Podchaser Logo
Home
Pushkin Hosts Celebrate World Happiness Day

Pushkin Hosts Celebrate World Happiness Day

Released Wednesday, 20th March 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Pushkin Hosts Celebrate World Happiness Day

Pushkin Hosts Celebrate World Happiness Day

Pushkin Hosts Celebrate World Happiness Day

Pushkin Hosts Celebrate World Happiness Day

Wednesday, 20th March 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:15

Pushkin. Hello,

0:22

and if you're listening to this on March twentieth, Happy

0:24

World Happiness Day. On the

0:26

Happiness Lab, we suggest you do something to

0:28

improve your well being every day of the year,

0:31

but if the United Nations wants us all to make a

0:33

special effort for twenty four hours in March,

0:36

then we're on board with that too. The

0:39

first International Day of Happiness was celebrated

0:41

back in twenty thirteen. The goal was

0:43

to raise awareness that our well being can be approved

0:46

if only more governments enacted policies

0:48

to help us all become a little happier. On

0:51

each International Day of Happiness, the United

0:53

Nations also issues the World Happiness

0:55

Report, written by scientists and academics.

0:58

This report examines different themes, showing

1:00

what we're getting right when it comes to happiness and

1:02

what we still need to work on. Past

1:05

reports have looked at happiness and parenting, what

1:07

living in cities does for our happiness, and

1:09

more recently, the impact that COVID nineteen

1:12

has had on our well being. Over the

1:14

next few episodes of the Happiness Lab, we'll be talking

1:16

to the experts behind this year's World Happiness

1:18

Report. They're among the best and brightest

1:20

in the field of happiness science. So these

1:22

are going to be some fantastic episodes, But

1:25

for the show today, we're doing something a little

1:27

different. The Happiness Lab is made by

1:29

Pushkin Industries, and many of the network's

1:31

other hosts have some pretty interesting takes on what

1:34

can make us all happier, so I decided

1:36

to talk to them about what they would have put

1:38

in this year's World Happiness Report. A

1:40

little later, you'll hear from revisionist Histories

1:43

Malcolm Gladwell.

1:44

I'm perfectly happy to suffer, but

1:46

I will not suffer for six hours.

1:48

And from Tim Hartford from cautionary.

1:50

Tales, the surgeon would leave

1:52

the probe in, so to speak, without

1:54

wiggling it around.

1:55

But we'll kick it off with an old, old friend

1:57

of mine.

1:58

It's worth sharing with folks that I've

2:00

actually known you since I was seventeen years old.

2:03

I was a student of yours.

2:05

I was full eight years now.

2:07

Now it feels like it's been so how much longer?

2:10

This is Maya Schunker. I taught her back

2:12

when she was an undergraduate at Yale, and we

2:14

kept in touch after she graduated and went

2:16

to work at the White House, where she advised

2:18

the Obama administration on how behavioral

2:20

science can improve government policy.

2:23

These days, Maya hosts The Pushkin Show,

2:25

a slight Change of Plans, a podcast

2:27

about who we become when we face big challenges

2:29

and decisions. Given all that,

2:31

she was perhaps the perfect person to ask my

2:34

question, if you were writing a chapter

2:36

of the World Happiness Report, what

2:38

would it be about?

2:39

Okay, Well, this one's really easy for me because

2:42

I think there is one thing that erodes

2:45

my happiness more than anything else, and

2:48

it's what our psychologist's friend, Ethan

2:51

Cross calls mental chatter.

2:52

Oh yeah, so Ethan Cross, he's a professor at

2:54

the University of Michigan and he's the author of this wonderful

2:57

book Chatter. Yeah exactly.

2:59

And it was really helpful for me when I learned

3:01

about this concept because I was like, wow, Ethan,

3:03

you've just captured what's been in my brain

3:06

for decades. So, Laurie, can

3:08

you tell us more about what mental chatter

3:11

is and how does that relate to the inner

3:13

dialogue that we have in our minds all the time.

3:16

Yeah, well, let me start with the inner dialogue, because

3:18

in some ways it's a really cool thing that we do as

3:20

humans. So inner dialogue, just as

3:22

it sounds, is like the self

3:24

talk that you have going on in your head, and it could

3:26

be about all kinds of things. Right, Our inner dialogue

3:28

is how we like make sense of the world

3:31

and build our own inner narrative. Our inner

3:33

dialogue is how we like plan for what

3:35

we're going to do after this. You know, when I was waiting for you

3:37

to hop on zoom, I'm like, oh, after this, I'm gonna

3:39

make dinner. And what do I have in my fridge? And I oh,

3:41

I have some black beans? Like all of

3:43

that is inner dialogue, right, But chatter,

3:46

as Ethan defines, it, is a little bit different.

3:48

It's when our inner dialogue goes

3:50

to the negative. Right, So it's that inner

3:52

voice of worry where you're thinking about the future

3:55

and feeling anxious about what's to come, or

3:57

that inner voice of rumination where you're

3:59

thinking about the past and beating yourself up for

4:01

something that you did do or that you didn't do, or

4:04

even just like our inner voice of self criticism

4:06

where we just kind of talk crap about ourselves

4:08

like all the time, no matter what's going on. And

4:11

so while our inner dialogue

4:13

itself can be really adaptive, mental

4:16

chatter is not. It kind of feels like crap,

4:18

And then there's lots of evidence that it affects our performance

4:20

negatively too.

4:22

I remember so when I had a conversation with

4:24

Ethan on a slight change of plans, it

4:26

was so helpful for me to even hear this

4:28

distinction, the distinction between the inner voice and

4:31

dialogue and mental chatter, because

4:33

I think what happens is in the throes of

4:35

chatter, you are so pissed

4:38

off at your brain. You're like, can you please stop?

4:41

You've been ruminating over this thing for you

4:43

three hours. You're not making any progress

4:45

at all, and you can really start to resent

4:48

your brain and resent the fact that it even has

4:50

this faculty. And so when

4:53

Ethan and I did more of this gratitude

4:55

moment together where we appreciated our inner

4:57

voice and to exactly your point, focused

4:59

on all the benefits that that voice

5:02

affords us in any given day, that

5:04

alone helped me have a different

5:06

relationship with my mental chatter, because at the end

5:08

of the I thought, well, I wouldn't want to do away with my inner

5:11

voice altogether. I mean, it's actually miraculous

5:13

that I can travel in time to the future

5:15

or the past in general. I mean, I might not like it in

5:17

this moment because I'm perseparating about something

5:19

even happened two weeks ago that I can no longer change.

5:22

But in general, it's such a cool feature

5:24

of our cognition, of human cognition, that

5:26

we have the ability to have these internal

5:29

conversations with ourselves.

5:30

And I think the beauty is that once you understand what chatter

5:33

is, you can also find strategies for controlling

5:35

it when it goes to the not so great

5:38

side, right, And that's the lovely thing about Ethan's work, because

5:40

he has all these different strategies that we can use to

5:42

like not shut our chatter up, but to use

5:45

self talk to be a little bit more productive and a

5:47

little bit kinder to ourselves.

