Podchaser Logo
Home
Where Do Feelings Come From?

Where Do Feelings Come From?

Released Monday, 8th January 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Where Do Feelings Come From?

Where Do Feelings Come From?

Where Do Feelings Come From?

Where Do Feelings Come From?

Monday, 8th January 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

This is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.

0:03

In Anthony Doar's wonderful novel, All the

0:06

Light We Cannot See, a

0:08

radio host poses a question to an

0:10

audience of children. The

0:13

brain is locked in total darkness, he

0:15

says. It floats in

0:17

a clear liquid inside the skull, never

0:19

in the light. And yet the

0:21

world it constructs in the mind is full of

0:24

light. It brims with

0:26

color and movement. So

0:28

how, children, does the brain, which lives without

0:30

a spark of light, build

0:33

for us a world full of light?

0:37

It's not just about light, of course. The

0:39

world inside our heads is full of sound,

0:42

movement and sensation. It

0:46

is suffused with feelings and emotion.

0:50

Imagine for a moment that your brain was

0:52

a person locked inside your head. How

0:54

does this person create a world so rich, so

0:57

varied and so beautiful, when

0:59

she is permanently trapped within the cage of

1:02

your skull? Most

1:04

of us have already answered. The

1:06

brain has many messengers that bring

1:09

it information. Signals stream

1:11

in from our eyes and ears and

1:13

skin. The brain takes in all

1:15

these signals, and like a film

1:17

editor splicing together a movie, assembles

1:20

our perceptions of the world. But

1:25

in recent years, some scientists have come to

1:27

believe that this is not what

1:29

actually happens. The light we

1:31

see and the sounds we hear are

1:34

not really comprised of signals from the outside world.

1:37

Instead, they are mostly

1:39

creations of the mind itself. When

1:43

I first heard this idea, it made

1:45

little sense to me. But then I

1:48

came by some interesting experiments. For

1:50

example, can you make out what I'm saying

1:52

here? I know, but I

1:54

can't see. I'll plug the plug in in 2015.

2:00

right? Here's what I said. The

2:03

novel, All the Light We Cannot

2:05

See, won the Pulitzer Prize in 2015.

2:09

Now, let me play you the same clip as before. If

2:12

you can now make out the

2:14

words, it's not because your mind

2:16

deciphered the audio. You tried

2:23

doing that, and it sounded like gibberish.

2:26

You can do it now, because your

2:28

brain knew what was coming. It

2:30

predicted what it was going to hear. As

2:35

we will explore today, the brain is doing

2:37

this in every domain. What

2:39

you hear, yes, but also

2:41

what you see, what you touch, what

2:43

you smell. This

2:46

week on Hidden Brain, the story of

2:48

a scientist who has spent years studying

2:50

how the brain constructs reality and

2:52

the surprising implications of our ideas

2:55

for our emotional lives. Lisa

3:09

Feldman Barrett grew up in Toronto, Canada.

