

Right. I think we do need to take action. And you

28:27

know, Bekelevi in her first paper was that if

28:29

we found that there was some kind of virus that

28:31

was reducing people's longevity

28:33

by seven and a half years, we would be taking action.

28:36

But actually what she argued is that,

28:38

you know, the ageism that permeates

28:40

our culture is, you know, is

28:43

a pathogen, it is having that effect.

28:45

As a society, we should be fighting ageism

28:48

a lot more fervently than we do at the

28:50

moment. So we need to stop reinforcing

28:53

this message that as you get older,

28:55

is automatically a time of vulnerability

28:58

and decline. So I think that's a no

29:00

brainer really is that we have to kind of, you

29:02

know, be a bit more careful about kind of what messages we're

29:04

sending to other people and ultimately what

29:07

messages we're internalizing ourselves.

29:10

You know, I watched a couple of Bekelevi's videos.

29:13

At one point, she suggests keeping

29:15

an ageism journal.

29:17

What that involves is for one week, writing down

29:20

all the messages about aging that you encounter,

29:22

whether it be in social media, whether

29:25

it be in a magazine,

29:27

seeing advertisements, whether it be, you know,

29:29

talking to a relative or

29:31

overhearing a conversation in a coffee shop, write

29:34

it down and then write it down whether it's positive

29:37

or negative.

29:37

And if it's negative, take a moment

29:39

and think, could there have been a different portrayal

29:41

of that older person? You know, it's true.

29:44

Think about it. It's no longer cool to make

29:46

fun of people's looks or race or

29:49

disabilities. But for some

29:51

reason, making fun of old

29:53

people is still fair game.

29:55

The conversations I had with my dad at the end of his life

29:57

were the same ones we had when I was about six. The

30:00

rolls just flip around. We

30:03

would go out to eat, walk into a restaurant.

30:05

I would look at my dad and go, shhh, listen to

30:07

our hair.

30:10

Don't touch anything. Don't

30:13

talk to anybody. I'll be back in a minute, alright? Wear

30:17

your shoes.

30:20

That's comedian David Dyer on YouTube.

30:23

Sure makes me look forward to getting old.

30:27

Anyway, so what have we learned? The

30:30

placebo effect is real, it's

30:32

measurable, it produces physical

30:34

changes in your body, and it's freaking

30:37

weird. Placebos even

30:39

work when you know you're getting them.

30:41

Even though you know they can't

30:43

work on you, they work on you. If

30:46

you ask Harvard's

30:47

Ted Kaptchuk, modern medicine

30:49

is just ignoring a vast

30:51

realm of potential treatments that

30:54

could be helping people right now.

30:56

He believes that placebos shouldn't just

30:58

be a nuisance variable in medical trials.

31:01

Placebos should be considered treatments

31:03

in their own right. They

31:05

work. But making them mainstream

31:08

will be an uphill battle. But

31:10

it hasn't caught on, and I've asked many

31:12

of my colleagues who've been doctors in my

31:15

studies, I said, what are you using when you thought these great

31:17

results? It's not standard care.

31:20

You know, you can train in medical school to

31:22

do placebos. You would train not to give placebos.

31:25

I'd give them a break, and it's

31:27

going to take a shift. But you know what? I never stopped

31:29

fighting.

31:30

So, we'll see where it goes.

31:41

In

31:55

Schuster and CBS Sunday Morning. And

31:57

it's produced by PRX Productions. For

32:00

Simon & Schuster, the executive producers are

32:02

Richard Rohrer and Chris Lynch. The PRX

32:05

production team is Jocelyn Gonzalez, Morgan

32:07

Flannery, Pedro Rafael Rosado,

32:10

and Morgan Church. Jesse Nelson

32:12

composed the Unsung Science theme music, our

32:15

fact checker is Christina Ribello, and

32:17

Olivia Noble fixed the transcripts.

32:20

For more of my stuff, visit DavidPogue.com

32:23

or follow me on Twitter at Pogue. That's

32:26

P-O-G-U-E. We'd

32:28

love it if you'd like and follow

32:29

Unsung Science wherever you get your

32:32

podcasts. And spread the

32:34

word, will you?

32:39

Hey, Prime members, you can

32:42

listen to Unsung Science ad-free

32:44

on Amazon Music.

32:45

Download the Amazon Music app today.

32:48

Or you can listen ad-free with Wondery

32:50

Plus in Apple Podcasts. But

32:52

before you go, tell us about yourself by completing

32:54

a short survey at wondery.com slash

32:57

survey.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features