5:49

So let's talk about some of those strategies for those

5:51

who are in the horrible

5:53

loop of mental rumination. I

5:55

want to give folks hope and help them

5:57

see that there is light at the end of the tunnel.

6:00

Well, one of Ethan's best strategies that I

6:02

love because it's like so super simple,

6:04

and in fact, there's lots of evidence that when you use this strategy,

6:06

it doesn't take any cognitive work. It happens super

6:09

fast. And that strategy is what he calls

6:11

distance self talk, which is just the simple

6:13

act of using your name and talking

6:15

to yourself in the third person. So normally,

6:18

if I'm thinking about my own like mental chatter,

6:20

I'll be using the first person. I'll be like, oh, why

6:22

did I do that? I said that stupid thing, like I should

6:24

have thought more. But it's I I

6:27

me, me, me me, right, that's a

6:29

first person perspective, and that's what we

6:31

often use when we get like all worrying and

6:33

ruminative, because it's all about us. But

6:35

distance self talk lets you get a little bit of psychological

6:38

distance because instead of talking like that, you

6:40

say, you know, maybe you messed up a little

6:42

bit, Laurie, like, maybe this is something that you need to think

6:44

about in the future. Right, So I'm using

6:46

the second person you, I'm using the third person,

6:49

like my name. And that is really

6:51

powerful because the only time in your life

6:53

you ever hear the second person you or your name

6:55

is when somebody else is talking to you. And

6:57

so it's this little cool linguistic device

6:59

that makes us feel like we're

7:01

hearing from some wise mentor we're hearing

7:03

from some other person, somebody who is distance from

7:05

that loop of chatter that we have going on. And

7:08

Ethan's found that this simple act of doing that you don't even

7:10

have to instruct subjects to like talk differently

7:12

to themselves. Just the act of switching

7:14

the pronouns that you use in your brain winds

7:17

up making you a little bit kinder to yourself.

7:19

Has all these wonderful emotional consequences

7:21

where you're a little bit anxious over time,

7:23

and it just lets you kind of get out of that loop so

7:25

that you can perform better. And what's cool is like

7:28

it doesn't take any work. It's just a matter of

7:30

changing the pronouns. It's not like developing

7:32

some complicated like cognitive

7:34

behavior therapy strategy. It's just like

7:36

you switch the pronouns and immediately you get this interesting

7:39

distance from your normal chatter.

7:41

Yeah. I remember reading in Ethan's book that Malala

7:43

did this. Of course, Malala the most

7:45

sophisticated psychologist of all time despite

7:48

never having studied it, because she's such a such

7:50

a genius. I really

7:52

love the fly on the wall perspective

7:55

because when I think about how I

7:57

counsel my friends or family members,

8:00

there's a certain objectivity that I feel I have

8:02

in that moment where I can see the situation

8:04

from a distance. The kind of hormonal

8:07

fog is removed, all of those heated emotions

8:09

are removed from whatever advice I'm giving,

8:11

And it feels so useful when

8:14

you're diagnosing your own problems to have that

8:16

objectivity right, to be able to look at it as

8:18

more of an impartial observer the distance.

8:20

Self talk is one way to do that, but even finds you can

8:23

also do that like literally taking the perspective

8:25

of a distanced observer. You know what would

8:27

Beyonce do? Sort of strategy where you

8:30

just say, you know, oh my gosh, I said that

8:32

thing, like that's so terrible. Well, what would

8:34

Beyonce do? Imagine not Beyonce? How would

8:36

I react to having said that? Like, I wouldn't care. I'd

8:38

be Beyonce right, Which sounds silly,

8:40

but the evidence really shows that, like taking this third

8:43

person perspective, like pretending

8:45

that you're somebody else, and especially somebody else who

8:47

has exactly the skills to deal with whatever

8:49

situation you're facing, all of a sudden,

8:51

like you wind up performing much better,

8:53

being less anxious, and you can just kind of shut up

8:56

the chatter because you kind of take on this other perspective.

8:58

My favorite part of it what would Beyonce do? Is

9:01

it turns out Beyonce herself uses a strategy

9:03

I guess whenever she's like feeling nervous

9:05

before shows, she has this persona that she calls

9:08

Sasha Fears where she's like, I'm gonna harness

9:10

Sasha Fierce, and then she pretends she's Sasha Fierce

9:12

and she just like goes out there and you know, does her

9:14

Beyonce thing. So what would Beyonce

9:16

do? Beyonce would use this form of distant

9:18

self talk where you pretend that you're somebody

9:20

cooler and wiser.

9:22

I mean to imagine someone cooler than Beyonce. But

9:24

fine, I guess she needs a different reference

9:26

point. I'm curious to know

9:28

what you think the mechanism is at play. Do you think

9:30

it's because we are better

9:32

at giving other people

9:35

advice than we are ourselves, or do you think it's that we're

9:37

better at following other people's

9:40

advice.

9:40

Yeah, my guess is that it's a combination of the two.

9:42

Right when we start using second person

9:45

pronouns like you know, hey, you need to do

9:47

a little bit better. I know that you know this has

9:49

been hard, but you da da da da, Like

9:51

I think we we rarely do that in like a mean

9:53

way, like you suck and you're terrible,

9:55

right, Like, that's just kind of not what most of us normally

9:58

do. So when we apply that pronoun you to ourselves,

10:00

I think it naturally makes us a little bit nicer.

10:03

So it means the advice we're giving ourselves feels

10:05

nicer. But I think hearing that self

10:08

talk involving you and third person like

10:10

you, Laura, you know, here's what you can do, all

10:12

of a sudden, it gets us out of that like mental chatter

10:14

frame where we're just talking to ourself and it

10:17

kind of feels like we're hearing advice from somebody

10:19

else. I think we both give advice differently, but

10:21

we're more like we resonate with that advice

10:23

differently too, We kind of hear it in a different way,

10:25

so it's like both parts wind up making us

10:28

feel better and perform better.

10:29

One of my favorite strategies that I use when

10:32

I am in the throes of mental chatter is

10:34

temporal distancing. Can you share a bit more

10:36

about what that is?

10:37

Yeah, So That is a strategy where you pretend

10:39

that you yourself are in the future thinking

10:42

about whatever ye it is that you're ruminating about

10:44

right now. Then I'll think, Okay, how

10:46

is ten years from now Lari going to think about that?

10:49

And I'm like, oh, she's not going to care

10:51

about that at all? Right, Like my emotions kind

10:53

of go down because it doesn't feel like it's that scary

10:55

anymore. But also, ten years from now, Luria

10:57

is going to think about that incident in a totally different way.

10:59

She's going to say, oh, I learned

11:01

something from that. And so this is this

11:04

strategy of temporal distancing. You think

11:06

about yourself in the future, how they

11:08

would think about that this incident, And usually

11:10

when they think about it, they're in a different mode than

11:12

you are. They're not like feeling all anxious and ruminative

11:14

about something. They're thinking from the

11:16

perspective of this wise future observer

11:19

who wants to go through hard things, who wants to

11:21

grow from them, who's thinking more in terms of

11:23

what they're going to learn rather than how it feels right

11:25

now.

