Podchaser Logo
Home
The psychology of self-persuasion with Elliot Aronson

The psychology of self-persuasion with Elliot Aronson

Released Tuesday, 5th December 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
The psychology of self-persuasion with Elliot Aronson

The psychology of self-persuasion with Elliot Aronson

The psychology of self-persuasion with Elliot Aronson

The psychology of self-persuasion with Elliot Aronson

Tuesday, 5th December 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:01

Ted Audio Collective. And

0:07

now a word from our sponsor, Robinhood. Make

0:10

your money do the most with

0:13

Robinhood Gold. Get 5% APY on

0:15

your cash and $2.5 million in

0:17

FDIC insurance through our partner banks,

0:20

all for just $5 a month. Get

0:23

30 days for free when you

0:25

sign up at robinhood.com/gold podcast. As

0:27

of 9-21-23 via bankrate, interest is

0:29

earned on idle cash swept from

0:31

your brokerage account to program banks.

0:34

Cash suite program and gold are offered through

0:36

Robinhood Financial LLC. Terms apply. Rate

0:39

may change. Robinhood is not a bank. Hey,

0:41

everyone. It's Adam Grant. Welcome

0:45

back to Rethinking, my podcast on the science

0:47

of what makes us tick. I'm

0:50

an organizational psychologist, and I'm taking you

0:52

inside the minds of fascinating people to

0:54

explore new thoughts and new ways of

0:56

thinking. My

1:01

guest today is the legendary social

1:03

psychologist, Elliot Aronson. He's

1:06

a pioneer of the study of

1:08

cognitive dissonance, the uncomfortable tension we

1:10

feel when our attitudes and actions

1:12

conflict. He co-authored a

1:14

book on dissonance that I think should be

1:16

required reading for all humans, Mistakes

1:18

Were Made, But Not By Me. He

1:21

also wrote The Social Animal, an

1:23

award-winning textbook beloved by generations of

1:25

psychology students, now in its 12th

1:28

edition. Elliot is the only

1:30

person ever to win the triple crown of

1:32

awards from the American Psychological Association

1:34

for research, teaching, and writing. And

1:37

he won the William James Award for lifetime achievement.

1:40

He's 91, and I'd love to be as

1:43

sharp any day as he is today. There's

1:48

so much I'm excited to talk about today. Elliot,

1:51

you're a legendary teacher. I know that

1:53

generations of students have admired your wisdom

1:55

and also your kindness. doing

2:00

is inviting or

2:02

maybe challenging your TAs to give a guest

2:05

lecture. Why? I

2:07

love teaching the introductory

2:09

social psychology course.

2:12

And it's always been a huge course.

2:14

Like at the University of Texas, we

2:17

would have about 600 students in there.

2:19

And even at the University of California

2:21

Santa Cruz, we would get 300, 350

2:23

students. And I always thought

2:27

it would be a nice

2:30

introduction for my TAs.

2:33

They were leading sections.

2:36

And I thought if they could get a

2:38

chance to address

2:40

a huge audience, it would

2:43

sort of defang the entire

2:45

process. The undergraduates are

2:47

very empathic of the graduate students,

2:49

who may be a little bit

2:51

nervous at the beginning. And that

2:54

shows and the students are with

2:56

them. It's

2:58

a very nice experience. Well,

3:00

it's something I started doing after seeing

3:03

that you had role modeled it. And

3:05

our PhD students just raved about it

3:07

as a very meaningful challenge and also

3:09

a chance for them to connect with

3:11

the students on a different level. Now,

3:15

I was reading a little bit about some of

3:17

the students who had gone through this experience with

3:19

you. And I came across a

3:21

hilarious story from a former TA

3:23

Larry White. Yeah, it

3:26

was a rainy day.

3:28

And there I am in the

3:30

auditorium waiting for Larry and he shows up

3:32

and he shows me his backside where he

3:35

slipped in the mud and

3:37

landed right on his ass. And

3:40

he's covered with mud. And he said,

3:42

I don't know

3:44

what to do. If the students

3:46

ever saw my backside, they would

3:49

be laughing at me. And

3:51

I said, no, no, no. What you have to

3:53

do is show them your

3:55

backside and tell them exactly

3:58

what happened. to

4:00

them that you were nervous about the lecture.