3:12

It was the late 1960s. Her dad was out

3:15

of the picture, so her mom raised her. Lisa

3:18

would spend every weekend with her

3:20

maternal grandparents. When

3:22

she was five, her mom remarried. Lisa

3:25

vividly remembers something that happened with her new

3:27

stepfather on the day of the wedding. I

3:30

remember his parents were there,

3:32

and I remember his parents saying

3:35

to me, so I think I had met

3:37

them maybe once or twice. I didn't really know them at

3:39

all. I remember my stepgrandmother saying

3:41

to me, well, go over to

3:43

your grandparents and congratulate

3:45

them on this wedding. Now, she's saying

3:48

this to a five-year-old. What does a

3:50

five-year-old know? I remember saying

3:52

to her, why do I have to do that? I

3:54

don't have to go congratulate my Zaydi, or that's what

3:56

I used to call him. It's the Yiddish word for

3:58

grandfather. make

6:00

someone feel guilty for their own

6:02

bad behavior, in which case if

6:04

you're being held responsible for their

6:06

feelings, then you pay

6:09

the price. You

6:11

said a couple of times that

6:13

your stepfather held you accountable for

6:16

your mother's feelings, and you also

6:18

just said that your stepfather might

6:20

hold you accountable for his feelings. Talk

6:23

about that idea for a moment. How did

6:25

he pick up on the sense that you were responsible for

6:27

how they felt? Well, he would

6:29

say it. He would say, you made your mother

6:32

feel embarrassed. You

6:34

embarrassed your mother. You made your mother angry. You

6:36

made your mother sad. For

6:39

example, when I was 12, I lived

6:42

in a Jewish area. My

6:44

family is Jewish. Everyone was

6:46

having bar mitzvahs and bat mitzvahs. I personally wasn't

6:48

because I didn't go to Hebrew school. We

6:51

couldn't afford that. But everyone

6:53

was going to bar mitzvahs and bat mitzvahs. It's

6:55

not crazy like it is now where people are

6:58

spending $100,000 or whatever on these massive

7:01

parties. But there was

7:03

still a party that you had to go to and

7:06

a present that you had to give. You had

7:08

to wear a dress. You needed shoes. I

7:10

didn't have dresses. I didn't have shoes. I

7:12

had one pair of shoes. They

7:15

weren't party shoes. I

7:18

remember I wrote

7:20

a little note to my mother on a

7:22

piece of paper and I decorated it with

7:26

– I like to draw, so I

7:28

decorated it with all sorts of flowers

7:30

and balloons and party things. I

7:32

said, can I have a pair of party shoes? I

7:35

slipped it under the door when she was in the

7:37

bathroom. I honestly don't

7:39

think that my mother would say to my

7:41

stepfather, she made me feel this way. I'm

7:45

guessing, but knowing my mother the

7:47

way that I do, I would expect that she

7:49

would say, I feel

7:51

bad that we don't have the money to buy

7:54

her a pair of shoes. Let's

7:59

just say that I didn't go to a party. bar

8:01

mitzvahs or bar mitzvahs for like a month, which

8:04

made me very unpopular because

8:06

I had already RSVP'd that I

8:08

was going to these parties and then at

8:10

the last minute sort of had to cancel

8:12

because I was grounded. Wait, so

8:15

your stepfather grounded you because you sent this note

8:17

to your mother? Yeah, like I slipped

8:19

it under the door asking, can

8:21

I please have a pair of party shoes? Yeah.

8:25

The response was, you know,

8:27

probably came like a day later and

8:31

it was swift and intense. As

8:37

you can tell, Lisa had a strained

8:39

relationship with her stepfather. She

8:41

was also something of an outlier in her family.

8:44

She was the first to go to college and then

8:46

to graduate school. But

8:48

our story today is not about

8:50

parent-child relationships or even about

8:53

the particulars of Lisa's own childhood. The

8:56

reason these stories are relevant to our episode today

8:58

is because they illustrate an idea that

9:01

is ubiquitous in all of our lives.

9:04

You probably had times in your own

9:06

life when someone told you that you

9:08

made them feel sad or angry or

9:10

happy, that you were the cause

9:12

of their emotions, that you were responsible

9:14

for how they feel. You

9:17

have surely felt this way about others. Someone

9:19

cuts you off in traffic and you say that

9:21

the other driver made you upset. A

9:24

friend brings over some food when you are

9:26

sick and you say your friend has comforted

9:28

you. We say that a winning

9:30

sports team has cheered us up and

9:32

that a losing sports team has brought us down.

9:37

It certainly feels as though our minds

9:39

are taking in signals from the outside

9:41

world and assembling our internal world, that

9:45

our emotions are caused by the things that

9:47

happen to us. But

9:50

as Lisa went on to become a psychologist

9:53

and neuroscientist, she was to

9:55

discover that our feelings are not, in

9:57

fact, responses to the world. They

10:00

are really predictions about the

10:02

world. She began

10:04

to ask herself a question. What

10:06

happens if we change

10:08

those predictions? You're

10:18

listening to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar

10:20

Vedanta. When

10:30

high school students start to learn to conduct experiments, their

10:34

teachers usually tell them to start by repeating

10:36

or replicating famous experiments from the past. The

10:40

physicist Isaac Newton, for example, discovered a

10:43

long time ago that heavier objects

10:45

do not fall faster than

10:47

lighter objects. All objects

10:49

on Earth experience gravity, and

10:52

this gravitational wave is not a problem. All

10:55

objects on Earth experience gravity, and

10:58

this gravitational force is constant regardless

11:00

of whether the object is heavy

11:03

or light. When

11:05

I was in high school, I remember teachers

11:08

showing us how to replicate Newton's experiment using

11:10

two balls of different weight that roll

11:13

down in incline. When

11:15

Lisa Feldman Barrett started working on her PhD,

11:18

she decided to replicate some famous experiments.

11:22

She was planning to become a clinician and

11:24

help people suffering with anxiety and depression, but

11:26

she also enjoyed doing research. The

11:29

theory that I was working with was

11:31

something called self-discrepancy theory. There were a

11:34

set of very simple experiments

11:36

that had been published where

11:38

people were just asked to

11:41

list the attributes or properties of

11:43

who they thought they were. You

11:46

know, I'm a nice person. I'm an honest person. I'm

11:49

a complex person. I'm

11:51

a whatever. And then the

11:53

features of who their ideal self.

11:56

And then you can compute the similarity

11:58

or... dissimilarity and

12:00

then you can also ask

12:03

people how they feel. The

12:05

idea was that if there were mismatches

12:07

between how people described themselves and who

12:09

they wanted to be, this

12:11

would make people sad. As part

12:13

of the experiment, Lisa had to ask people

12:15

how they felt and carefully

12:17

distinguish emotions like sadness from

12:20

emotions like anxiety. That's

12:22

what I did in the first couple of studies where

12:24

I was asking people in different ways

12:26

or I was attempting to measure

12:29

emotion in different ways, but

12:32

I was never able to replicate

12:34

those experiments. Lisa's

12:38

volunteers seemed to have a hard time

12:40

accurately classifying how they felt. As

12:43

a graduate student, Lisa figured she had

12:45

not run the experiment properly. She

12:47

tried again and again and

12:50

again. Well, I thought, well, I must

12:52

be doing something wrong. Maybe

12:54

I'm not sampling properly or

12:57

maybe I'm sampling people at the

12:59

wrong time of the semester. After

13:05

she failed for the eighth time, Lisa said,

13:08

okay, I am clearly missing something here.