11:26

So there have been a few times where temporal

11:28

distancing has failed for me, and

11:31

those are in moments where I'm sitting

11:33

there ruminating, and I imagine five

11:35

years from now, Maya, ten years from now Maya,

11:37

and I think to myself, I'm going to be worried about the same

11:40

damn thing even then. So for

11:42

those of you who are listening who feel like

11:44

they're very neurotic in this way, you're not alone.

11:48

I'm with you, and so I want to share what

11:50

I do in those moments. So what I do is

11:52

I think back to my past and I

11:54

try to think about some topic

11:56

that sees my brain that I was absolutely

11:59

convinced was going to plague me forever,

12:02

and then to look at my present self and to say,

12:04

huh, you're not actually worried about that

12:06

issue that you thought in college was

12:08

the biggest ever. And so sometimes

12:11

collecting personal evidence from your

12:13

own life that you were just wrong, you

12:15

misfecasted the impact that a particular

12:17

topic was going to have on you, can give

12:19

you the confidence that the current thing will

12:22

actually resolve in your brain over the next five

12:24

or ten years.

12:25

I love that because I do sometimes with that kind

12:27

of temporal distancing strategy, like I feel

12:29

like sometimes I'm so caught up in the moment with my chatter

12:31

that I'm like, oh, yeah, ten years from now, Laura, she's

12:33

going to be just as freaked out about this tiny

12:35

thing. But then when you look back, you're like, oh, yeah,

12:38

I guess I was wrong about those other ones, so maybe

12:40

I'm wrong about this one too.

12:41

So Laurie, what are other strategies that we can use

12:43

to distance ourselves from that chatter?

12:45

Well, other strategies come from somebody else we've had

12:48

on the Happiness Lab, Krista Naff, who really

12:50

talks about how we need to shut up the critical

12:52

side of our self talk voice. And this

12:54

is something that I think I've seen in my students

12:56

so much, right, Like, I think my students just are

12:59

so hyper ambitious. They talk to themselves

13:01

in like such harsh ways. And I

13:03

think they do that not because they're massochists, because

13:05

they think it works, right. They just have this assumption

13:08

that this really critic voices what's going to

13:10

kind of get me off my butt and I'll actually

13:12

do stuff and get motivated to do, you know, and

13:14

achieve whatever goals I had in the first place.

13:16

But a lot of kristin Neff's evidence suggests

13:19

that that's absolutely not true. Self critical voice

13:21

winds up causing you to procrastinate and

13:23

it feels really terrible and you just don't get

13:25

done what you need to get done. And she's found

13:27

that there's a powerful alternative to this, which she

13:29

refers to as self compassion. Again,

13:32

you're kind of marshaling the compassion that you'd give

13:34

to somebody else for yourself.

13:36

You kind of give yourself the same kindness

13:38

that you would an outside observer, but just

13:40

to kind of make it concrete. She talks about self compassion

13:43

as having these three parts. The first part is kind

13:45

of mindfulness. You need to recognize this

13:47

sucks right now. I'm having hard time right now,

13:49

I have failed and I feel ashamed. So you're

13:52

mindful about your feelings, the situation,

13:55

how bad it is. You're kind of like calling like

13:57

the emotional spade a spade, like this sucks right

13:59

now. The second part is what she calls calmon humanity,

14:01

which I think is super powerful. It's basically

14:04

saying it's normal. I'm human,

14:06

I'm going to screw up, I'm going to go through shame, I'm

14:08

going to feel luck sometimes like this is normative,

14:10

right, It is common humanity to experience

14:12

these emotions that I'm experiencing. And

14:15

then the third step is the self kindness part,

14:17

kind of using the same strategies we were just talking

14:19

about with Ethan, where you talk to yourself ideally

14:21

using the sort of second and third person and say,

14:24

Lauri, what can you take off your plate? Lauri, how

14:26

can you be kind to yourself right now? And she

14:28

finds this self compassion is this like

14:30

super powerful strategy where it can do

14:33

things like not just improve your performance and make

14:35

you feel better, but also like reduce trauma

14:37

when individuals are in combat situations.

14:40

It can increase the compassion that you give to

14:42

your team members and your partner. Right,

14:44

so do you engage in self compassion? It

14:46

boosts your other people compassion too,

14:49

And it just like has this enormous

14:51

effect on people's performance where you

14:53

find that people stop procrastinating,

14:55

they stop being afraid of the kind of tasks that they

14:57

have ahead of them, they can just kind of embrace them with

15:00

excitement.

15:00

Yeah. I had so many misconceptions about

15:02

the self compassion literature until I

15:05

dug.

15:05

In totally because it has really crappy brandis

15:07

right, It sounds like very woo like self

15:09

compassion. It doesn't sound like human

15:12

performance maximization, but that's

15:14

like ultimately what it is.

15:15

Yeah, I mean, the minute I learned wait, self

15:17

compassion can actually improve performance, I mean,

15:19

then it just becomes a no brainer. It no longer feels

15:21

like this soft woo woo narrative instead

15:24

one that feels very productive and functional

15:27

and ends up making you feel better, which matters

15:29

too.

15:30

Yeah, And I think one thing we get wrong when we hear

15:32

self compassion, And this is definitely something I've seen

15:34

when I teach the strategy to my students is

15:36

that they hear it as self indulgence. They

15:39

think, like, if you're being kind to yourself, you're going to

15:41

like let yourself off the hook or kind of

15:43

not call yourself out when you are acting

15:45

problematically, like when things are kind of a real problem.

15:48

And I think that's why this idea of talking to yourself

15:50

like you would a friend is so powerful. Like

15:52

Maya, you're my friend and former student. If you

15:54

were doing something that was really terrible, I

15:57

would give you a talking to, but I wouldn't do it

15:59

in a mean way and say, ma, you suck whatever.

16:01

I would say like, Maya, what is going

16:03

on? Like I just want to know how

16:05

I can help? What can I do right? And

16:07

so in some ways, this self compassion

16:09

isn't self indulgence, it's not kind of letting yourself

16:12

off the hook. If anything, it's what Kristin

16:14

f calls fierce, right, Like you are ready

16:16

to dive in even for tough problems and

16:18

not avoid them because you care about yourself

16:20

that much. Right. That's this kind of analogy with a friend.

16:23

If a good friend's going through something tough and they're

16:25

not behaving in the right way, you're going to

16:27

check in. But you're not going to check in in this kind of

16:29

mean, drill sergeant way. You're going to check

16:31

in with kindness and curiosity and

16:34

like understanding, right, And that's just kind of what

16:36

we need to apply to ourselves too.

16:38

Yeah, and that drill sergeant approach

16:40

can really backfire.

16:41

I remember.

16:42

One of the freshest insights that I learned

16:44

from Kristin is that when you

16:46

are crippled by shame, right, when

16:48

you feel that the thing you did is not just bad, that

16:50

you're bad, it actually closes

16:52

you off to the idea of improvement because

16:55

if you're bad, you're irredeemable. There's no chance

16:57

at making progress or ameliorating the situation.

16:59

So actually self compassion is

17:02

the instrument by which we

17:04

can unlock growth and do better.

17:06

So it's the opposite of letting ourselves off the hooktually,

17:09

the thing that allows our brains to be open minded enough

17:11

to think that there is redemption or

17:14

at least a path to progress.