4:04

And that's one of the

4:06

reasons why you were distracted and you

4:08

slipped and fell. And then you turn

4:10

around and show them your backside, and

4:13

they will love you for it. And

4:16

he did that, and it worked exactly

4:18

that way. The students gave him a

4:21

standing ovation, and I think it was

4:23

partly because of that

4:25

pratfall. There's a great coda

4:28

to the story, which is, he

4:30

said a student came up to thank him

4:32

afterward, and that's how he met

4:34

his wife, Hester. I

4:37

did know that, and I have forgotten, but it is a

4:41

wonderful coda, and I happen

4:43

to know that it's a marriage that has

4:46

lasted for quite a while.

4:48

Yeah, I think about you, Elliot, every time I make

4:50

a mistake. I think your

4:52

brilliant paper on the pratfall effect really changed the

4:54

way that I think about mistakes. And I guess

4:57

a personal example was, I remember

5:01

right when I was getting ready to interview

5:03

for a job at Wharton, I was

5:05

invited to speak at a conference here about

5:07

a month beforehand. And I arrived

5:09

in Philadelphia and realized I had

5:12

forgotten to pack pants. All

5:15

I had was pajama pants and sweatpants.

5:18

And I think it was 10pm, and my talk

5:20

was at 8am the next morning. And there was

5:22

nothing I could do, except

5:24

I had a cousin who was in school here. He

5:27

was bigger than me, but I borrowed his pants. And

5:29

then I walked in to give the talk, and I

5:31

opened, thinking of you, with the story

5:33

of how I forgot my pants, and I really

5:35

wanted to apologize for how ridiculous I looked. I

5:38

felt like a clown. And

5:40

it was the warmest audience reception I had

5:42

ever gotten at an academic conference. So

5:45

unpack this for me. What did your research

5:47

show about why this is effective? Well,

5:50

what we think it showed

5:53

is that if a person

5:55

seems really terrific to begin

5:57

with, he may be a little little

6:00

intimidating. He may make the

6:03

people who are evaluating him

6:05

feel not so

6:08

good by comparison, but

6:10

when he makes a pratfall, when

6:13

he falls down or forgets his

6:15

pants or something like that, it

6:18

makes people feel closer to him.

6:20

Now the important thing

6:22

to notice is that a mediocre

6:25

person who has a pratfall

6:27

simply seems that much more

6:30

mediocre for having the

6:33

pratfall. One of the things that I

6:35

took away from this research was that

6:38

although we're often encouraged to

6:40

humanize ourselves and to show vulnerability,

6:43

we shouldn't forget to establish

6:45

our competence. And it's a

6:47

lot easier to get away with showing vulnerability if

6:50

you're successful or if you have high status

6:52

or if you have a track record of achievement.

6:55

It also reminds me of a recent paper led

6:57

by Alison Wood Brooks where

6:59

a whole series of experiments showed that if

7:02

you're successful, revealing your failures on the

7:04

path to success and even some of

7:06

your current failures reduces

7:08

malicious envy. So people are

7:10

more likely to admire you as opposed

7:13

to maybe being out to get you. I

7:15

think that's probably true,

7:17

yeah. I'm curious about how

7:19

you think about the pratfall effect today

7:21

in a world that seems

7:24

to place conflicting pressures on people. On the

7:26

one hand, we're supposed to be perfect and

7:28

flawless. On the other hand, I think

7:31

a lot of people prized authenticity and

7:33

vulnerability. Do you have

7:35

other thoughts on the differences between a good pratfall

7:38

and a bad pratfall? My main

7:40

feeling is one should never

7:42

fake a pratfall. You know,

7:44

if you forgot your pants

7:46

on purpose and

7:49

for some reason the

7:51

audience had a

7:53

clue that that might have happened, I

7:55

think that would be a disaster. You

7:58

don't want to fake it. are we

8:01

all have our vulnerabilities and it's

8:03

good to reveal them in

8:06

the normal course of events

8:08

without the motivation to

8:10

appear human. I know so

8:12

many people who take pride in being rational,

8:15

but what you taught me is

8:17

we're actually much better at rationalizing. Yeah.

8:21

I'd love to hear how did you realize that?

8:23

When did you first come to that discovery? I

8:25

think we are rational beings. We

8:28

are capable of rational behavior,

8:30

but much more

8:32

powerful is our desire to

8:34

rationalize, our need to rationalize.