13:11

What am I not getting? I went

13:13

back and I looked at the eight experiments

13:15

that had failed and I realized, oh, actually,

13:19

the reason why they're failing

13:21

was that when people are reporting

13:24

how they feel, how much sadness

13:26

do you feel? How hopeless do

13:28

you feel? How depressed do

13:30

you feel? You're basically giving

13:32

them a set of words and you're asking

13:34

them to describe their feelings. You

13:38

can do the same thing with anxiety. How anxious do

13:40

you feel? How jittery do you feel? How

13:43

fearful do you feel? And so on and so forth. What

13:46

I noticed was that when people were

13:50

reporting that they were feeling

13:52

intense sadness, they were also

13:55

reporting that they were feeling

13:57

intense anxiety. When people

13:59

reported that they were feeling calm,

14:02

they were also reporting that they were

14:04

feeling happy. So what was

14:06

happening here in these eight

14:08

studies is that people were reporting

14:11

that they felt both sad

14:13

and anxious or that they felt neither

14:15

of those emotions, that they felt calm

14:18

and happy. Basically,

14:20

people were using sadness

14:23

and anxiety as synonyms for I

14:25

feel like crap. And

14:29

this was happening across eight different

14:31

studies. And so I thought, well,

14:33

that's the problem. The

14:35

problem is that people

14:38

are not reporting accurately

14:40

how they're feeling. This

14:43

was why the results of the studies were muddled.

14:46

If people reported feeling sad when they

14:48

really should have said they were feeling

14:50

anxious or vice versa, the

14:52

researchers wouldn't be able to

14:54

tell how mismatches between people's

14:56

ideal cells and actual cells

14:58

produce sadness. If I

15:00

want to measure emotion, I've got to find a way to measure

15:03

how they actually feel. Then

15:07

I'll be able to properly test

15:09

the hypothesis of the self-discrepancy hypothesis.

15:12

And maybe I might also figure

15:14

out why is it that people

15:16

are having trouble separating

15:18

anxiety and sadness? Because

15:21

everyone knows that anxiety and

15:24

depression, sadness and fear, that

15:26

these are different emotion categories.

15:31

And I became just captivated

15:34

and intrigued by this new problem

15:36

that I had encountered. In her

15:38

third year of graduate school,

15:40

Lisa experienced something in her

15:42

personal life that

15:46

matched the experience of her volunteers. There

15:50

was someone at the university who, you know, kept

15:52

asking me out for college. Coffee

16:00

or for dinner on.

16:03

I. Just didn't find this person very

16:06

appealing I guess and I wasn't

16:08

really that interested in I just

16:10

ignored his. His advances

16:12

so to speak, His interest, but

16:14

he was persistent, you know? And

16:17

so finally I saw it. All

16:19

right well I'm just gonna go out

16:21

with him for coffee. was his have

16:24

coffee and then you know. I'll

16:26

tell him I don't wanna start a relationship.

16:29

So we went out for coffee. And

16:31

we went to see how this place very

16:34

close by the university that all the graduate

16:36

students used to go to and were sitting

16:38

there having coffee. In this

16:40

wonderful little Mediterranean restaurant and I

16:43

started noticing that. Like my

16:45

cheeks or flies like I'm flushing. And.

16:47

I'm I'm a little warm. And

16:49

my heart was pounding a little harder

16:51

than usual and I was having a

16:54

little trouble concentrating. And that

16:56

thought. Like attracted

16:58

to this that have I done wrong

17:00

all this time? Maybe is more interesting

17:02

than I thought? Maybe I'm. Not

17:05

that. Bad. Looking and

17:07

I guess is sort of more interesting than

17:09

slut I thought before and gee maybe I

17:11

was wrong and he of by the end

17:13

of the conversation were there for like two

17:15

hours and I'm you know he owsley can

17:17

we? Can we see each other again? Can

17:19

we? Can we have dinner and I'm like

17:21

yes sir let's have dinner You know for

17:23

sure let's. Let's then. I will

17:25

start to walk back to the place

17:27

where I'm living and I'm thinking to

17:30

myself okay so maybe there's something here

17:32

and you know he said and done

17:34

believe your first impressions. A

17:38

fumble for my keys as I always

17:40

do, I unlock the door. And

17:43

then a wave. Of

17:46

intense. Nausea just flares

17:48

up. I dropped my stuff on

17:50

the floor, slammed the door, run

17:53

to the bathroom. And

17:55

less to say. Spend some

17:57

time. You know whether they say praying

17:59

to the. Or when god and then was in

18:01

bad for a. Week with the flu. Lisa

18:06

had thought was romantic attraction

18:08

was not in fact romantic

18:10

attraction. She had just

18:13

been feeling sick. How

18:15

could anyone mix up these two very different

18:17

things? If anything, this was

18:19

an extreme form of what Lisa

18:22

volunteers had been doing in her

18:24

eight failed experiments. They had been

18:26

mixing up anxiety and depression or

18:28

calmness and happiness. She was mixing

18:30

of the signals of a viral

18:32

infection and romantic attraction. But

18:35

Lisa fail to draw the right

18:38

implications from what had happened. She

18:40

was still certain that people salad

18:42

happy or sad or anxious or

18:44

angry. They just had a hard

18:46

time articulating what they felt. Her

18:48

job as a scientist was to

18:51

find an objective way to identify

18:53

emotions. It took

18:55

Lisa a long time and well after

18:57

she had graduated to realize her data

18:59

had been telling her a different story.