17:15

Exactly, Maya. I love that you've brought up

17:18

like the self talk and how we can use it better. I wish

17:20

that was a chapter in the World Happiness Report.

17:22

I think it's super important. Thank

17:24

you so much for coming on the Happiness Lab.

17:26

Thank you so much for having me, Laurie.

17:28

A little later we'll be talking to Malcolm Gladwell

17:31

about the joy or lack of it, he gets

17:33

from running. But next up, the economist

17:35

and Pushkin podcaster Tim Harford discusses

17:38

the famous happiness experiment that echoes

17:40

in his own medical history.

17:42

I have to have colorsco is quite often

17:44

we don't want to go into too many details, but it's

17:47

a whole journey.

17:48

All that after a quick break. If

17:55

you look back through previous World Happiness Reports,

17:58

you'll see that a lot of effort has been put into

18:00

investigating why some people are happier than

18:02

others, and indeed why some nations

18:04

seem happier than their neighbors, but

18:06

even in our own individual lives, our happiness

18:09

tends to ebb and flow. We can be happy

18:11

one year and down the next. Over

18:13

the course of just an hour, we can experience

18:15

a whole gamut of emotions, both good

18:17

and bad. But there's an interesting

18:19

bit of happiness research that shows just how

18:22

slippery our grip on happiness can be. And

18:24

that's the topic that was picked up by our next

18:26

guest on this special show.

18:28

I am Tim Harft. I

18:30

am a senior columnist at the Financial

18:32

Times, and I'm the host of Cautionary

18:35

Tales, which is a podcast

18:37

all about the catastrophes of the

18:39

past and how we can learn

18:41

from them.

18:42

Tim admits to being obsessed by the work of Nobel

18:44

Prize warning psychologist Danny Kanneman,

18:47

and especially the work that Danny did on how

18:49

we can remember bad experiences fondly

18:51

given the right circumstances. In a series

18:53

of experiments, Danny found that we can go through

18:56

some pretty harrowing experiences, but

18:58

with a couple of tweaks about how that ordeal

19:00

ends, we can look back on even terrible

19:02

times in a much more positive way than we

19:04

expect.

19:05

He emphasizes the difference between

19:08

remember happiness and

19:11

experienced happiness, and

19:13

you would think happiness is just happiness, right,

19:15

But of course Danny Carman gets to be Danny

19:17

Carman by drawing these fine distinctions

19:19

that never occurred to the rest of us. So

19:22

let me give you an example that he ran

19:24

an experiment where they've got people to hold

19:26

their hands in ice water for

19:29

sixty seconds, and using

19:31

a kind of computer mouse, they could register how

19:34

much that was hurting holding your left

19:36

hand in this cold water. They get a nice

19:38

warm towel, bit of a break, and then they

19:40

got them to put their other hand in the water, not

19:42

for sixty seconds, but for ninety seconds. But

19:44

for the last thirty seconds the water

19:47

got slightly warmer, I mean not warm,

19:49

but just a little bit less horrible,

19:52

And then you got your warm towel. But

19:54

then the people participating in this experiment

19:56

were asked do you want to do their left handing

19:59

again or do you want to do the right hand thing

20:01

again? In other words, do you want sixty seconds

20:03

of pain followed by nice

20:05

warm towels or do you want sixty

20:07

seconds of pain followed by thirty

20:09

seconds of slightly less painful pain

20:12

followed by nice, warm towels, and people

20:14

wanted the longer experience. They

20:16

wanted the longer, more uncomfortable experience because

20:19

they didn't remember it as more uncomfortable.

20:21

What they recalled was, oh, well,

20:23

I put my hand in iced water and it was painful,

20:26

or there was that other time I put my hand in iced water and

20:28

it wasn't as bad. I didn't remember it as being as

20:30

bad. And the reason they don't remember it as being as bad

20:32

is because it didn't end as uncomfortably.

20:35

So in this particular case, Karnaman

20:37

was highlighting, there's a clear irrationality.

20:40

Obviously, it's better to be less

20:42

time and pain.

20:44

Yeah, less time and pain, like as you're experiencing

20:46

them, the only difference between the two experiences

20:50

was one of them had thirty extra

20:52

seconds of discomfort. But as you

20:54

remembered them, they're very different. Okay,

20:56

so what has that got to do with happiness? Well, it turns

20:58

out that this distinction between what you're experiencing

21:01

as you go through it and then how you remember

21:03

it applies to all sorts of things

21:05

in our lives. You might experience

21:08

a happy relationship, but then it ends in

21:10

a really messy way, and then suddenly

21:12

the whole relationship is like, well that was a disaster. You

21:14

might experience a pleasant vacation, but then

21:17

you have all kinds of trouble getting home from the

21:19

vacation, and then the whole vacation is

21:21

spoiled. And so this distinction between

21:23

the stories we tell ourselves about our lives,

21:25

what we remember about our lives, and how we're

21:27

actually experiencing our lives as we go along,

21:30

it really matters. And I'm not sure I would

21:32

say that one of these things is the truth, like

21:34

the experience is the truth and the memory is false. I

21:36

don't think it's that simple, But there's a distinction there

21:38

that's worth exploring.

21:40

Tim is, of course right. That distinction

21:42

can have a huge impact on our lives. Twenty

21:44

years ago, Danny Carneman conducted a study

21:47

to see if the medical procedure used at the time

21:49

to examine the human bowel for disease could

21:51

be made less uncomfortable, at least in

21:53

our memories. If it could, then

21:56

fewer people might duck out of the exam because of

21:58

the discomfort, and more lives would be

22:00

saved. So, just like in the ice

22:02

water experiment, Danny decided

22:04

to extend the duration of a colonoscopy.

22:07

At the end of the procedure that basically

22:09

the surgeon would leave the probe

22:11

in, so to speak, without wiggling it around.

22:14

So it was kind of uncomfortable, but fine.

22:17

People rated those colonoscopies as

22:20

less unpleasant, even though minute by minute

22:22

it was clearly worse than the shorter

22:25

procedure. The joy is because of

22:27

a family history, I have to have colonoscopies

22:29

quite often. We don't want to go into too many

22:31

details, but the whole thing lasts

22:34

a couple of days, and it's

22:36

a whole journey.

22:37

I once presented the colonoscopy study

22:39

to a group of medical doctors who chastised

22:42

me afterwards because they noted that when Danny did

22:44

that study and people were in serious

22:47

rectal pain during the entire colonoscopy,

22:49

and we could kind of vary how it ended. That

22:52

that was before the beauty of anesthesia

22:54

that we have today, And those doctors

22:57

said, your colonoscopy won't be nearly as

22:59

bad. You'll just kind of get knocked out, have no remembered

23:02

happiness or experienced happiness,

23:04

and then you get a nice little bottle of juice.

23:06

Yeah. So I mean as a connoisseur of

23:09

of having cameras shoved

23:11

whether the sun doesn't shine, Yeah,

23:14

they're fine. Actually, don't avoid. Do

23:16

not avoid your kernoroscopy people that

23:19

it's fine.