8:39

Dissonance reduction is

8:41

neutral. It's a tool and

8:44

it has some uses. It helps us

8:46

sleep at night. You

8:48

make an important decision, like a financial decision,

8:50

like what kind of car to buy or

8:53

what kind of house to buy or anything

8:55

like that. And it's a big decision, it's

8:57

an important decision. And

9:00

if it's not perfect, then you're, oh

9:02

gee, should I have bought the other

9:04

one? And

9:06

Dissonance Reduction plugs in. You

9:09

convince yourself it is the best house

9:11

or the best car or the best

9:13

whatever, the best woman that

9:16

you married. Once

9:18

you start reducing dissonance, you can sleep

9:20

at night and that makes you a

9:22

healthier person. That can also

9:24

be used for self-deception.

9:27

It can be used to justify

9:29

a war and to justify sending

9:32

more troops and more troops and

9:34

more troops as you surge

9:36

the war, which is going

9:38

to end up very badly anyway, and

9:41

people get killed. So

9:43

when a person makes a mistake, a

9:46

bad mistake, a cognitive blunder,

9:49

or causes pain to

9:51

an innocent bystander for no good

9:53

reason, I think

9:55

that we try to

9:57

justify that because The

10:01

most powerful distance occurs

10:04

when I do something that

10:06

goes against my own self-concept,

10:09

that belies my own self-concept.

10:11

And that's the irony of

10:13

it, that in order

10:16

to make myself feel better, I

10:18

then set that person up for

10:20

even greater harm because I've now

10:22

convinced myself that he deserved all

10:25

the nasty things I had done

10:27

to him in the past. That's

10:30

one of the dynamics I want to talk more about. I

10:32

remember in Mistakes Were Made, but not by

10:34

me, you wrote about

10:36

some chilling examples of self-justification

10:39

to reduce dissidents. I was

10:41

particularly struck by prosecutors who

10:43

refused to recognize DNA evidence

10:45

that exonerated people they had

10:48

put in prison. I mean, that's

10:50

unfathomable. Because the

10:53

exquisite irony is

10:56

that the prosecutor who

10:58

refuses to reopen the case,

11:01

he's not refusing to reopen

11:03

the case because he's a

11:05

terrible person. He's

11:07

refusing to reopen the case because

11:10

he thinks of himself as

11:13

a good person and a

11:15

competent person who would never,

11:17

ever send the wrong man

11:19

to prison. So if

11:21

he sent somebody to prison for

11:23

10 years and then DNA evidence

11:26

shows up that could

11:28

exonerate that person, he refuses to

11:30

look at it because he sees

11:32

himself as a good and competent

11:34

person who would never send the

11:37

wrong man to prison for 10

11:39

years. Every time I

11:41

hear stories like this, I want

11:43

to sit people down and say, listen, just

11:46

because you're a good person doesn't mean you're not capable

11:48

of doing a bad thing. the

12:00

underlying reasons why people do

12:03

things that would

12:05

seem bizarre without a good theory

12:07

that helps explain how that

12:09

works. You're reminding me of

12:12

when I was early in grad school, Phoebe

12:14

Ellsworth said her definition of social

12:16

psychology was all the forces that ruin your

12:18

life every day. That's very good. That's

12:22

very good. Or

12:25

enhance our lives every day. What

12:28

we've been talking about is something

12:30

that we could call a vicious

12:32

circle. And it can

12:34

work in the other direction also. You

12:37

do somebody a favor. One

12:40

of my students, David Landy, did this

12:42

as an experiment. Oh, this

12:44

is Jekyll and Landy. I love that paper. It's

12:47

a great paper. And it shows

12:49

that what's important

12:52

is the giving. If

12:55

you do a favor for someone and

12:57

you don't particularly like that person

12:59

or dislike that person, to

13:02

begin with, in effect you're asking

13:04

yourself, well, how come I did

13:06

this great favor for that person?

13:09

He must be a terrific guy. I

13:12

see some really nice things about him.

13:14

He really deserves the favor I gave

13:16

him. Really

13:18

deserves all the things I did

13:20

for him. And therefore, it

13:23

increases the probability that you'll do

13:26

nice things for him in the future. And that

13:28

is one of

13:30

the underlying dynamics of

13:32

the Jigsaw classroom, which

13:35

is a series of experiments I once

13:38

did several years ago. Which

13:40

I hope we're going to get to. The idea that

13:43

you don't have to like someone to do them

13:45

a favor, but after you've done a favor for

13:47

them, you're likely to convince yourself that they're worth

13:49

liking. It's such

13:52

a fun example of how sometimes

13:54

dissonance can actually lead to good

13:56

things, not just bad things. I

13:59

want to ask you more about that. were

16:00

giving to a person who was

16:02

complaining that he had a bad heart.