19:02

For. A very long time. It

19:04

took me probably. Systematically.

19:07

About. Ten. Years maybe

19:09

more to come to the conclusion

19:12

that there are no indicators. Objective

19:14

indicators for. An emotion or

19:16

motion. Words like anger isn't a saying.

19:18

It's anger isn't a thing. It's actually

19:21

a. A category

19:23

of highly variable instances,

19:25

sometimes. When. You're angry.

19:27

You shout. Sometimes when you're angry,

19:30

you last. Sometimes when you're angry,

19:32

you cry. Sometimes. When

19:34

you're angry, you sit silently and

19:36

plot the demise of your enemy

19:39

in a sometimes see scowl. About

19:41

half the time when people scowl,

19:44

they're not angry. Dot. Significant

19:46

compared to chance. But it also means

19:48

that fifty percent of the time if

19:50

you assume that when someone is scowling,

19:52

they're angry, you're going be wrong. anger

19:55

or sadness or fear whatever will leave

19:58

or for to an emotion for really

20:00

referring to an instance of a category.

20:03

That's the first thing to understand. So

20:06

when you look at someone and they're scowling,

20:08

it could be that the person is angry.

20:10

It could be that somebody just told them

20:12

a really bad joke. It could

20:14

be that they're concentrating really hard. It

20:17

could be that they're experiencing a bad bout

20:19

of gas. Any

20:22

of those things and also other states could

20:25

have produced that

20:27

scowl on their face. And you

20:29

have to make a guess based

20:31

on your past experiences about

20:35

what that scowl means in this context.

20:38

Lisa ran an experiment that showed

20:41

our ability to read emotions is

20:43

heavily dependent on the context. Just

20:46

like that garbled sentence at the top of the

20:48

episode that made sense once you knew what I

20:50

was saying, it is the

20:52

context that helps us predict the emotions

20:54

of others. In

20:56

the study, Lisa and her colleagues

20:59

had an actor portray the emotion

21:01

in various scenarios. One

21:03

scenario asked people to imagine that a

21:05

coworker had caught them stealing and

21:07

was going to tell the boss. The

21:09

actor tried to depict the facial expression

21:11

of someone trapped in that difficult situation.

21:15

Lisa then brought in volunteers and showed them

21:17

the photos of the actor. She

21:20

asked the volunteers to guess what emotion

21:22

the actor was portraying. For

21:24

another group of volunteers, she provided the

21:26

scenario the actor was trying to portray

21:28

and asked the volunteers to guess the

21:30

emotions of people caught up in that

21:32

scenario. A third group

21:34

of volunteers got both the scenario and the

21:37

photos of the actor. We

21:39

compare people who rate the

21:41

face alone to the

21:43

people who rated the face in the context,

21:46

right? To people who

21:48

rate the context alone. So we can

21:51

ask the question, what is driving people's

21:53

perceptions? Is it the expression

21:55

on the face? Is it

21:57

the context? Or is it some combination? And

22:00

the answer is it's mostly the context.

22:03

The context always trumps the

22:05

actual facial movements. When

22:08

you're asking the question about how is

22:10

a perceiver experiencing a person's face, there's

22:13

no inherent meaning in the face. The

22:15

signals in the face are

22:18

not inherently meaningful as emotion.

22:20

The context is

22:23

creating the meaning, basically,

22:25

for what those facial

22:27

movements mean. Lisa

22:35

had an epiphany. If the

22:37

context is what helps us read the emotions of

22:39

others, is it possible that

22:41

it is the context that also shapes

22:43

how we read our own emotions? She

22:47

thought back to the story of her bad date. When

22:49

she thought about it again, she realized

22:51

that the interpretation of her emotion completely

22:54

depended on the context. The

22:57

more accurate way that I would

23:00

describe what happened is that my

23:03

brain made sense of those

23:06

physical signals coming from the

23:08

body as attraction. You

23:11

know, your brain is trapped

23:14

inside a dark silent box called

23:17

your skull, and it's

23:19

receiving signals from your body. You

23:22

have sensory surfaces all over your

23:24

body in your retina, in each

23:26

eye, the cochlea in each ear,

23:28

your skin. You have

23:30

sensory surfaces inside your body

23:32

for glucose, for temperature.

23:36

Your brain is constantly receiving sensory

23:38

signals from the sensory surfaces of

23:40

your body that inform the brain

23:43

of the changes in the body and in the

23:45

world. But the brain doesn't know the causes. It

23:47

only knows the signals themselves, which

23:50

are the outcomes. And this is

23:52

what philosophers call an inverse problem.