23:20

Yeah. I love the experience versus remembered

23:22

happiness stuff. I mean for a couple of reasons. One

23:24

is that I love that Danny's figured this out

23:26

and we can now start better engineering

23:29

enjoyably remembered experiences

23:31

just by making them kind of end pretty well

23:33

at the end, right, you know, if you've had a kind of

23:35

crappy vacation, you know, and it hasn't gone

23:37

so well at the end, you can just kind of stick in some

23:39

pleasant thing and then all of a sudden you can start

23:42

feeling a little bit happier. Danny also

23:44

gives a suggestion that, you know, if you've

23:46

had this vacation that's gone really well, and say

23:48

that the day that you're flying home, you know, everything falls

23:50

apart and terrible things happen. He

23:52

would say, well, then you need to kind of reframe

23:55

the vacation. There was the vacation, you

23:57

know, it ended on a high note, and then there

23:59

was the kind of crappy travel day home, but I'm just going to

24:01

kind of put that into a different mental slot, and

24:03

now all of a sudden, you can remember your vacation

24:06

pleasantly, even though it sort of ended on

24:08

a not so good no. And so I love this

24:10

strategy because by using what he

24:12

calls this peak end effect, where you're sort of paying

24:14

too much attention to the end of events, you

24:17

can sort of remember that the end of events matter

24:19

a lot, and you just need to make sure that things

24:21

end well, and then you'll kind of be happier. It's

24:23

also funny to me that I think there's so many natural

24:26

events in our lives that end well and we

24:28

remember them really fondly, like desserts

24:30

and orgasms and all these things that seem

24:33

to be particularly good at the end, and now

24:35

all of a sudden, we remember these things as

24:37

the best experience as ever.

24:38

Yeah, although meals, if

24:41

you go out for a meal, it doesn't then with dessert.

24:43

Have to end with the bill, ends with

24:45

the bill, Laurie, It.

24:46

Ends with somebody asking you to pay money. But

24:49

we still go out for dinner, and we

24:51

don't feel that was a mistake. So I guess we

24:53

successfully compartmentalize the bill as being

24:56

something else. But maybe

24:58

restaurants should experiment with getting

25:00

people to pay up front. If you go to really fancy

25:02

restaurant and it has a tasting menu, you can

25:05

actually know what the whole thing is going

25:07

to cost, and you could pay in advance. Love

25:09

this, Maybe that would be in everybody's

25:11

interest. You just remember the whole thing more fondly.

25:13

I do think some American restaurants have tried to come

25:15

over this. We have a few restaurants in my hometown

25:17

in New Heaven that when they bring the bill, they'll bring you like

25:20

a little candy or some Swedish fish or something.

25:22

So it's kind of this little surprise moment

25:24

at the ends. You're paying the bill, but then you get to have

25:27

some tasty candy at the end, but the bill at the beginning

25:29

will save them the candy cast. I love this idea.

25:31

Absolutely, But this distinction

25:33

between what we remember and what we

25:35

experience, I think it broadens out beyond

25:38

this narrow but important point of

25:40

we're really influenced by how things end. I mean, that's important

25:43

in itself, but if you think about, for example,

25:45

the standard question that people

25:47

are asked when they're asked to evaluate their

25:49

happiness, which is like, how's it going. I

25:52

mean, I realized there's a little bit more more

25:54

formal than that, but I mean that's like so metrics.

25:56

Folks that might say that is

25:59

but no, but seriously, all things considered,

26:01

you know, how happy were you this week? Right? That's

26:04

a remembered judgment. Right. People don't have

26:06

access to their experienced happiness during

26:08

the week at every moment when you're asking them

26:10

that question. All they have access to is that remembered

26:13

version. And if the remembered version is

26:15

biased, either because it pays too much attention

26:17

to what just happened or how things ended

26:19

or whatever, then we're just not going to get

26:21

great happiness judgments.

26:23

No, and you've phrased it, how do things

26:25

go this week? Which is one question.

26:27

But you could ask people how are things

26:30

going in general? How satisfied are

26:32

you with your life? Or you could ask people,

26:34

tell me about yesterday, how are things

26:36

yesterday? Or you can get them to focus in

26:39

in more detail. Let's walk through what happened

26:41

yesterday. Let's go through the breakfast, the morning

26:43

commute, you had these meetings, you had

26:45

lunch with a friend, all the different things you did.

26:48

So these are quite distinct ways

26:50

of thinking about measuring

26:52

happiness. If we're asked, for example, to

26:54

evaluate our lives and

26:56

we were just about to get married or were recently

26:59

married, you know, I'm getting married

27:01

or I just got married, is that like a huge

27:03

deal? But if instead it's like, well, my children

27:05

are graduating, they're going to leave home, they're

27:07

going off to college. Well that's what you

27:09

think about. Well maybe you're ill and that's

27:11

what you think about. But actually none of these things are

27:14

in fact as all encompassing as

27:16

they seem to be when you are directing

27:18

your attention at them.

27:20

Yeah.

27:20

I mean. The good news about these measures,

27:22

though, is that one could ask the question like, what are

27:24

we really trying to maximize? Right? You know, most

27:26

of the stuff we talk about in the Happiness Lab is all about

27:28

strategies that you can use to maximize

27:31

your happiness. As the question is what are

27:33

we trying to maximize? And I think to a

27:35

certain extent, what we're trying to maximize is what

27:37

people say in those remembered judgments. Right.

27:39

For example, if I do some sort of intervention,

27:42

right, like I get people to scribble in a gratitude

27:44

journal, or I get people to engage with

27:46

more social connection, and then later on I

27:48

asked them, hey, you know, all things considered, how are you feeling

27:50

with your life or how are you feeling yesterday? What was

27:52

your positive emotion like yesterday? And people

27:54

say like, oh, it was pretty good. Then

27:56

my sense is that that social connection intervention,

27:58

or that gratitude intervention, it did actually do

28:01

some work. It might just not be doing all

28:03

the work we assume it's doing because these judgments

28:05

are a little bit biased.

28:06

Yeah, I don't entirely disagree,

28:09

but I would want to raise a question.

28:12

So if Laurie, for example, you encouraged

28:14

your listeners to maybe go out

28:16

and have more diverse experiences,

28:19

go and meet more people, go to more

28:21

places, do more challenging things,

28:24

take more short vacations

28:27

rather than fewer long vacations.

28:30

Because all of these things are going to

28:33

lay down new memories, your life

28:35

is going to seem richer and more satisfying. I

28:37

mean, that's advice I would give myself, that's advice I

28:39

would take from you for sure. And yet,

28:42

and yet are you not actually

28:45

minute to minute potentially subjecting

28:47

yourself to a lot more stress, more

28:49

congestion, more uncomfortable situations,

28:52

more difficulty, more danger,

28:54

and actually you're going through your life potentially

28:57

having a worse experience moment

28:59

to moment, and yet at the end

29:01

of the year you look back at it and go, that was great.

29:04

Whose side to Thomas Shelling?

29:06

Economist Thomas Shelling would would talk about

29:08

this sort of thing, and he would raise

29:10

the question whose side should you be on in

29:13

that argument with yourself? Who's right?