16:05

Quite a powerful demonstration. If

16:08

you had started the experiment at 400 volts, people

16:11

say, no way. Absolutely not.

16:13

Exactly. Exactly. Imagine

16:15

going up to someone and saying, I'd

16:18

like you to give that innocent person 400 volts

16:21

of electricity. You'd look at them as if

16:23

you were crazy, but two out of three

16:25

people, one step at

16:27

a time justifying each improvement,

16:30

each improvement, strange

16:34

use of the word improvement,

16:36

each increase in voltage

16:39

because it's not that different from the

16:42

previous one. And you

16:44

look at that and you can see that

16:46

that is a model for what

16:49

seems to us as bizarre behavior

16:53

and how it can come about. It's

16:57

a replication of the out group

16:59

can always be vilified and

17:02

we can always justify harming

17:04

them in really important ways, whether

17:07

the out group happens to be

17:09

Israelis or happens to be

17:11

Palestinians. It happens and

17:14

it's still happening. It

17:16

is part of human nature, but it's

17:19

human nature that can be

17:21

overcome by tuning into the

17:23

possibilities of the virtuous

17:26

circle, which could reverse some

17:28

of the thinking, some of the

17:31

rationalizing and justification that

17:34

goes into the vicious circle, which

17:36

produces all this negative

17:38

stereotyping and negative behavior.

17:41

And we're doing it in this country. Thinking

17:44

about the virtuous cycles, what

17:46

does the psychology of cognitive dissonance teach

17:49

us about how to

17:51

fight this kind of victim blaming and

17:53

dehumanization and are there insights

17:55

about how we can solve it? I

17:58

go back to... some of the

18:00

research I did when we developed the

18:03

notion of the jigsaw classroom. I

18:05

was living in Austin,

18:07

Texas at the time. I was teaching

18:09

at the University of Texas. When

18:12

the Texas schools were desegregated finally

18:14

in 1971, most of us thought,

18:16

gee, that

18:20

will lead to great outcomes, a reduction

18:22

in presence, because if

18:25

people are segregated in schools

18:27

and residentially black kids, white

18:29

kids, Mexican-American kids, don't get

18:31

to see much of each

18:33

other, and therefore they can

18:36

build up the stereotypes. But

18:39

if you bring them together, it should

18:41

result in good things. But we

18:44

should have known better, because

18:47

it depends on how you bring them

18:49

together. And when

18:51

the Austin schools were desegregated, the

18:53

kids from minority

18:56

residential areas were simply bused

18:58

into the schools

19:02

of the white middle class, and

19:05

their prior preparation was

19:09

not very good. And

19:11

the classroom is a highly competitive situation,

19:15

where the minority kids

19:17

were guaranteed to lose. They

19:20

didn't know the answer when the teacher would stand

19:22

in front of the room and ask a question.

19:25

They tended not to participate after

19:28

a while, which

19:30

tended to exacerbate whatever existing

19:33

stereotypes were there. And

19:36

we were called in by the school

19:38

superintendent. I was asked if

19:41

I had any ideas as to how we

19:44

could change things, because there were fistfights breaking

19:47

out. There was a lot of aggression. Far

19:50

from being a good thing, it looked

19:53

like desegregating the schools only

19:55

brought people together who were going to fight with

19:57

each other. And what we did...

20:01

On the spot, my graduate students

20:03

and I invented a situation

20:05

which forced kids to

20:08

cooperate with each other. And

20:10

we called it the Jigsaw classroom because

20:13

the way it was set up, we would have

20:15

small groups

20:17

of five or six people, different

20:21

races and genders, as

20:24

diverse as we could possibly make each

20:26

group. And they

20:28

each had one piece of a

20:30

puzzle, like if it

20:33

was the biography of Eleanor

20:35

Roosevelt, they each had a

20:38

separate section of her life from childhood

20:40

all the way to old age. And

20:43

the only access you would

20:45

have to the entire thing

20:48

would be to listen to

20:51

each kid report on

20:53

his section. To

20:55

make a long story very short, it forced

20:58

kids to listen to each other.