23:54

So your brain is constantly having to

23:56

solve an inverse problem. It

23:58

has to guess. At

24:00

the meaning. Of

24:02

that the causes of those signals.

24:05

And. Even when your brain guess

24:07

is wrong. Those guess has

24:10

become your experience basically. So.

24:12

What my brain did in that. During.

24:14

That coffee was. Take these

24:17

sensory signals. Which. Were.

24:19

Being caused by. You. Know

24:21

a pathogen assistant biologically speaking in

24:24

in my body but my brain

24:26

didn't know about that pathogen, it

24:28

just knew the outcomes of the

24:31

pathogen. Which is this which were

24:33

the signals. Pro. I

24:35

think authentically I sounds romantic attraction

24:38

in that moment. It's

24:40

just that the biological cause. With.

24:42

Different than what it made might have

24:45

been at other times. That's how I

24:47

would understand what happened now. When

24:51

you went on that date with your

24:53

fellow graduate student, you had previously experiences

24:55

of what it's like to do, you

24:57

know, have coffee with someone you're attracted

25:00

to. You sort of knew what your.

25:03

Face being flushed could mean and you

25:05

knew what a flutter in your heart

25:07

could mean. And in some ways as

25:09

you're sitting there having coffee with this

25:11

other person, your brain in some ways

25:13

is saying where have these things happen

25:15

to me before? What is the context

25:17

that makes sense here and your brain

25:19

is trying to make sense of what

25:21

these signals coming in Or Utrecht Having.

25:24

Yes, Exactly. That's exactly right, and

25:26

you know each guess each predicts

25:28

in. Isn't. Wade Equally right,

25:31

there is a prior probability there's

25:33

some. Increased chance

25:35

that one. Predictive.

25:39

Contacts. To one one.

25:41

Story. Is is gonna be more

25:43

likely than. Than

25:45

another right answer your brain is is

25:48

weighing those. Yeah and if you hadn't gone

25:50

on the date that day, if your face had

25:52

just felt slashed and your heart felt like it

25:54

was fluttering and your and you felt a little

25:57

uncomfortable and you were just happened to be in

25:59

your lab. You wouldn't have draw the

26:01

conclusion. Oh, I'm attracted to someone you might well

26:03

have. Draw the conclusion. Something's wrong. I think I

26:05

might be feeling. yeah. Exactly exactly. That's

26:08

exactly right. If I had been out

26:10

for a run and I was feeling

26:12

slashed, I would experience it. As you

26:14

know, fatigue and that I need to

26:16

you know, have a have a glass

26:18

of water or a chocolate muffin. I

26:20

don't know, but you know that such.

26:22

There are other stories that my a

26:24

brain sort of told and I'll just

26:26

say that to me. What the evidence

26:28

suggests is the following: that the motions

26:30

aren't built into your brain. They're

26:33

built by your brain in the

26:35

moment. As needed.

26:38

And. That. Specificity

26:40

or granularity. With.

26:43

Which an emotion is filled

26:45

depends on. What

26:47

past experiences your brain is

26:50

bringing to bear. To.

26:52

Project and make sense of

26:54

the incoming sensory signals. From.

26:57

The body. And from

26:59

the world. Your

27:05

feelings turn out to be predictions about

27:07

the world, not reactions to it, The

27:10

always our brains prepare us for

27:12

action. When you hear footsteps

27:14

coming up quickly behind you in a dark

27:16

alley, your brain is making a prediction that

27:19

you might need to run away. And

27:21

you feel fear. When your

27:23

child cuddles up next to you on

27:25

the couch, your brain predicts he will

27:27

experience warmth and love. And you

27:29

reach out to give your try to have. Most

27:32

of the time, of course our emotions

27:35

don't see like predictions, but there are

27:37

times when we can actually see the

27:39

predictive machinery and action. Whoa.

27:43

Let's say you hear a loud bang. What

27:45

could that loud bang be? You.

27:47

Could be a firecracker? It could be a car

27:49

backfiring. It could be a gunshot. This to be

27:51

any number of different things. exactly and

27:54

what do you do when

27:56

it's a firecracker versus a

27:59

car backfiring versus a

28:01

gunshot, you do very different things. So

28:04

when a brain asks a question, it's a

28:06

question of what do I need to

28:08

do next to keep

28:10

myself alive? That's the question

28:12

that the brain is always asking, what do I have

28:14

to do? But the

28:17

interesting thing here is that for

28:19

the most part, brains are not reacting

28:22

to the world. The brain

28:24

doesn't hear a sound and then say, what is that?

28:27

What the brain is doing is

28:29

predicting. It's predicting all

28:32

the time what actions

28:34

will be required in the next instance and

28:37

what sensory signals will be

28:39

arriving in the next

28:42

instant. And then it compares

28:44

those predictions to

28:46

the incoming sense data. So

28:49

the sensory signals from the body, from

28:52

your eyes and your ears and your

28:54

skin and your nose and all the

28:57

surfaces inside your body are not

28:59

stimuli. They're signals

29:01

that either confirm predictions or

29:04

they change them. You

29:07

know, I was in Orlando,

29:10

Florida some time ago and I

29:12

was sitting in my hotel room

29:14

in the evening and

29:16

I heard some booms, loud noises.