29:15

And I don't think the answer is entirely obvious.

29:18

Yeah, I think one strategy we can use

29:20

to get better at it is to do

29:22

a better job of recognizing what's

29:24

happening in our moment to moment self. I think

29:26

the problem with the moment to moment self is that we're

29:29

not often doing that evaluation. We're not

29:31

taking time to be mindful and to recognize

29:33

what's going on. But I think these practices

29:35

where people engage in a little bit more mindfulness,

29:38

even when it is being mindful about kind of not

29:40

so great situations, you can kind of notice what

29:42

negative emotions you're experiencing. Those

29:44

kind of strategies can help us pay a little bit more

29:47

attention to the experience self in

29:49

the moment, so you're kind of kind of meta aware

29:51

as you're going through those kinds of events during

29:53

your day, and I think that can help us come

29:55

up with a little bit of a better judgment. Right,

29:58

we can kind of do the work to realize

30:00

like, yeah, you know, it was fun to think about going on that vacation.

30:02

That was great in my remembered happiness, but actually

30:05

I kind of hate the traffic. I kind

30:07

of hate going through you know, the tea essay or whatever.

30:10

That mindfulness can sort of help us pay attention,

30:12

and I think it can also help us pay attention in the other

30:15

direction too, Right, we can start noticing

30:17

the little good things about our life

30:19

that are going well, so that in times

30:21

that are kind of sucky, we can go back to our

30:24

experienced happiness and notice like, actually,

30:26

it wasn't that bad. I mean, this was to

30:28

a certain extent my experience during COVID,

30:30

where you know, in large part I was just starting

30:32

some of this happiness work. So I was doing all this work

30:34

and in the moment to kind of be mindful of

30:36

the taste of my coffee and be grateful for

30:39

the small things. And I think my overall

30:41

evaluation of how bad it was during COVID

30:44

is a little bit less bad than it could have been

30:46

in a remembered sense, because I was there

30:48

noticing mindfully some of these little

30:50

things in life that were good, that didn't go away even

30:52

in the midst of that pandemic time.

30:55

One thing I have been doing recently is

30:57

I have been keeping what is

30:59

sometimes called a good time journal. So

31:01

at the end of each day, I think back on what I've

31:04

been doing and how much fun it was. And

31:06

one thing I really noticed was

31:09

that intense physical exercise.

31:12

So going to the gym or kickboxing

31:14

classes, they were always great

31:16

in hindsight, and I know they

31:19

I mean they hurt, they properly hurt. At

31:21

the time, you were so glad. When they're over three

31:24

hours later, you're looking back and going that was

31:26

the best part of the day. And I guess that

31:28

that is part of the weirdness and the

31:31

fun of Danny Carnerman's distinction

31:33

that he's making.

31:34

I love that. I love that, as

31:37

you'd expect from a master podcaster. Tim's

31:39

talk there exercise sets us up perfectly

31:42

for the last part of this special show, which

31:44

Keen amateur runner Malcolm Gladwell turns

31:47

a familiar happiness maxim on its head,

31:49

it's.

31:49

The journey, not the destination. I just like, no,

31:53

it's the destination. Otherwise, what's

31:55

the point of the journey.

31:57

The Happiness Lab will be right back. Hey,

32:04

Hey, how's it going.

32:05

It's going well.

32:06

If I'm asking my fellow Pushkin host to re imagine

32:09

the World Happiness Report, there's no way I could

32:11

leave out revisionist history. Is Malcolm Gladwell.

32:14

I knew he was going to have something interesting and provocative

32:16

to add.

32:17

All right, we're ready, fire away, all

32:19

right.

32:20

The question I had for you, Malcolm, is if you were

32:22

an author of the World Happiness Report, if

32:24

you were doing your own chapter in this big report,

32:27

what would you want to talk about?

32:28

I would like to do my argument

32:31

that the phrase it's

32:33

the journey not the destination is

32:36

backwards. Oh, there's the whole

32:39

important class of happiness that's

32:42

about the destination and not the

32:44

journey. And there's

32:46

a special kind of deep and

32:49

enduring I think pleasure

32:53

fulfillment, where it's just all it's all

32:55

about where you end up, and that getting there

32:57

is sometimes hard and unpleasant, and

32:59

that that makes the ending

33:02

even sweeter. I've always

33:04

found something uniquely kind

33:06

of troubling about

33:08

that phrase, it's the journey, not the destination.

33:11

I just like, no,

33:14

it's like it's the destination.

33:17

Otherwise, what's the point of the journey.

33:19

Well, well, let's unpack that a little

33:21

bit, because there are spots. There are spots

33:23

where I agree with you, and there's spots where I think

33:25

the science might differ a little.

33:28

Like I think about this all time because I'm a runner.

33:30

Every time, I've been running my entire life.

33:32

So I've been running. I'm sixty,

33:35

I've been running for essentially fifty years.

33:38

Every time I go running, I have exactly

33:40

the same psychological experience, which

33:43

is I don't really want.

33:44

To do it.

33:46

I mean I make a place for it, and

33:48

I kind of formally look forward to it.

33:50

I packed my running clothes. I know when I'm going to go

33:52

running, I drive to a running place

33:54

or you know, I set it all up. But you

33:57

know, if you told me I could go

33:59

home and drink

34:01

a beer, you know, there's a powerful

34:03

temptation every time not to do it. And then

34:06

when I'm running, it's not always

34:08

pleasant. You know it's going to be. If

34:10

you're doing a hard track workout, it's hard,

34:13

it's daunting. I mean, you're pushing yourself

34:15

and it's but then when

34:18

you're finished, there is a kind

34:21

of experience from having finished

34:23

it that keeps me going back

34:25

to it for fifty years. It's

34:28

thirty two degrees out there today,

34:31

I'm going to go running. I don't want to go running in

34:33

thirty two degrees, but I will do it because

34:35

there's a plasure.

34:36

You know.

34:36

When I'm done and I'm back home and it's warm

34:38

again, I'm really really happy

34:41

that I did it right. But I

34:43

wouldn't describe the

34:45

actual experience. It's not masochism

34:48

because while I'm running, I

34:51

have in the back of my mind the

34:54

memory of the feeling of having finished

34:56

running, and that makes the

34:59

effort worth it and in

35:01

a certain way pleasurable in this sort of in

35:03

this sort of different way,

35:05

it's like you're testing yourself in this way

35:08

that you you kind of appreciate. That's

35:10

so that's the argument I think.