21:00

It forced kids to pay attention

21:03

to each other. And

21:05

within six weeks, that group was

21:07

functioning like a really good

21:09

basketball team, where it didn't matter who

21:11

put the ball in the hoop, you

21:13

pass the ball around until you find

21:16

the open man. The

21:18

results were spectacular on

21:20

the initial experiments. And every time we've

21:22

tried to replicate it, it's come out

21:24

in a very much the same way.

21:27

Presidents went down, liking for

21:30

school went up, absenteeism

21:32

decreased and general

21:34

empathy increased because

21:37

the individual kids, their

21:40

individual differences became important.

21:42

I've always thought that part of what's clever about

21:44

your Jigsaw classroom design is that there's

21:47

not just a common goal, you actually have to

21:49

rely on each other and help each other to

21:51

achieve it. I like that too.

21:53

In a Jigsaw group, each kid had

21:56

one assignment like Eleanor

21:58

Roosevelt's early years. We

22:00

took time and everybody

22:03

with Eleanor Roosevelt's early

22:05

years met together

22:08

outside of their Jigsaw group but

22:10

in what was called an expert

22:12

group, and they went over the

22:14

material together. They shared it with

22:17

each other. They talked about ways

22:19

they would have of presenting it

22:21

so that kids who might have been, had

22:24

a little more difficulty with the

22:27

presentation, were learning from

22:30

the other people who had the

22:32

same paragraph to report, and

22:34

therefore when they went back into their

22:36

Jigsaw group, they were

22:38

armed that would make it less likely

22:40

that they would drop that fly ball.

22:43

I was thinking about the Jigsaw classroom recently

22:46

when I was reading a paper by Shannon

22:48

White, Juliana Schroeder, and Jane Risen where

22:51

they studied Israeli and Palestinian

22:53

teenagers at Seeds of Peace

22:55

camp, which I'm

22:57

sure you've been familiar with for a long time, and

22:59

I was stunned to read that

23:02

just sharing a dialogue group, having

23:05

that common activity, made participants 15

23:08

times more likely to develop a

23:10

close friendship. And it seems

23:12

like you anticipated that finding. It

23:14

was always the hope. I

23:16

wouldn't even call it the anticipation.

23:18

It was the hope, and it

23:20

does work that way, especially

23:23

with kids. The younger the kids, the

23:25

easier it is to

23:27

achieve that before the prejudices

23:30

have hardened. Right. I was

23:33

riveted by your finding that the people we

23:35

like most are not necessarily the people we

23:37

liked all along, but the people

23:39

we started out disliking and then grew to

23:41

like. Is that part of what's

23:43

going on in the Jigsaw case? The

23:46

mind is an interesting thing, and

23:48

there are various theoretical ways to

23:50

get at what's happening, but

23:53

there's a consistency to all

23:57

of that stuff that these things

23:59

are all. than

30:00

the average person, and that we

30:02

have more integrity than the average

30:05

person. Now you're confronting some people

30:08

with the fact that behaving hypocritically

30:11

and they want to get

30:13

back to behaving

30:15

with integrity. And

30:18

how do you get back to behaving

30:20

with integrity? You start using condoms. And

30:22

that's what we found. That we

30:25

found a sharp increase in the use of

30:27

condoms. And six months later,

30:30

when a pool was conducted

30:33

by us, but it seemed to

30:35

be coming from someone else, six

30:37

months later, the people in

30:40

the hypocrisy condition were

30:42

still using condoms. That

30:45

was self-persuasion rather than

30:47

the persuasion that was being used

30:50

by the public health center at

30:52

my university, for example, giving

30:55

lectures, showing videos about using

30:57

condoms, giving out pamphlets, all

31:00

of that stuff, increased

31:02

the use of condoms by about 2%, from 17%

31:04

to 19%,

31:08

whereas we were getting more than 60%

31:12

using condoms six months later.

31:15

That was powerful data. I took two

31:18

things away from that research. The first

31:20

lesson was that if you

31:22

want to convince somebody, you should get them

31:24

involved in making the argument that you want

31:26

them to believe, not just telling

31:28

them the argument you want them to believe. And

31:32

then the second was that we should be

31:35

careful about the arguments we make,

31:37

because when you're persuading someone else, the

31:39

person you're most likely to convince is

31:41

yourself. That's absolutely right. Are

31:48

you up for a lightning round with some short questions?