29:20

And for a minute I was like, what

29:22

are those noises? What could they be? And

29:25

then I remembered of course that I was

29:27

at a resort in Disney World, I was

29:29

there giving a talk and I said, it's

29:31

Disney World, so it's evening and every day

29:33

at the end of the day, Disney

29:35

World celebrates the end of the day with the fireworks

29:38

display. So I rushed to the window, threw open the

29:40

blinds and got to watch the fireworks for

29:42

a little while. But this process where you

29:44

hear the booms, you're trying to figure out

29:46

what it is, you're trying to make sense,

29:49

I'm taking into account the fact that I'm

29:51

in Orlando, I'm at Disney World, that changes

29:53

the meaning of the booms that I'm hearing

29:55

and my brain essentially has made a prediction

29:57

of saying These booms are

29:59

probably. The Not someone opening fire on new.

30:01

These booms are fireworks and you should rush

30:04

to the window to get a glimpse of

30:06

the. Exactly an insect

30:08

if you read reports of

30:10

people who have actually been

30:13

in situations where there is

30:15

gunfire. At. First.

30:17

They. Don't know what's happening. They

30:19

can't tell. Necessarily.

30:22

That they're that what they're hearing is a

30:24

gun. And then when they realize it's a

30:26

gun, They. Can't tell necessarily if

30:28

it's friend or foe. It's.

30:31

Not a situation that we're infrequently because

30:33

most of the time were not sitting

30:35

around wondering. what is that flash of

30:37

light? What is that chemical change? What

30:40

is that? You know? most of the

30:42

time our brains are predicting pretty well.

30:44

I'm but there are these moments where

30:46

you know a brain makes itself aware

30:48

of having to guess. Most

30:53

of the time or predictions don't

30:56

see like predictions. When

30:58

I reach for the mug on the desk in

31:00

front of me, it feels as though I'm looking

31:02

at a mug and directing my fingers to grasp

31:04

the handle. But what is

31:07

really happening is that I have reached

31:09

for my month so many hundreds of

31:11

times that my brain can precisely predict

31:13

the size, date, and location of the

31:15

month. It can predict how I

31:17

was raised them up to my lips and what my

31:20

t is going to Tesla? It's

31:22

still uses visual signals from my eyes,

31:24

tactile signals from my fingers, and a

31:26

taste signals from my tongue. But.

31:29

Only to fine tune it's predictions. If

31:32

I have forgotten to add sugar to my

31:34

T, my taste buds will inform my brain

31:36

that it's prediction of the taste to the

31:38

T was off. It would

31:40

make me at some sugar. Why

31:42

the World. Is predicting instead of simply painting

31:45

a picture of the world from the signals

31:47

coming into the brain. since

31:50

most of what most of us

31:52

to most to the time in

31:54

was things we have done before

31:56

lisa says he would be metabolic

31:58

li inefficient due process everything as

32:00

if it were happening for the first time. The

32:02

most effective way to run a system is

32:05

to predict the state of the system

32:07

and correct when necessary. It's not to

32:11

wait and react to

32:13

things. Reaction is more

32:15

expensive metabolically than prediction.

32:18

And a major selection pressure on

32:21

a species, but also on an

32:24

individual, like an individual's ability, for

32:26

example, to remain healthy and to

32:28

be able to reproduce, pass

32:31

its genes on to the next

32:33

generation is metabolic fitness, metabolic efficiency.

32:35

This is a, you know,

32:37

in psychology, we don't experience every hug

32:40

we give, every emotion we

32:42

experience, every thought we have, you know,

32:44

every insult we bear. We don't

32:46

experience these things in metabolic terms.

32:49

We experience them in psychological terms, but

32:51

there's always a metabolic cost because

32:54

there's always electrical and chemical

32:56

signaling going on underneath the

32:58

hood. And it turns

33:01

out that the metabolic cost of

33:03

signaling is a

33:05

major, major concern that

33:08

any organism system has to

33:10

deal with. When

33:12

I'm in the hotel room and I hear a loud

33:14

bang, my brain quickly asks itself

33:17

a few questions. Are these

33:19

booms taking place in a war zone or

33:21

a holiday resort? Second, it

33:24

asks, where have I seen or

33:26

heard this before? Third,

33:28

and perhaps most important, it asks,

33:31

what do I need to do? So

33:33

the brain is basically creating

33:35

prediction signals that are

33:38

fundamentally, fundamentally they start

33:41

not as your experience of the

33:44

world, but as your actions in

33:46

the world. So your every prediction

33:48

signal starts as a plan

33:51

for regulating the body. And

33:54

then the signals, you know, come in

33:56

that either confirm those predictions

33:59

or... wreaking

36:00

in pleasure. It was a

36:02

joy to behold. First

36:05

of all, it demonstrates that prediction

36:07

signals are not these abstract things.