35:11

Yeah, I mean, I think it maps on to this distinction

35:14

that I feel like it's mountaineering folks

35:16

who started this distinction between type one and

35:18

type two fun. So type one fun is

35:20

really just the beer, just sitting home

35:23

having the beer, you know, like you know, hot

35:25

fudge Sunday's orgasms, Like just like the

35:27

in the moment stuff is just good

35:29

and deeply pleasurable, whereas type

35:31

two fun is sort of the opposite. It's like, again,

35:33

it's not fun in the moment. It's not fun when

35:35

you're like putting your shoes on and that first blast

35:37

of the thirty two degree weather when you're running. But

35:40

the fact that there's a goal at the end that you're going

35:42

to get to means the type two fun winds

35:45

up being really interesting. And this is like

35:47

just a distinction that like sports nuts and like

35:49

people who write in mountaineering blogs make, But

35:51

it's actually something that the economist George Lowenstein

35:53

studied himself. He wrote this paper of like why

35:55

climb a mountain? But the idea is like, why

35:58

would you ever do something

36:00

where it's like kind of again not masochism,

36:02

maybe not miserable in the moment, but it doesn't have fun

36:04

in the actual journey itself. It just has fun

36:07

when you hit the end of it. Yeah, And so he

36:09

argues that this is like a deep feature of

36:12

human pleasure seeking, is that we don't

36:14

just seek pleasure kind of in the moment for

36:16

the journey, like most of the good, meaningful

36:19

pleasures we get involve some hard stuff.

36:21

I mean, you're talking about running, but I know you're also a

36:23

dad and raising a kid, and that's the kind of thing

36:25

that in the moment, the pleasure is not great.

36:27

But when you get to these achievement moments like graduates

36:30

from kindergarten or do these fun things

36:32

like those matter a lot more. And so Lowenstein's

36:35

argument is that there's so much of human motivation

36:38

is motivation not to do the thing kind

36:40

of in the moment for the journey, but the

36:42

motivation kind of comes from the very

36:44

fact of there being an arrival at

36:46

the end. I think the problem, though, is when

36:48

everything's about the arrival at the end, and I

36:51

think this is the kind of thing I see maybe

36:53

with my students right where they get mistaken

36:55

about how much they're going to enjoy the arrival at the

36:57

end of I don't know, getting into a

36:59

super good college, or

37:01

getting married, or there's all these big

37:04

things in life that we put our happiness only

37:06

at the arrival at the end, and sometimes that can

37:08

set us up for like kind of mispredicting

37:10

how good that's going to feel. When students get into college

37:13

there's all these videos now of like the acceptance

37:15

moment when students click on the link and they find out

37:17

did I get into Yale or did I not get into

37:20

Yale? And when they click on the link

37:22

and they get in, they start screaming like yeah, that's great.

37:24

But students will self report afterwards like five

37:26

minutes later, well that

37:28

was a letdown, Like there's just the next caret

37:31

to go after in the next caret, And so I

37:33

think the challenge is, like how do we balance both

37:35

of those. On the one hand, we want to get the meaningful

37:38

pursuit from the big arrival moments in life,

37:40

but we don't want to like have those only

37:42

be the things, or be picking things

37:45

where their arrival isn't as good as we expected.

37:47

We kind of mispredict how awesome it'll be in the end.

37:50

I think part of the answer is, I'm thinking again

37:52

of the running example. Part of the answer

37:54

is in understanding that the kind

37:57

of satisfaction that you get from

38:00

the journey

38:02

is not less, it's

38:04

just different. So when I go

38:06

for a long run, there's always a moment

38:09

in a long run where like in the

38:11

middle, where you're

38:14

filled with this sense of awe

38:18

about what human

38:20

It's funny, in fifty years,

38:23

I've always had this, always, this moment wherein

38:25

I think, Holy mackerel, I

38:29

can't believe people. It's never

38:31

personal. It's all about the class of runners.

38:34

I can't believe we're capable of doing this,

38:37

Like, you know, you might be You're

38:39

eight miles into a twelve mile run, so

38:41

you've been out there for an hour, and

38:45

you're like, is it really possible for

38:47

someone to be a

38:49

middle aged man to go out and

38:51

run twelve miles and be fine about

38:54

it? Like it just seems like it seems incredible

38:56

to me, Like you're moving, You're not meandering,

38:58

You're like moving on. You know. It's sort of a fairly

39:00

decent clip. And that's like I

39:03

was it always. It fills me this with the same kind

39:05

of wonder that I get whenever I

39:07

see anyone doing something thing that

39:10

requires effort and

39:12

talent and persistence.

39:15

Right, it makes me feel better about

39:17

human beings that we can

39:20

we can sort of pull this off.

39:22

I love that and it fits with I mean, there's

39:24

this lovely work by Daker Keltner that looks at

39:26

all these domains in which people experience awe

39:29

and wonder, and I think we assume that

39:31

that's going to be, you know, these moments in

39:33

nature when you connect with the divine.

39:35

And he finds that the most common moments of awe

39:38

in people's everyday experience is when we experience

39:40

awe for the awesomeness of

39:42

human beings, like human's moral character or

39:44

their individual performance and achievement. And

39:47

so I love that you get that while you're running, but that's

39:49

not I mean, I'm not a runner, but I do, like, you

39:51

know, these long, hardcore yoga routines,

39:53

and that is not my experience in the moment

39:56

of the top yoga routine. My experience

39:58

is always like why am I doing this?

40:01

Sucks?

40:02

Like I need to figure out, like I

40:04

need to find ways to get to these deep moments of

40:06

awe during.

40:07

Because you know the There's another way in

40:10

which journeys differ from destinations,

40:12

which is that the pleasure that comes

40:14

from reaching the destination

40:17

is I don't want to say fixed,

40:20

it's one very specific, singular

40:22

thing, whereas the satisfaction

40:25

that comes from the journey, you're cycling through a

40:27

series of responses, so it's

40:29

thirty two degrees out or whatever.

40:32

And grew up in Canada going and running. I've

40:34

gone rung in minus twenty before there's

40:37

that dread, Oh you know, shit,

40:39

go ahead. Then there's like ten

40:41

minutes and you're like it's not that bad. And then

40:44

fifteen minutes in your relax and just sort of running

40:46

easily and you're not tired yet. And then there's that all

40:48

moment like I can't believe I'm doing

40:50

this. It's kind of amazing, right, and then there's

40:53

that kind of like it's almost over

40:56

exhilaration. It's like the

40:58

journey is six different emotional states.

41:01

The destination is one, and

41:03

it's just and I think whenever

41:06

I try to get non runners to run,

41:08

it's very difficult to explain them that

41:11

they're fixated on the first state, which

41:14

is, oh man, it's hard, I don't know about there,

41:16

and they forget no, no, no, there's like there's

41:19

there's five more after that. You just have to

41:21

get to them. This is a big deal in Canada

41:23

because of how much running you have to do in the

41:25

cold, that you have to understand

41:28

that cold only only

41:30

is a problem for the first five minutes.

41:32

And I think that's true for so many experiences that

41:34

ultimately give us happiness right, Like I think,

41:37

you know, on the show, we talk a lot about social connection,

41:39

for example, like just talking to a stranger,

41:42

which ultimately, once you're five minutes into it

41:44

and it's feeling good, is awesome and

41:46

you really enjoy it. But the friction

41:48

at the start of it, that first question

41:50

that kind of awkward or they're going to hate me. All

41:52

those predictions are off. And so I think this

41:54

is like maybe a deep truth of things that make

41:57

us like happy, is that a lot

41:59

of them start with some friction, and

42:01

like the first the first step is sucky,

42:03

and you have to overcome the sucky step to get to the

42:05

good part. But a lot of times we like miss

42:08

miss the sucky I mean, I think that that's a real

42:10

problem with so many of our happiness pursuits,

42:12

is that, like we have to overcome that moment

42:14

of friction, but there's often an opportunity

42:16

cost of the thing that has no friction, you know, for you

42:19

with the run, it's like instead of getting out in the like

42:21

thirty two degree day, sit home

42:23

and have the beer. Right, the frictionless thing is

42:25

always appealing, but to get to the thing that

42:27

makes us truly kind of feel great, we have to kind

42:29

of overcome those first steps of friction.