31:51

I'm up for it. What is the worst

31:53

piece of advice you've ever gotten? The

31:56

worst piece of advice I've ever gotten was

31:58

going go to college,

32:01

go to work. Wow, I'm glad you

32:03

ignored that one. Support your mother. Yeah,

32:05

it was right after my father died. I was

32:08

a junior in high school. My brother

32:10

was already in college, so my aunts

32:13

and uncles were saying, well,

32:15

Elliot, when he graduates from

32:18

high school, can go

32:20

work for the Ford Motor Company

32:22

on the assembly line. And

32:24

the pay is really good. You can

32:27

support his mother that way, etc. And

32:29

I looked at it and I thought, hey, that was a pretty

32:32

good idea. I'd be making a

32:34

couple of hundred dollars a week. And

32:36

my brother said, screw that.

32:39

Elliot's going to college and we

32:42

can handle it. And that was

32:44

the end of it. My brother, he really

32:46

did save me from a life working for

32:48

the Ford Motor Company. Is

32:50

there a favorite psychology book that you'd

32:52

like to recommend? Either of

32:54

the Nisbet Ross books. I like them a

32:56

lot. What is something you've

32:59

rethought lately? I

33:02

have to admit, I haven't rethought

33:04

very much lately. I guess

33:07

when you get to be an old guy, you're

33:09

busy collecting the thoughts you do

33:11

have rather than changing much.

33:14

I don't think I've thought very much.

33:17

Elliot, is there a question you have for

33:19

me? How does it feel to

33:21

be a psychologist

33:24

in a business school rather than

33:26

in a liberal arts department? What

33:29

are the advantages? What are the disadvantages?

33:32

Oh, I don't think anyone's asked

33:34

me that before. When I got to

33:36

Michigan for my doctorate

33:38

in psychology, I wanted to be an

33:40

organizational psychologist. And I felt like the

33:42

more basic scholars were sort

33:44

of looking down their nose at me. Like,

33:46

why aren't you doing neuroscience or physiology work?

33:49

Why do you care about people's jobs and

33:51

doing applied research? And

33:54

coming to a business school has obviously changed that. I

33:56

think there's an expectation that we're going to do practical

33:59

work. That was one of the

34:01

big points of inspiration that I took from your work was

34:04

seeing how you went into the real world and said,

34:06

I want to see if I can improve people's lives

34:08

using the tools of psychology. Interesting.

34:11

Yeah. If you were to make

34:13

your Mount Rushmore of psychology, who would be on

34:15

it? There'd be Kurt Lewin

34:18

and Leon Festinger,

34:20

for sure. I

34:22

would put in B.F. Skinner and

34:27

just for diversity, maybe

34:29

Carl Rogers. Paul

34:33

Mill. What's

34:35

the idea from psychology that has most been

34:38

useful in your life outside of dissonance?

34:42

What Gordon Alport wrote in

34:45

Nature of Prejudice about what

34:47

needs to happen in

34:50

order for prejudice to be reduced?

34:53

He actually listed all of the

34:55

things that went

34:57

into the Jigsaw classroom. It

35:00

wasn't just bringing kids together

35:02

in the same school. It

35:05

was sanctioned by

35:07

authority, working together

35:09

to achieve a common goal. All

35:14

of that stuff, I think,

35:16

was really an important

35:18

position that he took way back in 1954.

35:23

People ignored it because desegregation

35:26

really didn't work the way it

35:28

was supposed to. I

35:30

guess one thing I'm curious about is how do

35:32

you deal with cognitive dissonance personally?