36:11

They are the brain changing the

36:13

firing of its own neurons to

36:15

begin to construct an

36:17

experience before all

36:20

the information is in from the environment.

36:23

I mean, I have to tell you that

36:25

I knew what I was doing, and as

36:27

I was pouring the grape juice into the

36:29

urine cups, I'm telling you it smelled like

36:31

pee. Like, it just, there was a whiff

36:33

of pee that I could

36:35

smell. And basically, your brain

36:37

is predicting, and

36:40

those predictions are real

36:42

signals that construct

36:45

sensory experience. When

36:54

we come back, how Lisa's research can help all

36:56

of us take back control of

36:59

our emotions. You're listening to

37:01

Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantan. This

37:18

is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantan. Psychologist

37:21

and neuroscientist, Lisa Feldman Barrett is

37:23

the author of the book, How

37:26

Emotions Are Made, The

37:28

Secret Life of the Brain. In

37:30

it, she makes the case that emotions

37:32

are constructed by the brain as it

37:35

sits inside the cage of the skull

37:37

and tries to guess the meaning of

37:39

the electrical signals flooding in from the

37:41

eyes and ears and skin. But

37:44

if emotions are predictions that are designed

37:46

to guide our behavior, Lisa

37:48

says we can exercise more control

37:50

over those predictions than we realize.

38:00

bad moods were really predictions and

38:02

that she could choose to make different

38:04

predictions. When it came to

38:06

fighting a tantrum, that involved making a

38:08

new prediction about a cranky

38:11

fairy. I wanted to give

38:13

her other options for how to experience this

38:15

mood. So I

38:17

invented the cranky fairy. When

38:22

she would start to feel cranky or crabby, it

38:25

meant that the cranky fairy was coming to

38:27

visit. And then whenever she would

38:30

feel the cranky

38:32

fairy preparing to make a visit,

38:34

we gave her a special

38:36

chair, an Elmo chair, a chair

38:38

that was like a little Elmo

38:40

character from Sesame Street, and

38:43

that she could go to and sit in the

38:45

chair when the cranky fairy

38:47

was visiting and sort of

38:49

vanquished the cranky fairy from her life

38:52

for that day. And

38:56

over time, what happened was she

38:58

would actually march herself to

39:01

the chair when she felt like she was

39:03

going to lose control and have a tantrum.

39:05

And she would sit in that chair. And

39:09

she might look at a book or she

39:11

might play with some plastic animals. But

39:14

basically, she was teaching herself

39:16

how to regulate her

39:19

own behavior. And

39:21

so these stories wove together to give

39:25

her options, other options than

39:27

just making emotions, negative

39:29

emotions out of the sensations that she

39:32

was experiencing. But

39:34

there's a larger lesson in this for all

39:36

of us, not just for two-year-olds, which is

39:38

that when we experience emotions, those

39:40

emotions in some ways, as you're pointing out,

39:43

are predictions about what the world

39:45

is like, what we should be feeling, how we

39:47

should be responding. And

39:49

in exactly the same way that you taught your

39:52

daughter to, in some ways, create a different social

39:54

reality, we have some control over

39:56

our emotions in ways that many of us might

39:58

not realize. And

40:00

you know, none of us have as

40:02

much control as we would like and

40:05

gaining control is a little harder than anyone might

40:07

want But if a three-year-old can do

40:09

it then anybody

40:11

can do it and the point

40:14

here is that we created a context

40:16

for her to Rewire

40:18

her brain in a sense, right? She

40:20

she could take advantage

40:23

of the situation to give herself

40:25

new Experiences that

40:27

the brain would learn so that her

40:29

brain would predict later in a different

40:32

way. So at first She

40:35

would sit in the chair after she had a

40:37

tantrum and then she would sit in the chair,

40:39

you know Right before she was having a tantrum

40:41

and then she might go sit in the chair

40:44

You know even when a you know,

40:46

the situation might in 10 or 15

40:48

minutes prompt a tantrum. She was

40:50

able to Practice this

40:53

skill and it was hard

40:55

at first, but it's just like driving basically.