42:31

Yeah.

42:32

You know, various sports have different relationships

42:34

to these questions that we're talking about, and one

42:37

of the most extreme is cyclists.

42:40

I used to listen a lot to still do to Lance

42:43

Armstrong's podcast, which is actually

42:45

really good. You know, there's always a moment where

42:48

Lance has one of his fellow cyclists

42:50

on and they just talk about start talking about

42:52

suffering, and like you realize

42:54

they don't mean suffering the way we

42:56

mean suffering. I don't think there's anything

42:58

that's as painful as the Twitter France.

43:01

I don't I just nothing. Running a marathon

43:03

for a world class athlete, it's like two hours and

43:05

ten minutes and then you're done. The Twitter France

43:07

guys are out there like all day for

43:10

like weeks. It's insane. They're

43:12

like risking their lives, They're

43:14

losing twenty pounds, their butts,

43:16

sore, their back. I mean, it's just like incredible,

43:18

Like what they go through. The whole thing is just nuts.

43:21

It's just nuts. I mean it does look

43:23

to the rest of us like masochism, but

43:25

their ability to kind of

43:28

reinterpret masochism

43:30

as something fulfilling and

43:32

redeeming, and it's just it's just amazing

43:35

to me. I remember once Lence was talking

43:37

to some guy and they were talking about how they're trying

43:39

to teach their kids to suffer

43:42

in the way that they liked suffering, and how it was just

43:44

impossible. Like it's not a generational thing. It's

43:46

just that those cyclists

43:48

are so singular in their ability to reinterpret

43:51

pain.

43:52

Well, you'll appreciate that. In fact, one

43:54

of the most famous papers on what's

43:56

known as rosy retrospection, which is this

43:59

idea that you look back at an experience that was

44:01

kind of sucking, you think that was awesome. I would totally

44:03

do it again. It actually looked at competitive

44:06

cyclists they oh really yeah, yeah,

44:09

people's happiness at every at various

44:11

moments along the trip. And you know, when you're going to

44:13

the trip for cycling, you feel great, and then

44:15

you're on the trip and every rating is

44:17

low, and then you come back and it's

44:20

and you say, what was your average rating on the trip,

44:22

And that retrospective average rating

44:24

on the trip is like many points higher

44:26

than the actual average at any point on the

44:28

trip, So you kind of think back positively.

44:31

So maybe it's they didn't. They didn't look into the individual

44:33

differences that cyclist, and they were trying to

44:35

make a general point about human nature and rosy

44:38

retrospect, and they weren't making at individual

44:40

differences and cyclists in particular. But maybe

44:42

they should have. Maybe cyclists especially I.

44:45

Used to cycle a lot, and I just stopped. I

44:47

can't reinterpret my suffering the way they

44:49

do. Let's go do a century. You

44:51

know, you ride by one hundred miles, so

44:54

nuts. It's like, I'm perfectly

44:56

happy to suffer, but I will not suffer for six

44:58

hours.

44:59

So how do you get how do you get through the initial friction

45:01

on your runs? Right? What's the what's a

45:03

tip that our listeners can use to kind of bust

45:05

through that friction to get to the happier,

45:08

longer, more meaningful journey.

45:10

At the end starts low as

45:12

the obvious one. In the beginning, you're

45:14

trying to distract yourself and you're thinking about kinds

45:16

of things. You will eventually as you get into

45:18

it, be running associatively where you just

45:21

be focused on yourself. And that's very kind

45:23

of you know, as runners high whatever

45:25

they want to call it, but I don't. I prefer

45:28

it sounds to me, that makes it sound very extravagant.

45:31

It's just a kind of point of

45:33

equilibrium. You'll get there eventually.

45:36

I think a lot of the problems that beginning runners

45:38

have is it sounds very paradoxical,

45:40

is their runs are too short. So

45:43

go out for two miles. No, no, no, no, two miles.

45:45

I'm sorry, you're not transitioning

45:47

to anything if all you're doing is running two miles, Like

45:50

there is a kind of I've always thought many runers with me

45:52

that there is a magic about going

45:54

past an hour that once

45:56

you get into hour two, really

45:58

really lovely things happen. It

46:01

could be forty five minutes, but it's certainly

46:03

not fifteen minutes, like it's

46:05

not happening.

46:06

Yeah, this is cool. You've got to get You have to give yourself

46:08

the time, and then once you get into it, the

46:10

flow start kicking in.

46:12

Yeah. The same thing, by the way, with writing

46:14

a book, any kind of long concentrated activity,

46:18

is just you have to readjust your time

46:20

horizons. You're not making sense

46:22

of a draft in two

46:24

days. You know, if you're disappointed after

46:26

two days, it's because your time horizon was wrong.

46:29

And this raises a question of like how we can get to better

46:31

time horizons. But ironically, some

46:33

of the research by Shichi Twang and

46:35

Jennifer Aker at Stanford suggests that one way

46:37

we can get to longer time horizons is

46:40

to start thinking about the journey more. They have all

46:42

this work on what they call journey mindset. For

46:44

example, like I want to lose some weight, I want to know hit

46:46

my goal weight. Like no, Actually, what you want to do

46:48

is like be it your goal weight for a really long

46:50

time. Or I want to like get

46:52

this feeling of happiness that comes from like writing the book.

46:55

I want to get through the book Like no, you want

46:57

to experience the benefit of having written the book

46:59

and be able to talk to, you know, the people who read it

47:01

and experience those ideas later. Or for my

47:03

college students, like I want to get my degree, No,

47:05

you want to like get a degree so you can be a lifelong

47:08

learner and get the skills you need to learn

47:10

in the future. So they find that

47:12

it's easier to sustain motivation, for example,

47:15

for getting a college degree or writing for the book,

47:17

if you think of the kind of thing that

47:19

you're going to get out of it that's beyond the achievement,

47:22

and so ironically you might have gotten back to the fact

47:24

that the journey maybe is good.

47:26

No, Remember I didn't say journy didn't

47:29

matter. I was objecting to the phrase

47:31

it's the journey, not the

47:33

destination.

47:34

The journey and the destination.

47:36

It's the journey and the destination. Yes, I'll

47:38

buy that.

47:40

So Malcolm wants to see the destination given

47:42

a bit more love in the World Happiness Report,

47:45

while Tim Harford would like to add a chapter on

47:47

our memories of happiness, and Maya

47:49

Schunker thinks that tackling our disruptive

47:51

inner monologues should be included. But

47:54

we'll be back to examine what's in the Real World

47:56

Happiness Report. We'll talk to its

47:58

authors about what they think are the most pressing

48:00

issues facing us in twenty twenty four.

48:03

All that on the next episode of The Happiness

48:05

Lab with Me Doctor Laurie Santo's

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features