35:35

Me, if I'm making an

35:37

important decision, especially one that

35:40

involves other people and

35:42

could cause pain to other people, I

35:45

really have to ask myself, is

35:47

this something I really believe in

35:49

or am I simply acting in

35:51

a way that justifies a previous

35:54

decision and helps me

35:56

feel better about myself? I

35:59

think knowing about dissonance and knowing

36:01

how susceptible I am to

36:04

reducing dissonance and

36:06

then finding out later that I was

36:09

doing that. In

36:11

some cases, it's harmless, but

36:13

in some cases, it could be

36:16

very hurtful to

36:19

others. In the cases where

36:21

other people's happiness is involved,

36:23

like when I was actively

36:26

teaching, I

36:28

would make a decision, do I want to work

36:31

with this person or not? And

36:33

if somebody who started to work with me, and

36:35

they really weren't panning out, and I had

36:39

to be sure that my

36:41

decisions were based

36:43

on specific instances that I

36:46

could document if I had

36:48

to, rather than I just

36:50

didn't like the guy very much or something

36:52

like that. I would do

36:54

a lot of questioning in

36:57

advance of a decision, sort of

36:59

trying out the decision one way and the

37:01

other way and seeing how that made me

37:03

feel, what are

37:06

all the possible dissonance

37:08

reductions, self-justification aspects that

37:11

I really need to

37:13

take into account. I would do

37:15

a lot of that. When we

37:17

first moved to Minneapolis, and

37:19

we bought a house, the first house we

37:22

ever bought, we

37:24

didn't have any money. And so

37:26

there were only two houses that we

37:29

liked and that we could afford,

37:31

and they couldn't have been

37:33

more different. One was close to

37:35

the university. It was

37:37

my dream to live walking distance from

37:39

campus so that my graduate students could

37:41

come over at four o'clock in the

37:44

afternoon. We could drink some

37:46

scotch or coffee and

37:48

talk research. But it

37:51

didn't have much of a yard. The other one

37:53

was out in the suburbs, but

37:55

it had a big backyard and it was

37:57

pleasant and okay, and it was near a

37:59

lake. we

38:02

ended up buying the house

38:04

in the suburbs and I had a

38:06

lot of dissonance about that because if

38:08

I was living alone I would have bought the

38:11

one close to school but given

38:13

the fact that we had four kids it

38:15

was the best decision to

38:18

make but I still

38:20

had a lot of dissonance about it. This

38:22

was in early December I

38:25

was in my office and I

38:27

saw an ad in the newspaper about

38:30

an old town canoe used for sale

38:34

that somebody was selling and

38:37

I bought the canoe I put it on

38:39

top of my car on the luggage rack,

38:42

drove home in December and Vera

38:45

my wife was looking out the window

38:47

of the kitchen

38:50

as I drove into the driveway with the

38:52

canoe on top of the car and she

38:54

burst out laughing. One

38:56

might say that the canoe sat in

38:58

our garage doing nothing but taking up

39:00

space for six

39:02

months until I could use it but

39:05

I would say it did a lot more than take

39:07

up space. It helped me feel

39:10

better and therefore sleep better

39:12

at night about having chosen

39:15

the house in the suburbs because

39:17

we had this canoe. That's a

39:19

great story. It's a simple thing

39:23

and yet it's really important and

39:25

we do that all the time and we can ignore it.

39:29

It doesn't matter but

39:31

there's so many dissonance reduction things

39:34

that do matter and those are the

39:36

things we really have to scrutinize and

39:39

the more you know about dissonance

39:41

the less confident I am that

39:44

my decisions are made for exactly

39:46

the right reasons. That

39:48

seems like the kind of intellectual humility we can

39:50

all use more of. Okay finally

39:53

on the personal front I love this note

39:55

from your son Joshua. I'm just going to

39:57

read it to you and let you react.

40:00

He said that you and your

40:03

wife Vera have been really happy together for over

40:05

70 years. He says,

40:08

really, they are still completely in love. When

40:10

COVID hit and they had to be quarantined together, just

40:12

the two of them, it was as if they were

40:14

given the second honeymoon at the age of 90. So

40:17

what advice do you have in this day and age

40:20

about choosing a life partner, building a

40:22

life together and making love last? I

40:25

don't give advice. If I

40:27

were giving advice, I would say, you want

40:30

to have a happy marriage but last? Marry

40:33

Vera. Vera

40:36

is magnificent. And it was the

40:38

happiest day of my life when

40:40

I met her and we became

40:42

friends. And Maslow hired

40:45

the two of us. We were

40:47

his two favorite students, mostly Vera,

40:49

a little bit me. And

40:52

we worked together and we

40:54

fell in love. And it was just beautiful.

40:57

She's smart. She has

41:00

serenity. I've

41:02

never met anyone like her. The

41:05

interesting thing to me is that when

41:07

I was 20 years old, I was convinced that

41:09

I would never get married. And

41:12

whenever I was dating

41:14

a woman and we started to get a

41:16

little bit serious with each other, I would

41:19

make that announcement. I hope you realize that

41:21

I'm never going to get married. Because

41:25

my parents were very unhappily married.