40:57

It's hard at first It takes a lot

40:59

of energy. It's an investment in being a

41:01

better you in the future There's

41:04

another important insight that you are raising

41:07

here Lisa Which is that when we

41:09

experience emotions if we start to

41:11

see them as predictions about the world One

41:14

thing we might do is instead of simply following

41:16

our emotions. We might actually be curious about

41:18

them We say what is causing me to

41:20

feel this way? What are the factors that

41:22

might be driving this? So in other words,

41:24

you're sitting across someone at a coffee shop

41:26

and your face feels flushed Instead

41:28

of automatically saying I'm attracted to this person. You

41:30

could say what might be the different reasons my

41:33

face is flushed exactly

41:35

and curiosity

41:38

is really I think Underrated

41:41

and undervalued in our culture

41:43

particularly at this particular moment

41:45

in time So this

41:48

is something that works when you

41:50

are Really even

41:52

in the throes of great

41:54

difficulty. You can become curious

41:57

instead of becoming

42:00

confident that you know

42:02

what the cause is of

42:04

a set of sensations. So

42:06

a perfect example of this was when my

42:09

daughter was 12, she was

42:11

testing for her black belt in karate. She

42:14

was barely five feet tall and she was

42:16

going to have to spar, that is fight,

42:19

with these like six foot hulking

42:22

adolescent boys in

42:24

order to get her black belt. And

42:27

her sensei, who was this

42:29

tense degree black belt, this guy could break

42:31

a board by looking at it, you know,

42:33

sort of saunters up to her and he

42:36

crosses his arms and he says, get

42:39

your butterflies flying in formation. And

42:43

I thought that is brilliant

42:45

because he's not saying be

42:48

calm because she needs that arousal in

42:50

order to perform, in order to do

42:52

the test. But he gave her a

42:54

different meaning for it and therefore she

42:57

had a different set of actions that were available to

42:59

her. You

43:02

make the point in the book that

43:04

one of the things the brain does

43:06

is that it is very carefully calibrating

43:08

how much energy it's using and making

43:11

predictions about how much energy it needs.

43:13

And many of our mood states, in

43:15

fact, are the brain's predictions about what

43:17

is coming and how much it has

43:19

by way of stored energy. What

43:22

are the implications here for ordinary people,

43:24

Lisa, with this idea, especially when it

43:26

comes to things like food and exercise

43:28

and sleep? You know, when we think

43:30

about our well-being, what does your

43:32

science say about those things that everyone

43:34

struggles with? Evolution

43:37

tells us very clearly that the brain's

43:39

most important job is coordinating

43:42

and regulating the body in

43:45

the most metabolically efficient way. And

43:47

you put it beautifully, that the brain

43:49

is always attempting to predict

43:53

what its energy needs will be in

43:56

the next moment because

43:58

that's the most... efficient way to run

44:01

a body. Brains regulate

44:03

the body, the energetic

44:05

needs of the body, by anticipating those needs

44:07

and attempting to meet those needs before they

44:09

arise. The metaphor that

44:12

I often use for the brain's regulation of

44:14

the body is that the brain is running

44:16

a budget for the body. So the technical

44:19

term for the predictive regulation

44:21

of the body is allostasis. But the

44:23

metaphor is body budgeting. Your brain is

44:25

running a budget for your body. It's

44:28

not budgeting money, it's budgeting glucose and

44:30

salt and oxygen and so on. Depression

44:32

is like a

44:34

bankrupt body budget. It's

44:37

basically your brain is

44:39

attempting to reduce its

44:42

costs. And in doing

44:44

this, it will

44:46

create fatigue, which will lead

44:48

you to move less. So that's a reduction

44:51

in cost. The brain is like

44:53

trapped in its predictions.

44:55

It's not going to update. It's

44:58

not going to learn from prediction error

45:00

because learning is metabolically expensive.

45:03

So even if there are pleasant

45:06

things, like things in the world that

45:09

could lead you to experience pleasure, you

45:12

won't pay attention to them and you

45:14

won't learn about them. You won't take

45:16

advantage of them because it's just too

45:18

expensive. So

45:21

basically the brain is trapped

45:23

in these predictions that

45:26

will lead to more

45:28

unpleasant or continuing unpleasant mood.

45:31

So when you feel stressed, it's

45:33

because your brain has predicted that a

45:37

big metabolic outlay is

45:39

going to be necessary in the next

45:41

moment. So

45:44

like when you're being criticized by

45:47

someone or when someone is bullying

45:50

you or when you're having conflicts with

45:52

people and so on. And then

45:54

if you add to that, maybe not

45:56

getting enough sleep or

45:58

maybe not eating healthily. It

48:00

just turns down the dial on

48:04

the intensity of

48:06

your discomfort in a way

48:08

that I find to be really productive. Lisa

48:21

Feldman Barrett is a psychologist at

48:23

Northeastern University. She's the author

48:25

of How Emotions Are Made, The Secret

48:27

Life of the Brain, and

48:29

Seven and a Half Lessons About the Brain. Lisa,

48:32

thank you so much for joining me today on Hidden Brain.

48:35

It's been my pleasure. Thank you so much. Hidden

48:56

Brain is produced by Hidden Brain Media. Our

48:59

audio production team includes Bridget

49:01

McCarthy, Annie Murphy-Paul, Kristen Wong,

49:04

Laura Querell, Ryan Katz,

49:06

Autumn Barnes, Andrew Chadwick, and

49:08

Nick Woodbole. Tara

49:10

Boyle is our executive producer. I'm

49:13

Hidden Brain's executive editor. If

49:21

you enjoy the show and want more of our

49:23

work, please join Hidden Brain Plus, our

49:26

podcast subscription on Apple Podcasts.

49:29

Hidden Brain Plus is a great way

49:31

to support the show and get access

49:33

to exclusive conversations you won't hear anywhere

49:35

else. You can find it

49:37

by searching for Hidden Brain on the Apple Podcasts app

49:40

or by going to apple.co.uk. I'm

49:44

Shankar Vedantam. See you soon. Hidden

49:59

Brain Thank you.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features