41:27

I always thought of marriage as

41:29

an unnatural state of affairs,

41:31

to be spending your whole life with

41:33

one other person and trying

41:36

to keep out of each other's way and making

41:40

mistakes and getting on each

41:42

other's nerves and embarrassing each

41:44

other. I mean, it

41:46

didn't seem right to me. And then when

41:49

I was 21, I met Vera. And

41:52

when I was 22, we got married. If

41:54

I were giving advice, I would say,

41:57

learn how to communicate, learn

41:59

about information. interpersonal communication. Learn

42:03

not to judge or

42:05

criticize when there

42:07

are disagreements, and there will always

42:09

be disagreements. You know, a

42:12

lot of people will think that a

42:14

good marriage is one where you never

42:16

argue or never disagree. Of course you're

42:18

going to argue and disagree. How can

42:20

two people live together without disagreeing about

42:22

things? But the idea

42:25

is to do it civilly and

42:27

with care. And it's how you

42:29

argue and how you discuss things

42:32

that's really important. How you

42:34

disagree. She's

42:37

my best friend. She's always been my best friend.

42:39

Josh was wrong, by the way. It's only been

42:41

69 years, not 70. I've been very lucky.

42:46

Lucky with the

42:48

choice. Lucky that she

42:50

loved me as much as I

42:53

love her, which astonishes me. And

42:56

lucky that we're both

42:58

relatively healthy

43:02

in our 90s, so

43:04

that it's lasted a long time

43:06

and will continue, I hope. So

43:09

that's beautifully put. And it

43:12

sounds like she forced you to overcome some

43:14

dissonance in order to get married. She

43:17

didn't do anything, you know. When

43:19

I made that pronouncement to her, she

43:21

just saw the smile. She

43:25

knew it wouldn't last, I think. Well,

43:28

Elliot, I can't thank you enough for taking

43:31

the time to share your wisdom today. I've

43:33

long looked up to the way you do

43:35

your work as a psychologist,

43:38

a teacher, a mentor. But

43:41

I think I have even greater admiration for

43:43

how you live your life. Well, thank you.

43:46

It was a pleasure talking to you. I really

43:48

enjoyed it. I assure you the pleasure

43:50

was all mine, and I can't wait to share

43:52

it with our listeners. I

43:55

can't wait to hear what I

43:57

had to say. I

44:05

think the major lesson of Elliot's work

44:07

is that the world needs more rationality

44:09

and less rationalizing. Rationalizing

44:12

is searching for justifications after you've reached

44:14

an opinion or decision. Rationality

44:17

is seeking the best logic and data

44:19

before you commit and staying open to

44:22

changing your mind. Rethinking

44:30

is hosted by me, Adam Grant,

44:32

and produced by Tien with Cosmic

44:34

Standard. Our team includes Colin Helm,

44:36

Eliza Smith, Jacob Winnick, Asia Simpson,

44:38

Samaya Adams, Michelle Quinn, Fan Bantang,

44:40

Hannah Kingsley Ma, Julia Dickerson, and

44:42

Whitney Pennington Rogers. This episode

44:44

was produced and mixed by Cosmic Standard.

44:46

Our fact checker is Paul Durbin, original

44:48

music by Hans-Eil Su and Alison Leighton-Brown.

44:56

I remember reading it not by chance alone that

44:58

Sesinger had quite a line about Maslow.

45:00

We were in a bar having a

45:02

drink and he said, by the

45:05

way Elliot, how did you ever get interested

45:07

in psychology? And I said,

45:09

well, you know, I happened to wander

45:11

into this class being taught by this

45:14

guy, Abraham Maslow. Maslow

45:16

was the guy who

45:18

got you interested in

45:21

psychology. That guy's ideas

45:23

are so bad they're not even wrong.

45:25

But of course what he meant was

45:27

you couldn't test them and he

45:29

was absolutely right. Did Maslow have

45:31

an equally devastating comment on Fesinger?

45:34

Well, it was devastating. It

45:36

wasn't quite as clever. He said,

45:38

well, who are you working with

45:40

out there at Stanford? And

45:42

I thought, oh, this guy, Leon Fesinger,

45:45

he said, Fesinger, that

45:47

bastard, how can you stand

45:49

him? Do you ever

45:52

feel like your laptop just keeps

45:54

going but you are completely drained?

45:56

I think a lot of us

45:58

don't realize how much pain. pain

46:00

we live in because of our interactions

46:02

with computing. NPR's Body

46:04

Electric, a special interactive series

46:07

investigating how to fix the

46:09

relationship between our tech and

46:11

our health. Listen in

46:13

the TED Radio Hour feed wherever you get

46:15

your podcasts.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features